A Scientific Basis for Talent Identification and Elite Player Development October 2006 - PowerPoint PPT Presentation

1 / 58
About This Presentation
Title:

A Scientific Basis for Talent Identification and Elite Player Development October 2006

Description:

Each component is expressed numerically from 1 (low) to 7 (high) NB. ... Amongst the girls the RAE is not significant, although the fourth quarter is ... – PowerPoint PPT presentation

Number of Views:283
Avg rating:3.0/5.0
Slides: 59
Provided by: lks4
Category:

less

Transcript and Presenter's Notes

Title: A Scientific Basis for Talent Identification and Elite Player Development October 2006


1
A Scientific Basis for Talent Identification and
Elite Player DevelopmentOctober 2006
2
Questions we asked..
  • Is there any evidence in the scientific
    literature on the ability (or inability) to
    identify sporting giftedness/talent?
  • Are there any known indicators of potential (but
    which are invariant to age and training)?
  • Are there specific age-related developmental
    stages in developing players?
  • What time and/or effort is required to master a
    task (in this instance, football)?
  • Are we making appropriate use of the entire
    talent pool available to us?

3
Gagnés Differentiated Model of Giftedness and
Talent (DMGT)
Under the partial influence of genetic endowment
4
Gagnés Differentiated Model of Giftedness and
Talent (DMGT)
  • Giftedness is defined as the possession and use
    of untrained and spontaneously expressed natural
    abilities (called aptitudes or gifts), in at
    least one ability domain, to a degree that places
    an individual at least amongst the top 10 of age
    peers
  • Talent is defined as the superior mastery of
    systematically developed abilities (or skills)
    and knowledge in at least one field of human
    activity to a degree that places an individual at
    least amongst the top 10 of age peers who are or
    have been active in that field or fields
  • Where giftedness is the beginning of the learning
    process, talent is the result of it.

5
Gagnés Differentiated Model of Giftedness and
Talent (DMGT)
  • Three components influence the learning/practice
    process that changes natural athletic ability
    into systematically developed sporting skills
    (talent)
  • Intrapersonal catalysts (physical and
    psychological factors)
  • Environmental catalysts
  • Chance factors
  • There is an element of both nature and nurture in
    this model
  • Without an innate ability no amount of training
    will create a top elite player, and
  • Without the appropriate quantity and quality of
    training a player will not develop into a top
    elite player

6
Questions we asked..
  • Is there any evidence in the scientific
    literature on the ability (or inability) to
    identify sporting giftedness/talent?
  • Are there any known indicators of potential (but
    which are invariant to age and training)?
  • Are there specific age-related developmental
    stages in developing players?
  • What time and/or effort is required to master a
    task (in this instance, football)?
  • Are we making appropriate use of the entire
    talent pool available to us?

7
Ability or inability to identify or predict
giftedness/talent
  • Is it possible to identify a priori which persons
    might become talented footballers, and which not?

8
Talent Prediction
  • A number of studies have been undertaken to
    discriminate between elite and non-elite soccer
    players on the basis of anthropomorphic,
    physiological, psychological and soccer-specific
    skills tests
  • Univariate differences
  • Endomorphy (fatness), skinfolds (7 areas) and
    body fat
  • Sprint speed, speed endurance, aerobic power,
    agility, standing vertical jump
  • Task-orientation, anticipation, somatic anxiety
  • Dribbling skill
  • Multivariate (discriminant) analysis
  • Agility, 30m sprint time, ego orientation and 1
    vs 1 anticipation were the set of variables that
    provided the best predictors of talent.
  • Results indicate that
  • Advanced biological maturity status is associated
    with slightly better technical performance on the
    tests specifically, older, more mature players
    perform better in dribbling and in ball control

9
Talent Prediction
  • Leaves us wondering if
  • these variables/model are only useful in talent
    discrimination rather than talent
    identification/prediction
  • these measures are able to discriminate among
    players already selected and exposed to
    systemised training, because the sensitivity of
    the tests tends to decrease once players reach
    the elite level

10
Talent Prediction
  • No consensus about the relative importance of
    physical, psychological and physiological
    qualities in predicting football talent (Williams
    and Reilly, 2000)
  • Talent ID programs around the globe are not
    firmly based on scientific rationale (Williams
    and Franks, 1998) and rely heavily on intuition
    or eye of expert coaches and talent scouts to
    identify talented performers

11
Talent Prediction
  • It is probable that talent detection,
    identification and development are not amenable
    to a reductionist process that can permit
    ultimate potential to be defined with much degree
    of certainty. The problem is especially complex
    in sports such as soccer where performance itself
    is multifactorial. At present anthropomorphic and
    physiological profiling is best viewed as an
    objective means of monitoring young players,
    while emphasis should be placed on technical
    skills and engagement in teamwork.
  • (Reilly, T., Bangsbo, J. and Franks, A. (2000).
    Anthropometric and physiological predispositions
    for elite soccer. Journal of Sports Sciences, 18,
    669-683.)
  • A coaches judgement is the best solution for
    identifying talent, not some scientific detection
    model.
  • (Tranckle, P. (2004). Understanding giftedness
    and talent in sport. The Coach, 21, March/April
    2004, 61-73.)

12
Talent Prediction
  • Psychological factors are what distinguishes
    successful elite players from their non-elite
    counterparts (Morgan 1979). Little work done on
    psychological factors in talent ID, or even which
    psychological factors, or how to measure them
  • Cognitive factors and game intelligence
    perceptual skill in soccer is a promising area
    for talent ID
  • Skilled players are better than lesser skilled
    players at recognising structured patterns of
    play because their knowledge allows them to
    chunk perceptual information into larger and
    more meaningful units (cf. Ross (2006) regarding
    expert chess players). This enables them to
    recognise emergent features of a pattern of play
    early in its initiation, thereby facilitating
    anticipation
  • Skilled players also use advance cue
    utilization (i.e. ability to make accurate
    predictions based on information arising from an
    opponents posture and bodily orientation). This
    has been tested extensively in the penalty
    situation.

13
Talent Prediction
  • It is conjectured that skilled players use their
    expert knowledge to dismiss many potential
    situational events as being highly improbable
    and attach a hierarchy of probabilities to
    remaining events, i.e. likelihood of occurrence.
  • Can perceptual skill be developed through
    training?
  • Teach young players about important
    features/structures of play. Set pieces are
    obvious starting points.
  • Focus on postural cues, and the relationship
    between these and subsequent performance (via
    video simulation).
  • Decision making skills can be taught from as
    young as 7 years, i.e. earlier than expected.

14
Talent identification as it currently happens
  • Vrljic and Mallet interviewed five youth coaches
    responsible for selecting and coaching Under
    15/16 Queensland boys teams at the national
    championships.
  • Results show that four categories are considered
  • Physical skills
  • Running speed (over short distances, say 30-40
    meters) most important
  • Physical strength
  • Skill-based (i.e. being able to win the ball)
  • Physical (i.e. physical appearance of
    athleticism)
  • Aerobic fitness
  • Technical skills
  • Ball control at speed
  • Ball control under unpredictable circumstances
  • Ball control with various body parts

15
Talent identification as it currently happens
  • Cognitive-perceptual skills
  • Reading the game / anticipating play
  • Decision making / choosing the best option /
    vision (usually have to be taught this in the
    development process, so should not be part of the
    identification process)
  • Personal qualities
  • Hunger to succeed
  • Mental toughness
  • Unselfish, i.e. team player rather than
    individual
  • Coaches preferred to identify talent in authentic
    playing situations (i.e. a match) than in
    skill/drill tests.

16
Body shape
  • Is there a particular body shape that is best
    suited to elite football?

17
Body shape (somatotype)
  • 3 components
  • Endomorphy (fatness)
  • Measured by skinfolds, adjusted for height
  • Ectomorphy (linearity/tallness)
  • Measured by height/weight ratio
  • Mesomorphy (skeletal/muscular)
  • Measured by limb circumferences corrected for
    fatness, skeletal breadths/widths, height
  • An individuals physique includes all 3
    components e.g. A typical adult elite player
    might have Endo / Meso / Ecto 2.8 / 5.3 / 3.3
  • Each component is expressed numerically from 1
    (low) to 7 (high)
  • NB. Components change with growth and maturation
    during adolescence

18
Body shape (somatotype)
  • Youth soccer players tend to have physiques
    similar to those of elite adult players, with
    adult elite players more mesomorphic, probably
    reflecting growth of muscle and skeletal mass
  • Potential effects of training on muscularity at
    more elite levels
  • Youth soccer players tend to have more fat and be
    relatively speaking taller than elite adult
    players
  • None of this research is predictive, i.e. the
    shape of body that might indicate a potential
    elite player

19
Questions we asked..
  • Is there any evidence in the scientific
    literature on the ability (or inability) to
    identify sporting giftedness/talent?
  • Are there any known indicators of potential (but
    which are invariant to age and training)?
  • Are there specific age-related developmental
    stages in developing players?
  • What time and/or effort is required to master a
    task (in this instance, football)?
  • Are we making appropriate use of the entire
    talent pool available to us?

20
Invariant indicators of potential
Is it possible to identify the subset of the
population that are gifted in the domains that
are relevant to sporting ability? i.e. have the
right nature in the nature/nurture debate? The
measure would have to be invariant to age and to
training (If so, we might concentrate our talent
identification/development process on that
subset, assuming that very few others will ever
become talented) Proviso Any procedure we
use will have to be practical and easy enough to
implement
21
Measuring innate ability in sport
  • Ability in sports is highly related to male
    physical competitiveness
  • It is well researched that the ratio of the
    length of the 2nd to the 4th fingers (2D4D) is a
    negative correlate of prenatal and adult
    testosterone
  • Men with lower 2D4D ratios reported higher
    attainment in a range of sports and had higher
    mental rotation scores (a measure of
    visual-spatial ability) than those with high
    2D4D ratios.
  • Implication Testosterone promotes the
    development and maintenance of traits which are
    useful in sports, and the 2D4D ratio is a way to
    measure this ability, independent from previous
    exposure to or training in the sport.
  • Recent research (Spector, T. (2006). British
    Journal of Sports Medicine) shows that this holds
    in the case of females as well. It is conjectured
    that genetic factors play a major role.

22
Measuring innate ability in sport
  • This is promising research, but a little
    impractical at this stage
  • Could there be other such measures?

23
Questions we asked..
  • Is there any evidence in the scientific
    literature on the ability (or inability) to
    identify sporting giftedness/talent?
  • Are there any known indicators of potential (but
    which are invariant to age and training)?
  • Are there specific age-related developmental
    stages in developing players?
  • What time and/or effort is required to master a
    task (in this instance, football)?
  • Are we making appropriate use of the entire
    talent pool available to us?

24
Age-related development stages
  • When is the best age to concentrate training?

25
Age-related development
  • Coaches worldwide currently design long and
    short-term athlete training models as well as
    competition and recovery programs based on their
    athletes chronological age.
  • Research has shown that chronological age is not
    a good indicator on which to base athlete
    development models for athletes between the ages
    of 10 to 16. There is a wide variation in the
    physical, cognitive and emotional development of
    athletes within this age group.
  • Ideally, coaches should determine the biological
    age of their athletes and use this information as
    the foundation for athlete development models.
    Unfortunately, there is no reliable procedure to
    identify biological age non-invasively.

26
Age-related development
  • Use the onset of Peak Height Velocity (PHV) as a
    reference point for the design of optimal
    individual programs with relation to critical
    or sensitive windows of trainability during the
    maturation process.
  • Prior to the onset of PHV, boys and girls can
    train together and chronological age can be used
    to determine training, competition and recovery
    programs.
  • The average age for the onset of PHV is 12 and 14
    years for females and males respectively.
  • The onset of PHV is influenced by both genetic
    and environmental factors, including climate,
    cultural influences, and social environment.

27
Age-related development
  • Using simple measurements, PHV can be monitored
    and training can be related to and optimised to
    exploit the critical periods of trainability.
  • All energy systems are always trainable, but
    during the so-called critical periods
    accelerated adaptation will take place if the
    proper volume, intensity and frequency of
    exercise are implemented.

28
Questions we asked..
  • Is there any evidence in the scientific
    literature on the ability (or inability) to
    identify sporting giftedness/talent?
  • Are there any known indicators of potential (but
    which are invariant to age and training)?
  • Are there specific age-related developmental
    stages in developing players?
  • What time and/or effort is required to master a
    task (in this instance, football)?
  • Are we making appropriate use of the entire
    talent pool available to us?

29
Time and effort to develop mastery in football
  • How much / long does it take to master a sport
    (i.e. to attain elite level)?

30
The 10 year / 10,000 hours rule
  • The Theory of Deliberate Practice is predicated
    on the notion that it is not simply training of
    any type, but rather a minimum of 10 years
    engagement in deliberate practice that is the
    necessary condition for the attainment of
    expertise. Deliberate practice refers to practice
    activities done with the specific instrumental
    goal of improving performance, and which
  • Are performed in a daily, work-like manner
  • Require effort and attention
  • Do not lead to immediate social or financial
    rewards
  • Are frequently not enjoyable to perform
  • (i.e. the hard yards)
  • First suggested by Simon and Chase (1973) in
    relation to skill in chess
  • Subsequent work has shown this to apply to
    expertise in other domains, such as music,
    mathematics, swimming, distance running, tennis,
    soccer, hockey etc

31
The 10 year / 10,000 hours rule
  • Subsequent work tested subjects (experts and
    non-experts) in hockey, netball and basketball in
    Australia.
  • This research concludes that international
    standard players in almost all sports took
    between 10 and 13 years to develop fully to
    international level, with around 6,000 hours of
    sport-specific training. The balance of hours
    could be attributed to
  • Maintaining the level of expertise
  • Other sports training (both before attaining
    expert status in the chosen sport, and
    non-specific sports training, e.g. running to
    maintain fitness).
  • This implies a strong leaning towards the
    nurture side of the nature/nurture debate, i.e.
    its all in the training

32
The 10 year / 10,000 hours rule
  • Other studies have shown a negative correlation
    to exist between hours of sport-specific training
    required to reach international expertise and
    number of prior sporting activities experienced,
    i.e. participation in other sporting activities
    may aid development of expert decision-making
    skills, and transfer of learning may take place
    from one sport to another.
  • Significantly, the results point to an
    implication that early specialisation may not be
    a necessary requirement for expert level
    performance in decision-making sports.

33
Effortful study is a prerequisite for success
  • A recent study (in Scientific American) has
    looked at how chess grand-masters are able to
    assimilate so much information so quickly and
    accurately.
  • Concludes (based on academic research) that
    effortful study is the key to achieving success
    where effortful study. entails tackling
    challenges that lie just beyond ones
    competence, i.e. to get good you must extend
    yourself.
  • This explains
  • why you can only be the best at competitive teams
    sports if you are playing with the best and
  • why a very strong RAE among youth may be somewhat
    counterbalanced at an older age (if the youngest
    in an age group have to engage in more effortful
    study than others of their age in order to excel
    and if those differences become habits then
    persist into the professional years while the
    early innate benefits of being slightly older
    disappear)
  • the characteristics of a developmental pathway
    (i.e. it must provide substantial opportunity for
    effortful study at all ages)

34
The 10 year / 10,000 hours rule
  • Generally accepted categorisation of sports
    participation
  • Sampling years deliberate play (6 to 12 years)
  • Broad range of sporting activities
  • Emphasis on fun
  • Specializing years (13 to 15 years)
  • Decrease in other sporting activity, starting to
    specialize in one sport
  • Growing emphasis on skill development
  • Investment years (16 and over)
  • Devotion to one primary activity
  • Strategic and competitive skills training
  • Teachers become more demanding as one moves
    through the stages

35
Questions we asked..
  • Is there any evidence in the scientific
    literature on the ability (or inability) to
    identify sporting giftedness/talent?
  • Are there any known indicators of potential (but
    which are invariant to age and training)?
  • Are there specific age-related developmental
    stages in developing players?
  • What time and/or effort is required to master a
    task (in this instance, football)?
  • Are we making appropriate use of the entire
    talent pool available to us?

36
Squandering the talent pool the Relative Age
Effect
  • Systematic exclusion of groups of players, some
    of which might be highly talented

37
Relative Age Effect (RAE)
  • The relative age effect (RAE) in sport was first
    noted among elite level ice hockey players in the
    USA. These findings demonstrated that for major
    junior leagues and the NHL player birth dates
    decreased in frequency from January through
    December.
  • The RAE is strikingly evident in activities that
    are competitive and where performance is highly
    correlated with age and maturity, (e.g. football
    in Australia)
  • It was theorized that the RAE arose from the
    consequences of grouping young boys for entry
    into organized sport, thereby producing a
    one-year age range for the participants. As size,
    speed, and coordination are highly correlated
    with age, older players within the age-group
    will, on average, show superior performance.

38
Relative Age Effect (RAE)
  • Thus maturity has been mistaken for ability by
    coaches, peers and the individuals themselves.
    The age-advantaged children are imbued with
    greater self-confidence and regard by others. The
    opposite will likely hold for those younger than
    their group-mates, who are likely to suffer from
    lowered self confidence and self-esteem.
  • A consequence is an increased drop-out rate for
    those disadvantaged by age.
  • Predictably, the RAE has also been found in a
    host of other competitive sports such as
    baseball, basketball, soccer, rugby, American
    football, cricket etc etc.

39
Relative Age Effect (RAE)
  • RAE has been observed in activities other than
    sport as well the obvious one is the cut-off
    for school entering in all schools, but also
    musical ability, youth suicide etc
  • There are some sports and activities in which no
    RAE is evident (e.g. dance, table tennis,
    gymnastics to name a few). These sports depend
    heavily on the technical ability (or motor skill)
    of the participant.

40
Relative Age Effect (RAE) Australian data
  • Boys Under 14 and 15 NYC 2006 (n351)

Very highly statistically significant trend
41
Relative Age Effect (RAE) Australian data
  • Boys Under 14 and 15 NYC 2006 (n351)

Very highly statistically significant trend
42
Relative Age Effect (RAE) Australian data
  • Under 17 and 20 national teams 2006 (n47)

Statistically significant trend
43
Relative Age Effect (RAE) Australian data
  • Under 17 and 20 national teams 2006 (n47)

Statistically significant trend
44
Relative Age Effect (RAE) Australian data
  • HAL and overseas professionals (includes
    Socceroos and Futsalroos) (n278)

Statistically significant trend
45
Relative Age Effect (RAE) Australian data
  • HAL and overseas professionals (includes
    Socceroos and Futsalroos) (n278)

Nearly statistically significant trend
46
Relative Age Effect (RAE) Australian data
  • Socceroos 2006 FIFA World Cup squad (n23)

Not statistically significant (sample size)
47
Relative Age Effect (RAE) Australian data
  • Socceroos 2006 FIFA World Cup squad (n23)

Not statistically significant (sample size)
48
Relative Age Effect (RAE) Australian data
  • Girls NTC U15 and U17 2006 (n268)

Not statistically significant
49
Relative Age Effect (RAE) Australian data
  • Girls NTC U15 and U17 2006 (n268)

Not statistically significant
50
Relative Age Effect (RAE) Australian data
  • Matildas and Young Matildas 2006 (n52)

Nearly statistically significant trend
51
Relative Age Effect (RAE) Australian data
  • Matildas and Young Matildas 2006 (n52)

Statistically significant trend
52
Relative Age Effect (RAE) Australian data
  • Observations on the Australian data
  • RAE is greatest at youngest age levels for boys
    (U14 and 15), and decreases as ages (and
    abilities) increase
  • Estimated loss of the talent pool is around 42
    at the U14/15 age group!
  • The RAE effect reduces until at the professional
    level it is fairly small
  • Socceroos negative RAE! we conjecture that
    this is caused by very talented youngsters having
    to push themselves and play above themselves to
    keep up with the older kids, and this work ethic
    has remained with them throughout their career,
    leading to them staying ahead of their peers

53
Relative Age Effect (RAE) Australian data
  • Observations on the Australian data (contd)
  • Socceroos seems to indicate an August cut-off
    (43 in Aug to Oct, same proportion as the
    U14U15 (42) and U17U20 (44)) slipped into
    the European cycle?
  • Amongst the girls the RAE is not significant,
    although the fourth quarter is still
    under-represented we conjecture that this is
    because competition for places is less fierce
    than in the boys competition
  • In the elite senior women the RAE is again
    strongly evident competition is stronger at
    this level

54
Relative Age Effect (RAE) Australian data
  • We are not alone!
  • Football 1st January 31st December
  • AFL 1st January 31st December
  • Rugby Union 1st January 31st December
  • Rugby League 1st January 31st December
  • Netball 1st January 31st December
  • Cricket 1st September 31st August
  • This means that a significant proportion of kids
    born later in the year are, on average, dropping
    out of all organised winter team sports because
    of maturity issues (RAE) rather than ability.
    There is thus a huge pool of talent out there
    which could be tapped.
  • Question Does this mean that competitive and
    specialised team sports (i.e. football) should
    start later, once the maturity/ability factors
    have aligned (say only at age 13?)

55
Relative Age Effect (RAE)
  • Potential remedy offered
  • Classification system based on biological age
    (e.g. anthropomorphic or physiological
    measurements)
  • Classification systems based on chronological age
  • Variation of cut-off date within the competition
    year from year to year.
  • Rotation of RA advantage by using a period
    different from a 12-month period - this is
    probably unmanageable and impractical, and would
    reduce the number of players per age group
  • Change soccers cut-off date away from January to
    take advantage of drop-outs from other
    footballing codes.
  • Create different squads of developing players who
    are technically well-developed, but are currently
    lacking in terms of physical development.
  • Start competitive competition at a later age,
    e.g. 14 or 15, after physical development has
    equalised.

56
Relative Age Effect (RAE) Australian data
  • A variation on RAE occurs in the later years due
    to FIFAs policy of having the Under 17 and Under
    20 World Youth Cup in odd years only (e.g. 2005,
    2007 etc)
  • This means that every second year of players
    misses out on this pinnacle event, but each group
    should play in one of the two events (but
    unlikely to play in both)
  • It is conjectured that the Under 20 competition
    favours those born in odd years, and the Under 17
    competition favours those born in even years

57
Relative Age Effect (RAE) Australian data
  • A variation on RAE Under 17 and 20 national
    teams Year of birth

Disadvantaged too young for 2005, but too old
for 2007
Disadvantaged too young for 2005, but too old
for 2007
Under 17
Under 20
58
Summary Some key assertions about elite coaching
  • Sustained success comes from training and
    performing well over the long-term rather than
    winning in the short-term.
  • There is no short-cut to success in athletic
    preparation.
  • Overemphasizing competition in the early phases
    of training will always cause shortcomings in
    athletic abilities later in an athletes career.
  • (Balyi, I. and Hamilton, A. (2004). Long term
    athlete development trainability in childhood
    and adolescence. Olympic Coach, 13, No.3 (Spring
    issue). http//coaching.usolympicteam.com/coaching
    /kpub.nsf/v/2ltad04.
Write a Comment
User Comments (0)
About PowerShow.com