Does Teacher Feedback Make a Difference in Second Language Learning - PowerPoint PPT Presentation

1 / 34
About This Presentation
Title:

Does Teacher Feedback Make a Difference in Second Language Learning

Description:

Porcupines, Skunks, Hares, & Giraffes. T3: The porcupine? Sara? ... Porcupines, Skunks, Hares, & Giraffes. T3: And so the skunk, what does it do? Karen ... – PowerPoint PPT presentation

Number of Views:186
Avg rating:3.0/5.0
Slides: 35
Provided by: royly2
Category:

less

Transcript and Presenter's Notes

Title: Does Teacher Feedback Make a Difference in Second Language Learning


1
Does Teacher FeedbackMake a Differencein Second
Language Learning?
  • Dr. Roy Lyster
  • McGill University
  • Montreal, Canada

2
Teaching in the 80s
  • Krashen said that feedback was useless, and
    harmful, and would cause anxiety (more recently,
    see Krashen, 1994 Truscott, 1996).
  • (I did not want anxious students, so I provided
    very little feedback.)
  • Researchers said that errors would diminish over
    time.
  • (I wanted to be patient, so I provided very
    little feedback.)
  • My students had studied their L2 for 8 years
  • (How long would it take for errors to work
    themselves out?)

3
Serge and the Negotiation of Form
  • I observed a teacher named Serge who provided
    feedback without causing any any observable
    anxiety (Lyster, 1994).
  • There was good-natured humour and the
    communication flow did NOT stop.
  • Serge negotiated form with his students
  • Serge How is formal correspondence different
    in English and French?
  • Student 1 The thing at the bottom
  • Serge The thing at the bottom?!
  • Student 1 The final salutation

4
Feedback Terminology
  • Error correction
  • Negative feedback
  • Corrective feedback
  • Interactional feedback
  • Negotiation of meaning
  • Negotiation of form

5
Rationale for FeedbackTransfer-appropriate
Learning
  • The context in which learning occurs should
    resemble the context in which the learning will
    be put to use (Segalowitz, 2000)
  • Language features learned in isolated grammar
    lessons may be remembered in similar contexts
    (e.g., during a grammar test), but hard to
    retrieve in the context of communicative
    interaction.
  • Language features noticed during communicative
    interaction may be more easily retrieved in
    communicative contexts.

6
Teachable Moments Focus on Form
  • Providing feedback in the heat of the moment
    when a learner really has something to say,
    rather than waiting till later
  • How can teachers do this? How can they focus on
    form during meaningful interaction?
  • By providing different types of interactional
    feedback recasts or prompts.

7
Recasts
  • In a recast, the teacher implicitly reformulates
    the students utterance, minus the error.
  • Example 1
  • Student Before someone will takes it.
  • Teacher Before someone takes it.
  • Example 2
  • Student Or an boat.
  • Teacher Yes, thats true that it could be a
    boat, but there theyre giving addresses.

8
Frequency of Recasts
  • Recasts are the most frequent type of feedback in
    a wide range of classroom settings
  • elementary immersion classrooms
  • (Lyster Ranta, 1997 Mori, 2002)
  • university-level foreign language classrooms
  • (Doughty, 1994 Roberts, 1995)
  • high school EFL classrooms
  • (Tsang, 2004)
  • adult ESL classrooms
  • (Ellis, Basurkmen, Loewen, 2001 Panova
    Lyster, 2002)

9
Theoretical Value of Recasts
  • Based on claims that children frequently repeat
    their parents recasts during L1 acquisition,
    recasts have been promoted as an effective type
    of feedback
  • Some researchers hypothesize that recasts help
    learners to notice the gap between interlanguage
    forms and target forms, thus serving as negative
    evidence
  • Doughty (2001)
  • Long (1996)
  • Long Robinson (1998)

10
Practical advantages
  • Recasts provide supportive scaffolding that helps
    learners participate in lessons when the target
    forms in question are beyond their current
    abilities.
  • Recasts are ideal for facilitating the delivery
    of complex subject matter (Lyster, 2002).

11
Disadvantages of Recasts
  • Recasts do not lead to any self- or peer-repair
    when there is repair, the student can only repeat
    the teachers reformulation
  • In L2 classrooms, many recasts can be ambiguous
    and therefore do not help learners to notice
    their mistakes (Lyster, 1998).

12
Ambiguity of Recasts
  • Recasts Compete with Non-Corrective Repetition
  • Recast
  • T6 What smells so good? Allen?
  • St Sap maple.
  • T6 Maple sap. Thats good.
  • Non-corrective repetition
  • T6 What do we call the baby of a hen? Nicole?
  • St Chicks.
  • T6 Chicks. Thats good.

13
Ambiguity of Recasts
  • Recasts Compete with Signs of Approval
  • Example 1
  • T5 What are orders?..Yes?
  • St Its, just like uhh you say us, do this,
    do that
  • T5 Exactly, its when someone tells us Do that,
    go there, eat that.
  • Example 2
  • T6 A hole in which a rabbit lives, Patrick?
  • St A din.
  • T6 A den, thats good.

14
Perlette and the Water Cycle
  • T5 Whats a stream again? Yes?
  • StA Its like a small lake.
  • T5 A small lake we said?
  • StA Its an little river.
  • T5 Thats it. Its a little river, O.K.?
    Because a lake is a, a place where theres water
    but its a ...
  • Sts Like a circle.
  • T5 And so she finds herself near a forest. What
    do they do in the forest? Will?
  • StB They cut down trees.
  • T5 They cut down trees.

15
Perlette and the Water Cycle
  • T5 What do they do to transport the wood?
  • StC Um, you put the wood in the water and the
    um, how do you say emporter?
  • Sts Carries.
  • T5 Carries, good.
  • StC Carries tree to an place and another
    person who puts the wood.
  • T5 Thats it. So, they put the wood in the
    river so it gets transported from one place
    to another.

16
Perlette and the Water Cycle
  • T5 And when hes talking to Perlette, what
    happens to the fish?
  • St Hes going to drink her.
  • T5 Hes going to drink Perlette? No, hes not
    going to drink Perlette.
  • St Uhm, the fish is friend of her.
  • T5 Yes, thats it, theyre friends and they talk
    together. Then suddenly what happens? Yes?
  • StD A person fishing took.
  • T5 Exactly. Right, theres a hook with a little
    worm on it and so the fish turns around....

17
Perlette and the Water Cycle
  • T5 Why does she want to warm up do you think?
    Yes?
  • StA Because she has too cold to go into all
    the ?
  • T5 Because she is too cold, O.K. Yes?
  • StB She has too frightened.
  • T5 Because she is frightened, yes.

18
Experimental Studies of Recasts
  • Some experimental studies have shown that recasts
    are more effective than no feedback
  • in laboratory settings
  • Long et al. (1998)
  • Mackey Philp (1998)
  • in a classroom setting
  • Doughty Varela (1998) showed that corrective
    recasting was more effective than no feedback
  • St I think that the worm will go under the soil.
  • T I think that the worm will go under the soil?
    I thought that the worm would go under the
    soil.

19
Prompts Negotiation of form
  • Clarification request
  • The teacher pretends that the message has not
    been understood and that a repetition or a
    reformulation is required
  • Pardon me?
  • I dont understand
  • Repetition
  • The teacher repeats the students erroneous
    utterance, adjusting the intonation to highlight
    the error
  • He goed?

20
Prompts
  • Metalinguistic clues
  • The teacher provides comments or questions
    related to the accuracy of the students
    utterance, without explicitly providing the
    correct form
  • Do we say goed in English?
  • No, thats not it.
  • Elicitation
  • The teacher directly elicits correct forms from
    students by asking questions such as
  • How do we say that in English?
  • He what?

21
Prompts
  • Self-repair
  • Prompts lead to student-generated repair because,
    unlike recasts, they withhold correct forms and
    providing clues instead, pushing students to
    retrieve correct forms on their own (i.e., peer-
    or self-repair).
  • Frequency
  • Prompts accounted for 38 of all feedback in
    French immersion classrooms (Lyster Ranta,
    1997) and 26 in Japanese immersion classrooms
    (Mori, 2002).

22
Porcupines, Skunks, Hares, Giraffes
  • T3 The porcupine? Sara?
  • St Its the pines on its back, its ...
  • T3 The pines. Do we say pines?
  • StD The upines.
  • T3 The...?
  • StD The quills.
  • T3 The quills. Very good. The quills.

23
Porcupines, Skunks, Hares, Giraffes
  • T3 And so the skunk, what does it do? Karen
  • St Uhm...it does...Well theres a stream of
    perfume that doesnt smell very good...
  • T3 A stream of perfume, well call that a ...?
  • Sts Liquid.
  • T3 Liquid. A liquid . . .?
  • StD Smelly.
  • T3 A smelly liquid. We also call that ..

24
Porcupines, Skunks, Hares, Giraffes
  • T3 The hare. Joseph could you tell us what its
    means of defense are?
  • St It runs fast and it hops.
  • T3 It runs fast.
  • StD It jump.
  • T3 It jump?
  • Sts It jumps.
  • T3 It jumps, from the verb. . . ?
  • Sts To jump.
  • T3 To jump. It jumps about. Right, it jumps.
    Next, Joseph?

25
Porcupines, Skunks, Hares, Giraffes
  • T3 Bigger than you would be what?
  • St The giraffe? masc.
  • T3 The giraffe? masc.
  • St The giraffe. fem.
  • T3 The giraffe.fem. But is the giraffe an
    animal from Canada?

26
Effectiveness of Prompts
  • Prompts can improve control over
    already-internalized forms by providing
    opportunities for
  • pushed output, hypothesized by Swain (1985,
    1988) to move interlanguage development forward,
  • practice that helps learners in the transition of
    declarative to procedural knowledge (de Bot,
    1996 Lyster Ranta, 1997).

27
Effectiveness of Prompts
  • L2 learners benefit more from being pushed to
    retrieve target language forms than from merely
    hearing the forms in the input
  • because the retrieval and subsequent production
    require a deep level of processing that
    stimulates connections in memory (de Bot, 1996).
  • Studies comparing recasts with prompts in
    classroom settings have shown that prompts are
    more effective than recasts
  • Havranek Cesnik (2001)
  • Ammar (2003)
  • Lyster (2004)

28
Ammar (2003)
  • Third-person possessive determiners in English
    (his and her) were targeted in three 6th-grade
    intensive ESL classrooms over a four-week period.
  • One class received recasts, another received
    prompts, and the third received no feedback.
  • The group receiving prompts significantly
    outperformed the recast group on written and oral
    post-tests
  • Prompts were particularly effective for
    lower-proficiency learners, whereas
    higher-proficiency learners benefited similarly
    from both recasts and prompts.

29
Lyster (2004)
  • Grammatical gender in French was targeted by
    three 5th-grade immersion teachers in different
    ways that permitted comparisons of three oral
    feedback options prompts, recasts, and no
    feedback.
  • A comparison group received no form-focused
    instruction nor any pre-planned feedback on
    grammatical gender.
  • The analysis of eight proficiency measures
    administered over time showed that the group
    receiving prompts distinguished itself by being
    the only group to significantly outperform the
    comparison group on all eight measures.

30
When to use recasts
  • Depending on the interactional context, learners
    are likely to notice the corrective quality of
    many recasts, especially when
  • the recasts have been shortened and/or provided
    with added stress to highlight the error
  • the target forms are beyond the students current
    abilities.

31
When to use prompts
  • Learners benefit from being pushed to produce
    modified output by means of prompting, especially
    when
  • recasts might be perceived ambiguously as
    approving students use of non-target forms
  • students have reached a developmental plateau in
    their use of the non-target forms (i.e.,
    fossilized forms) and need to automatize target
    forms.

32
Conclusion
  • Continued recasting of what students already know
    is likely ineffective for ensuring continued
    development of L2 accuracy.
  • Continued prompting of learners to draw on what
    they have not yet acquired will be equally
    ineffective.
  • Effective L2 teachers need to orchestrate the use
    of both recasts and prompts, without abandoning
    one at the expense of the other.

33
References
  • Ammar, A. (2003). Corrective feedback and L2
    learning Elicitation and recasts. Unpublished
    doctoral dissertation, McGill University,
    Montreal.
  • Braidi, S. (2002). Reexamining the role of
    recasts in native-speaker/nonnative-speaker
    interactions. Language Learning, 52, 1-42.
  • de Bot, K. (1996). The psycholinguistics of the
    output hypothesis. Language Learning, 46,
    529-555.
  • Doughty, C. (2001). Cognitive underpinnings of
    focus on form. In P. Robinson (Ed.), Cognition
    and second language instruction (pp. 206-257).
    New York Cambridge University Press.
  • Doughty, C., Varela, E. (1998). Communicative
    focus on form. In C. Doughty J. Williams
    (Eds.), Focus on form in classroom second
    language acquisition (pp. 129-154). New York
    Cambridge University Press.
  • Havranek, G., Cesnik, H. (2001). Factors
    affecting the success of corrective feedback.
    EUROSLA Yearbook, 1, 99-122.
  • Krashen, S. (1994). The input hypothesis and its
    rivals. In N. Ellis (ed), Implicit and Explicit
    learning of Languages. London Academic Press,
    pp. 45-77.
  • Leeman, J. (2003). Recasts and second language
    development Beyond negative evidence. Studies in
    Second Language Acquisition, 25, 37-63.
  • Long, M. (1996). The role of the linguistic
    environment in second language acquisition. In W.
    C. Ritchie T. K. Bhatia (Eds.), Handbook of
    language acquisition Vol. 2. Second language
    acquisition (pp. 413-468). San Diego, CA
    Academic Press.
  • Long, M., Inagaki, S., Ortega, L. (1998). The
    role of implicit negative evidence in SLA Models
    and recasts in Japanese and Spanish. Modern
    Language Journal, 82, 357-371.

34
References
  • Long, M., Robinson, P. (1998). Focus on Form
    Theory, research, and practice. In C. Doughty
    J. Williams (Eds.), Focus on form in classroom
    second language acquisition (pp.15-41). New York
    Cambridge University Press.
  • Lyster, R. (1994b). La négociation de la forme
    stratégie analytique en classe dimmersion. The
    Canadian Modern Language Review, 50, 446-465
  • Lyster, R. (1998). Recasts, repetition, and
    ambiguity in L2 classroom discourse. Studies in
    Second Language Acquisition, 20, 55-85.
  • Lyster, R. (2002). Negotiation in immersion
    teacher-student interaction. International
    Journal of Educational Research, 37, 237-253
  • Lyster, R. (2004). Differential effects of
    prompts and recasts in form-focused instruction.
    Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 26
    399-432.
  • Lyster, R., Ranta, L. (1997). Corrective
    feedback and learner uptake Negotiation of form
    in communicative classrooms. Studies in Second
    Language Acquisition, 19, 37-66.
  • Mackey, A., Philp, J. (1998). Conversational
    interaction and second language development
    Recasts, responses, and red herrings? Modern
    Language Journal, 82, 338-356.
  • Oliver, R., Mackey, A. (2003). Interactional
    context and feedback in child ESL classrooms. The
    Modern Language Journal, 87, 519-533
  • Swain, M. (1985). Communicative competence Some
    roles of comprehensible input and comprehensible
    output in its development. In S. Gass C. Madden
    (Eds.), Input in second language acquisition (pp.
    235-253). Rowley, MA Newbury House.
  • Swain, M. (1988). Manipulating and complementing
    content teaching to maximize second language
    learning. TESL Canada Journal, 6, 68-83.
Write a Comment
User Comments (0)
About PowerShow.com