Generative Historical Syntax and the Linguistic Cycle - PowerPoint PPT Presentation

1 / 91
About This Presentation
Title:

Generative Historical Syntax and the Linguistic Cycle

Description:

In Joan Bybee and Paul Hopper, eds. Frequency and the Emergence ... Jack, George 1978. Negative adverbs in early ... (e.g. the Spanish inflectional morpheme ... – PowerPoint PPT presentation

Number of Views:63
Avg rating:3.0/5.0
Slides: 92
Provided by: EllyvanG8
Category:

less

Transcript and Presenter's Notes

Title: Generative Historical Syntax and the Linguistic Cycle


1
Generative Historical Syntax and the Linguistic
Cycle
  • Elly van Gelderen
  • ellyvangelderen_at_asu.edu
  • 29 March 2013
  • Harvard Linguistic Circle

2
Outline
  • A. What is Generative Historical Linguistics?
  • B. The healthy tension between generative grammar
    and historical linguistics, in both directions
    and how the current Minimalist Program is
    conducive to looking at gradual, unidirectional
    change.
  • C. Examples of Linguistic Cycles and how they can
    be explained and some challenges.

3
Model of language acquisition(based on Andersen
1973)
  • Generation n Generation n1
  • UG UG
  • experience experience n
  • I-language n I-language n1
  • E-language n E-language n1
  • innovations

4
Internal Grammar
5
Reanalysis is crucial
6
As for the tension Introspection vs text
  • Generative syntax has typically relied on
    introspective data.
  • For historical periods, such a method of data
    gathering is obviously impossible.
  • Generative grammar places much emphasis on the
    distinction between competence and performance,
    i.e. on I(nternal)- and E(xternal)-language.

7
Use (of texts and) corpora
  • Finding a pattern in a (spoken) corpus shows that
    there is something systematic going on
    repeatedly finding shouldof and shoulda indicates
    that something interesting is happening with
    modals and perfect auxiliaries
  • (1) I should of knew this was too good to be
    true.
  • (2) There xuld not a be do so mykele.
  • There shouldnt have been done so much.
    (Margaret Paston a1469)

8
That-trace
  • (1) Ac hwaet saegst ðu ðonne ðaet hwaet sie
    forcuðre ðonne sio ungesceadwisnes?
  • But what say you then that -- be wickeder
    than be foolishness
  • But what do you say is wickeder than
    foolishness?' (Boethius 36.8, from Allen 1977
    122)

9
Parsed Corpora
  • Since the 1990s, a group of generative linguists
    has worked on the creation of parsed corpora (see
    http//www.ling.upenn.edu/histcorpora/).
  • Result much better descriptions of changes in
    the word order (e.g. work by Pintzuk, Haeberli,
    Taylor, van Kemenade and others), changes in
    do-support (e.g. Kroch and Ecay), Adverb
    Placement (Haeberli, van Kemenade, and Los), and
    pro drop (Walkden).
  • Corpus work has reinvigorated Historical
    Linguistics.

10
  • Other historical (parsed) corpora have appeared
    or are appearing and spurring much work among
    generative and non-generative linguists
  • the Tycho Brahe parsed corpus of historical
    Portuguese, o corpus do Português,
  • the Corpus del Español,
  • the Regensburg Russian Diachronic Corpus,
  • a Hungarian corpus is under construction,
  • and COHA with a very helpful interface!

11
Some other issues of discussion
  • Change is unidirectional or not
  • and gradual or not
  • Current theory-internal questions
  • The role of UG
  • Language-specific or third factor
  • The role of features

12
The role of grammaticalization and
unidirectionality.
  • Is grammaticalization epiphenomenal or real?
  • Newmeyer (1998 237) Roberts Roussou (2003 2)
    and others grammaticalization is a regular case
    of parameter change and epiphenomenal all
    components also occur independently.
  • Others, e.g. van Gelderen (2004 2011), argue
    that the unidirectional patterns that are shown
    by grammaticalization can be explained the
    child reanalyzes the input in a certain way. This
    is where cycles come in!

13
Is change gradual or abrupt?
  • Most functionalist explanations assume it is
    gradual whereas many formal accounts think it is
    abrupt.
  • Early generative approaches emphasize a
    catastrophic reanalysis of both the underlying
    representation and the rules applying to them.
    Lightfoot, for instance, argues that the category
    change of modals is an abrupt one from V to AUX,
    as is the change from impersonal to personal
    verbs (the verb lician changing in meaning from
    please to like).

14
How to see the role of UG?
  • In the 1960s, UG consists of substantive
    universals, concerning universal categories (V,
    N, etc) and phonological features, and formal
    universals relating to the nature of rules. The
    internalized system is very language-specific.
  • Semantic features ..., are presumably drawn
    from a universal alphabet (Chomsky 1965 142),
    little is known about this today.

15
1990s-2013
  • Parameters now consist of choices of feature
    specifications as the child acquires a lexicon
    (Chomsky 2004 2007).
  • Baker, while disagreeing with this view of
    parameters, calls this the Borer-Chomsky-Conjectur
    e (2008 156)
  • "All parameters of variation are attributable to
    differences in the features of particular items
    (e.g., the functional heads) in the lexicon."

16
Shift
  • With the shift to parametric parameters, it
    becomes possible to think of gradual change
    through reanalysis as well (e.g. Roberts 2009 and
    van Gelderen 2009).
  • Word order change in terms if features e.g.
    Breitbarth 2012, Biberauer Roberts.
  • The set of features that are available to the
    learner is determined by UG.

17
Features and word order
  • Biberauer Roberts (2008) in examining the shift
    from OV to VO crucially rely on a EPP-feature. If
    T bears an EPP feature, a D head will adjoin to T
    or a DP will move to the specifier of the TP in
    Modern English. Languages can also have a VP or
    vP satisfy the EPP feature rather than just the
    DP contained in the VP or vP.

18
Features and grammaticalization
  • Another minimalist approach using features, not
    concerned with word order, can be found in van
    Gelderen (2004 2010) who argues that
    grammaticalization can be understood as a change
    from semantic to formal features.
  • For instance, a verb with semantic features, such
    as Old English will with volition, expectation,
    future, can be reanalyzed as having only the
    grammatical feature future.

19
A second shift
  • Faculty of Language is determined by
  • (1) genetic endowment, which sets limits on the
    attainable languages, thereby making language
    acquisition possible
  • (2) external data, converted to the experience
    that selects one or another language within a
    narrow range
  • (3) principles not specific to FL the Faculty of
    Language. Some of the third factor principles
    have the flavor of the constraints that enter
    into all facets of growth and evolution.... Among
    these are principles of efficient computation.
    (Chomsky 2007 3)

20
Third factors
  • We need more on third factors not well defined
    and invoked to account for a number of phenomena,
    e.g. pro-drop (Sigurðsson 2011), phrase structure
    (Medeiros 2012), and language change (van
    Gelderen 2011).
  • Constraints on word learning, such as the shape
    over color bias (Landau et al 1992), would also
    be third factor. Like UG before it, third factor
    reasons would remain stable and not responsible
    in language change.

21
Cycles tell us which features matter
  • Subject and Object Agreement
  • demonstrative/emphatic gt pronoun gt agreement gt
    zero
  • Copula Cycle
  • a demonstrative gt copula gt zero
  • b verb gt aspect gt copula
  • Case or Definiteness or DP
  • demonstrative gt definite article gt Case gt zero
  • Negative
  • a negative argument gt negative adverb gt negative
    particle gt zero
  • b verb gt aspect gt negative gt C
  • Future and Aspect Auxiliary
  • A/P gt M gt T gt C

22
Heine, Claudi Hünnemeyers 3 types
  • 1. isolated instances of grammaticalization, as
    when a lexical item grammaticalizes and is then
    replaced by a new lexeme. For instance, the
    lexical verb go (or want) being used as a future
    marker.
  • 2. subparts of language, for example, when the
    tense-aspect-mood system of a given language
    develops from a periphrastic into an inflexional
    pattern and back to a new periphrastic one or
    when negatives change.

23
and
  • 3. entire languages and language types but
    there is more justification to apply the notion
    of a linguistic cycle to individual linguistic
    developments, e.g. the development of future
    markers, of negatives, and of tense, rather than
    to changes in typological character, as in from
    analytic to synthetic and back to analytic.

24
Caution about the third kind
  • Heine et als reasons for caution about the third
    type of change, i.e. a cyclical change in
    language typology, is that we dont know enough
    about older stages of languages.
  • Most linguists are comfortable with cycles of the
    first and second kind but they are not with
    cycles of the third kind, e.g. Jespersen (1922
    chapter 21.9).

25
Macroparameters and microparameters
  • Baker (2001) and, more recently, Biberauer
    Roberts (2012) have formulated macro and micro
    parameters.
  • Macroparameters for Baker define the character of
    a particular language, e.g. polysynthetic or not,
    whereas microparameters for BR may involve the
    features of a particular lexical item.

26
Macrocycles and microcycles
  • In the same vein, it is possible to distinguish
    two kinds of cycles, a macrocycle and a
    microcycle.
  • A microcyle involves just one aspect of the
    language, for instance, negatives or
    demonstratives being reinforced by adverbs, as in
    English those people there. They include Heine et
    als first and second kind.
  • Macrocycles, more controversially, concern the
    entire linguistic system, i.e. Heine et als
    third kind.

27
von der Gabelentz 1901
  • Nun bewegt sich die Geschichte der Sprachen in
    der Diagonale zweier Kräfte des
    Bequemlichkeitstriebes, der zur Abnutzung der
    Laute führt, und des Deutlichkeitstriebes, der
    jene Abnutzung nicht zur Zerstörung der Sprache
    ausarten lässt. Die Affixe verschleifen sich,
    verschwinden am Ende spurlos ihre Funktionen
    aber oder ähnliche drängen wieder nach Ausdruck.

28
ctd
  • Diesen Ausdruck erhalten sie, nach der Methode
    der isolierenden Sprachen, durch Wortstellung
    oder verdeutlichende Wörter. Letztere unterliegen
    wiederum mit der Zeit dem Agglutinationsprozesse,
    dem Verschliffe und Schwunde, und derweile
    bereitet sich für das Verderbende neuer Ersatz
    vor ... immer gilt das Gleiche die
    Entwicklungslinie krümmt sich zurück nach der
    Seite der Isolation, nicht in die alte Bahn,
    sondern in eine annähernd parallele. Darum
    vergleiche ich sie der Spirale. (von der
    Gabelentz 1901 256)

29
  • The history of language moves in the diagonal of
    two forces the impulse toward comfort, which
    leads to the wearing down of sounds, and that
    toward clarity, which disallows this erosion and
    the destruction of the language. The affixes
    grind themselves down, disappear without a trace
    their functions or similar ones, however, require
    new expression. They acquire this expression, by
    the method of isolating languages, through word
    order or clarifying words. The latter, in the
    course of time, undergo agglutination, erosion,
    and in the mean time renewal is prepared
    periphrastic expressions are preferred ... always
    the same the development curves back towards
    isolation, not in the old way, but in a parallel
    fashion. That's why I compare them to spirals.

30
Comfort Clarity Grammaticalization Renewal
  • Von der Gabelentz examples of comfort
  • the unclear pronunciation of everyday
    expressions,
  • the use of a few words instead of a full
    sentence, i.e. ellipsis (p. 182-184),
  • syntaktische Nachlässigkeiten aller Art
    (syntactic carelessness of all kinds, p. 184),
  • and loss of gender.

31
(No Transcript)
32
Von der Gs examples of clarity
  • special exertion of the speech organs (p. 183),
  • Wiederholung (repetition, p. 239),
  • periphrastic expressions (p. 239),
  • replacing words like sehr very by more powerful
    and specific words such as riesig gigantic and
    schrecklich frightful (243),
  • using a rhetorical question instead of a regular
    proposition,
  • and replacing case with prepositions (p. 183).

33
Grammaticalization one step
  • Hopper Traugott 2003 content item gt
    grammatical word gt clitic gt inflectional affix.
  • The loss in phonological content is not a
    necessary consequence of the loss of semantic
    content (see Kiparsky 2011 Kiparsky Condoravdi
    2006 Hoeksema 2009).
  • Kiparsky (2011 19) in the development of case,
    bleaching is not necessarily tied to
    morphological downgrading from postposition to
    clitic to suf?x.
  • Instead, unidirectionality is the defining
    property of grammaticalization and any exceptions
    to the unidirectionality (e.g. the Spanish
    inflectional morpheme nos changing to a pronoun)
    are instances of analogical changes, according to
    Kiparsky (2011).

34
  • In acknowledging weakening of pronunciation (un
    affaiblissement de la pronunciation), Meillet
    (1912 139) writes that what provokes the start
    of the (negative) cycle is the need to speak
    forcefully (le besoin de parler avec force).
  • Kiparsky Condoravdi (2006) find no evidence for
    phonetic weakening in Jespersens Cycle in Greek
    and similarly suggest pragmatic and semantic
    reasons. A simple negative cannot be emphatic in
    order for a negative to be emphatic, it needs to
    be reinforced, e.g. by a minimizer. When emphatic
    negatives are overused, their semantic impact
    weakens and they become the regular negative and
    a new emphatic will appear.

35
Main question
  • How does the child respond to these fast changes?
  • Feature-spread through the clausal skeleton is
    reanalyzed.

36
Microcycle
  • (1)a. Im gonna leave for the summer.
  • b. Im gonna to Flagstaff for the summer.
  • Nesselhauf (2012) identifies three features,
    intention, prediction, and arrangement, in the
    change of shall, will, ll, be going to, be to,
    and the progressive) in the last 250 years as
    the sense of intention is lost and replaced by
    the sense of prediction, new markers of intention
    will appear
  • want has intention in (4a) and it is starting to
    gain the sense of prediction, as in (4b).
  • (2)a. The final injury I want to talk about is
    brain damage ... (Nesselhauf 2012 114).
  • b. We have an overcast day today that looks like
    it wants to rain. (Nesselhauf 2012 115).

37
Going to
  • Nesselhaufs data on BE going to show that its
    use as a future marker has increased, both in the
    intention and prediction sense, and that the
    proportion of pure prediction is increasing.
  • Once the sense of prediction prevails, another
    verb may be taking over to compensate for the
    feature of intention.

38
Macrocycles
  • Hodge (1971)
  • Proto-Afroasiatic analytic Sm
  • Old Egyptian synthetic sM
  • Late Egyptian analytic Sm
  • Coptic synthetic sM
  • Huang (to appear)
  • Chinese, from moderately synthetic to analytic to
    moderately synthetic.
  • August Wilhem von Schlegel 1818 for the use of
    analytic and synthetic.

39
Attachment Type Cycle
  • Isolating
  • Inflectional Agglutinative
  • Morphemes per word?

40
Four (micro)cycles I will look at
  • Negative Cycles
  • negative argument gt negative adverb gt negative
    particle gt zero
  • negative verb gt auxiliary gt negative gt zero
  • Subject Agreement Cycle
  • demonstrative/emphatic gt pronoun gt agreement gt
    zero
  • Copula Cycles
  • demonstrative/verb/adposition gt copula gt zero
  • Demonstrative gt article/copula/tense marker

41
Two Negative Cycles
  • I Indefinite phrase gt negative Jespersens
    Cycle
  • Negation weakens and is renewed. For instance
  • (1) I cant do that gt
  • (2) I cant see nothing
  • II Verb gt negative
  • (3) is-i ba-d-o Koorete
  • she-NOM disappear-PF-PST
  • She disappeared' (Binyam 2007 7).
  • (4) is-i dana ush-u-wa-nni-ko
  • she-NOM beer drink-PRES-not_exist-3FS-FOC
  • She does (will) not drink beer. (Binyam 2007
    9).

42
Negative Cycle in Old English450-1150 CE
  • a. no/ne early Old English
  • b. ne (na wiht/not) after 900, esp S
  • c. (ne) not after 1350
  • d. not gt -not/-nt after 1400

43
  • Old English
  • (1) Men ne cunnon secgan to soðe ... hwa
  • Man not could tell to truth ... who
  • No man can tell for certain ... who'.
  • (2) Næron 3e noht æmetti3e, ðeah ge wel ne dyden
  • not-were you not unoccupied. though you well not
    did
  • You were not unoccupied, though you did not do
    well'.

44
Weakening and renewal
  • (1) we cannot tell of (Wycliff Sermons from the
    1380s)
  • (2) But I shan't put you to the trouble of
    farther Excuses, if you please this Business
    shall rest here. (Vanbrugh, The Relapse1680s).
  • (3) that the sonne dwellith therfore nevere the
    more ne lasse in oon signe than in another
    (Chaucer, Astrolabe 665 C1).
  • (4) No, I never see him these days (BNC - A9H
    350)

45
Negative source is a verb
  • (1) wo mei you shu Chinese
  • I not exist book
  • I don't have a book.
  • (2) Yao Shun ji mo ... Old Chinese
  • Yao Shun since died
  • Since Yao and Shun died, ...'
  • (Mengzi, Tengwengong B, from Lin 2002 5)
  • (3)yu de wang ren mei kunan, ... Early Mandarin
  • wish PRT died person not-be suffering
  • If you wish that the deceased one has no
    suffering, ...'
  • (Dunhuang Bianwen, from Lin 2002 5-6)

46
One Negative Cycle, e.g. English, French, Arabic
  • XP
  • Spec X'
  • na wiht X YP
  • not gt nt

47
  • And a second
  • According to Lin, mei went through a perfective
    stage, so
  • (4) dayi ye mei you chuan, jiu zou le chulai
  • coat even not PF wear, then walk PF out
  • He didn't even put on his coat and walked out.'
    (Rulin Waishi, from Lin 2002 8)
  • (5) NegP
  • Neg ASPP
  • mei
  • ASP VP
  • mei
  • V ...
  • mei

48
The Subject Cycle
  • A. demonstrative gt third person pron gt clitic gt
    agreement
  • B. oblique gt emphatic gt first/second pron gt
    clitic gt agreement
  • noun gt
  • (1) Shi diné bizaad yíní-sh-ta'
  • I Navajo language 3-1-study
  • As for me, I am studying Navajo.

49
Brazilian Portuguese
  • (1) Vossa mercê gt Vosmecê gt (V)ocê gt cê
  • your favor/mercy you you-indefinite
  • (see Mattoso Câmara 1979 Gonçalves 1987 Dutra
    1991, cited in Vitral Ramos 2006)
  • (2) cê only in subject position and pre-V
  • (3) ele(s) gt el, es
  • ela(s) gt éa, éas
  • (4) es inventa um bocado de coisa / eles inventam
  • they invented (S)

50
Some stages
  • Japanese and Urdu/Hindi full pronoun
  • (1) watashi-wa kuruma-o unten-suru kara.
  • I-TOP car-ACC drive-NONPST PRT
  • I will drive the car'. (Yoko Matsuzaki p.c.)
  • (2)a. m?y nee us ko dekha
  • 1S ERG him DAT saw
  • b. aadmii nee kitaab ko peRha
  • man ERG book DAT read
  • (3) ham log we people
  • (4) m?y or merii behn doonõ dilii m?y rehtee h?
  • I and my sister both Delhi in living are

51
English in transition
  • (a) Modification, (b) coordination, (c) position,
  • (d) doubling, (e) loss of V-movement, (f) Code
    switching
  • Coordination (and Case)
  • (1) Me and Kitty were to spend the day.
  • (2) while he and she went across the hall.
  • Position
  • (3) Shes very good, though I perhaps I shouldnt
    say so.
  • (4) You maybe you've done it but have forgotten.
  • (5) Me, I was flying economy, but the plane,
    was guzzling gas

52
Doubling and cliticization
  • (1) Me, I've tucking had it with the small place.
  • (BNC H0M 1608)
  • (2) Him, he ....
  • (3) Her, she shouldnt do that (not attested
    in the BNC)
  • (4) As for a dog, it should be happy.
  • CSE-FAC
  • uncliticized cliticized total
  • I 2037 685 (25) 2722
  • you 1176 162 (12.1) 1338
  • he 128 19 (12.9) 147

53
Loss of V-movement and Code switching
  • (5) What I'm gonna do?
  • What am I going to do'
  • (6) How she's doing?
  • How is she doing
  • (7) He ging weg he went away Dutch-English CS
  • (8) The neighbor ging weg

54
French
  • (1) Se je meïsme ne li di Old French
  • If I myself not him tell
  • If I dont tell him myself. (Franzén 193920,
    Cligès 993)
  • (2) Renars respond Jou, je nirai
  • R answers Me, I wont go.
  • (Coronnement Renart, A. Foulet (ed.) 1929 598,
    from Roberts 1993 112)

55
  • (1)a. Je heureusement ai vu ça I I probably
    have seen that
  • Ive probably seen that.
  • b. Kurt, heureusement, a fait beaucoup d'autres
    choses.
  • Kurt fortunately has done many other things
  • Fortunately, Kurt did many other things
    (google search of French websites)
  • (2) Où vas-tu Standard French
  • where go-2S
  • (3) tu vas où Colloquial French
  • 2S go where Where are you going?'

56
Subject Cycle
  • Full phrase move to Spec TP gt
  • Head moves to T
  • Reanalysis as to what the head is pronoun or
    agreement.
  • (Economy agreement uninterpretable and then
    this needs an interpretable feature as well)

57
Copula cycle, sources
  • Verbs
  • Demonstratives
  • Prepositions etc
  • Reanalysis of location, identity, and aspect
    features

58
Copulas in English
  • The flavors e.g. English be, become, go, fall,
    turn, seem, appear, stay, and remain.
  • semantic features
  • be remain seem, appear stay
  • location duration visible duration
  • equal

59
Demonstrative and adverbial source of copulas
  • (1) a. Mi da i tatá Saramaccan I am your
    father
  • I am your father. (McWhorter 1997 87)
  • b. Hen dà dí Gaamá
  • he is the chief
  • He's the chief. (McWhorter 1997 98)
  • (2) Dí wómi de a wósu
  • the woman is at house
  • The woman is at home. (McWhorter 1997 88)

60
Demonstrative to article cycle
  • (1) demonstrative/adverb gt definite article gt
    Case/non-generic gt class marker gt 0
  • (2) gife to þa munecas of þe mynstre give to
    the monks of the abbey (Peterborough Chron. 656)
  • (3) the

61
Reduction of the article and renewal
  • (3) Morret's brother came out of Scoteland for
    th'acceptacion of the peax
  • (The Diary of Edward VI, 1550s)
  • (4) Oh they used to be ever so funny houses you
    know and in them days They used to have big
    windows, but they used to a all be them there
    little tiny ones like that. (BNC - FYD 72)

62
Around 1200 a reanalysis
  • (1) gaddresst swa þe clene corn
  • and so you gather the clear wheat. (Ormulum
    1484-5, Holt edition)
  • (2) 3ho wass Elysabæþ 3ehatenn
  • She was called Elisabeth. (Ormulum 115)
  • (3) swa þe33 leddenn heore lif Till þatt te33
    wærenn alde
  • and so they led their lives until they were
    old. (Ormulum 125-6)
  • (4) þin forrme win iss swiþe god, þin lattre win
    iss bettre.
  • Your earlier wine is very good, your later wine
    is better. (Ormulum 15409)

63
Demonstratives, pronouns, and pro-drop in Old
English
  • (1) þæt fram ham gefrægn Higelaces þegn, god mid
    Geatum, Grendles dæda se wæs moncynnes mægenes
    strengest on þæm dæge þysses lifes, æþele ond
    eacen.
  • Hygelacs thane heard about Grendels deeds
    while in Geatland he (Hygelacs thane) was
    mankinds strongest man on earth, noble and
    powerful.

64
Old English ctd
  • Het him yðlidan godne gegyrwan, cwæð, he
    guðcyning ofer swanrade secean wolde, mærne
    þeoden, þa him wæs manna þearf. ðone siðfæt him
    snotere ceorlas lythwon logon, þeah he him leof
    wære.
  • (He) ordered himself a good boat prepared and
    said that he wanted to seek the king over the sea
    since he (the king) needed men. Wise men did not
    stop him (Hygelacs thane) though he was dear to
    them. (Beowulf 194-98)

65
Traugott (1992 171)
  • (2) Þa clypode an ðæra manna Zebeus gehaten and
    cwæð to ðam cyninge
  • Then cried one of-the men Zebeus called and
    said to the king
  • Eala ðu cyning þas fulan wuhta þu scoldest
    awurpan of ðinum rice.
  • Oh you king the foul creatures you should
    throw-out of your kingdom
  • ðylæs ðe hi mid heora fylðe us ealle besmiton
  • in-case that they the foul creatures with
    their filth us all affect
  • Hi habbað mid him awyriedne engel. mancynnes
    feond.
  • They the foul creatures have with them
    corrupt angel, mankinds enemy
  • and se hæfð andweald on ðam mannum ðe heora
    scyppend forseoð.
  • and he the angel has power over those men that
    their creator despise
  • and to deofolgyldum bugað
  • and to idols bow.
  • (DOE Segment 8 Ælfrics Catholic Homilies,
    second series M. Godden 1979, p. 283. 110 115)

66
What happens?
  • Externally a strengthening of the third person
    features in the pronoun and a shift in the
    relationship with the demonstrative.
  • This reinforcement through external pronouns, she
    and they, brought about a reanalysis of the
    features of the pronoun as deictic.

67
Internal External
  • se --gt the seo --gt she
  • that --gt that hi --gt they
  • him/her --gt himself/herself
  • a. se/that gt the
  • i-loc/i-phi u-T/u-ps ( -Ps)
  • b. he/hi is replaced by he
  • heo/ha is replaced by she (possibly via seo)
  • hi/hie is replaced by they
  • i-phi i-phi/i-loc

68
  • Demonstrative
  • i-phi/ i-loc
  •  article Dem C copula
  • u-phi i-phi u/i-T u-phi
  • i-loc i-loc
  • Also degree adverb and tense marker
    (Tibeto-Burman)
  • Feature Economy
  • Utilize semantic features use them as for
    functional categories, i.e. as formal features.

69
Types of minimalist features
  • The semantic features of lexical items (which
    have to be cognitively based not UG)
  • The interpretable ones relevant at the
    Conceptual-Intentional interface.
  • Uninterpretable features act as glue so to
    speak to help out merge. For instance, person and
    number features (phi-features) are interpretable
    on nouns but not on verbs.

70
The importance of features
  • Chomsky (1965 87-88) lexicon contains
    information for the phonological, semantic, and
    syntactic component.
  • Sincerity N, -Count, Abstract...)
  • Chomsky (1995 230ff 236 277ff)
  • semantic (e.g. abstract object),
  • phonological (e.g. the sounds),
  • and formal features
  • intrinsic or optional.

71
Formal features are interpretable and
uninterpretable (1995 277)
  • airplane build
  • Interpr. nominal verbal
  • 3 person assign non-human
    accusative
  • Uninterpr Case phi

72
Simplifying checking
  • He reads books
  • before
  • checking i-3S u-phi i-3P
  • after
  • checking i-3S u-phi i-3P
  • Thats why me sees him is ok!

73
Major Issues
  • Where do features come from?
  • Cartography vs Bare Phrase Structure
  • (1) Tpast Tfut Moodir Modnec Modpos
    ASPhab ASPrep ASPfreq
  • once then perhaps necessarily possibly
    usually again often
  • (from Cinque 1999 107)

74
Semantic and formal overlap
  • Chomsky (1995 230 381) suggests "formal
    features have semantic correlates and reflect
    semantic properties (accusative Case and
    transitivity, for example)."
  • I interpret this If a language has nouns with
    semantic phi-features, the learner will be able
    to hypothesize uninterpretable features on
    another F (and will be able to bundle them
    there).

75
Feature Economy
  • (a) Utilize semantic features use them as for
    functional categories, i.e. as formal features.
  • (b) If a specific feature appears more than once,
    one of these is interpretable and the others are
    uninterpretable

76
Innate vs acquired
  • shapes grammatical number
  • negatives negation
  • if
  • real-unreal irrealis
  • /-individuated mass-count
  • duration progressive

77
Loss of semantic features
  • Full verbs such as Old English will with
  • volition, expectation, future features are
    reanalyzed as having only the feature future in
    Middle English.
  • And the negative
  • OE no/ne gt ME (ne) not gt -nt
  • gt ModE nt ... nothing, never, etc

78
The various cycles in terms of features
  • The cycle of agreement
  • noun gt emphatic gt pronoun gt agreement gt 0
  • sem i-phi i-phi/u-phi u-phi
  • The cycles of negation
  • a Adjunct/Argument Specifier Head (of
    NegP) affix
  • semantic gt i-NEGgt u-NEG gt --
  • b. Lexical Head gt (higher) Head gt (higher) Head gt
    0
  • neg i-NEG/F F

79
Verb and demonstrative to copula
  • Assume copulas have
  • be remain seem
  • i-loc i-loc i-loc
  • i-ASP i-M
  • Source for loc? Verbs and demonstratives
  • D gt copula gt zero
  • i-loc gt i-loc gt --
  • i-phi gt u-phi
  • u-T

80
Demonstrative gt article
  • a. DP gt b. DP
  • that D' D'
  • i-ps D NP D NP
  • i-locu- N the N
  • i-phi u-phi i-phi
  • Hence (1) I saw the
  • (2) I saw that/those.

81
  • Demonstrative
  • i-phi
  • i-loc
  •  
  • article pronoun C copula
  • u-phi i-phi u/i-T i-loc
  • u-T

82
A possible Feature Macro-Parameter
  • Phi-features Case'
  • (for head-marking) (for dependent-marking)
  • yes no yes no
  • Japanese Japanese Navajo
  • u-F i-F English
  • English Navajo
  • Phi-T no
  • English Bantu (EvG to appear)

83
Explanations of the Cycle
  • Recent shift towards third factors and parametric
    features we need to be careful how many
    mechanisms we allow.
  • Therefore, Feature Economy makes sense
  • All change is in the lexicon semgti-Fgtu-F
  • Why?
  • Maximize syntax?
  • Keep merge going?
  • Lighter?

84
Summary
  • Review of GG and HL
  • Introspection vs corpora/texts
  • Gradual, unidirectional change
  • Role of UG determines what changes
  • PS rules gt parameters gt features
  • Challenges

85
Some References
  • Allen, Cynthia 1977. Topics in Diachronic English
    Syntax. UMASS Diss.
  • Andersen, henning 1973. in Language.
  • LArrivée, Pierre 2010. The pragmatic motics of
    the Jespersen Cycle Default, activation, and the
    history of negation in French. Lingua 120 (9)
    2240-2258.
  • Baker, Mark 2001. The Atoms of Language. New
    York Basic Books.
  • Baker, Mark 2008. The syntax of Agreement and
    Concord. Cambridge Cambridge University Press.

86
  • Biberauer, Theresa Ian Roberts 2012. The
    Significance of What Hasnt Happened. DIGS 14,
    Lisbon, 4 July.
  • Bopp, Franz 1816. Über das Conjugationssystem der
    Sanskritsprache in Vergleichung mit jenem der
    griechischen, lateinischen, persischen und
    germanischen Sprachen. Frankfurt-am-Main.
  • Breitbarth, Anne 2012. The Development of
    Conditional Modal Verbs. DIGS 14, Lisbon, July.

87
  • Chomsky, Noam 1965. Aspects of the theory of
    syntax. Cambridge, MA. MIT Press.
  • Chomsky, Noam 1995. The Minimalist Program.
    Cambridge MIT Press.
  • Chomsky, Noam 2007. Approaching UG from below. In
    Uli Sauerland et al. (eds), Interfaces
    Recursion Language, 1-29. Berlin Mouton de
    Gruyter.
  • Closs, Elizabeth 1965. Diachronic Syntax and
    Generative grammar. Language 41.3 402-415.
  • Closs Traugott, Elizabeth 1972. A History of
    English Syntax. New York Holt, Rinehart
    Winston.
  • Condillac, Etienne Bonnot de 1746. Essai sur
    lórigine des connaissances humaines. Paris.
  • Crowley, Terry. 1992. An Introduction to
    Historical Linguistics. Auckland Oxford
    University Press.
  • Dahl, Osten 2001. In?ationary Effects in Language
    and Elsewhere. In Joan Bybee and Paul Hopper,
    eds. Frequency and the Emergence of Linguistic
    Structure, 471-480. Philadelphia Benjamins.
  • Franzén, Torsten 1939. Etude sur la syntaxe des
    pronoms personnels sujets en ancien français.
    Uppsala Almqvist Wiksells Boktryckeri.
  • Gabelentz, Georg von der 18911901. Die
    Sprachwissenshaft. Ihre Aufgaben, Methoden und
    bisherigen Ergebnisse. Leipzig Weigel. reprint
    Tübingen Narr 1972.

88
  • . Gardiner, Alan H. 1904. The word ...
    Zeitschrift für Ägyptische Sprache und
    Altertumskunde 41 130-135.
  • Gelderen, Elly van 2000. A History of English
    Reflexive Pronouns. Amsterdam John Benjamins.
  • Gelderen, Elly van 2004. Grammaticalization as
    Economy. Amsterdam John Benjamins.
  • Gelderen, Elly van 2008. Where did Late Merge go?
    Grammaticalization as Feature Economy. Studia
    Linguistica 287-300.
  • Gelderen, Elly van 2011. The Linguistic Cycle.
    Oxford Oxford University Press.
  • Givón, Talmy 1971. Historical syntax and
    synchronic morphology. Chicago Linguistic Society
    Proceedings 7 394-415.
  • Givón, Talmy 1976. Topic, pronoun, and
    grammatical agreement. In Charles Li (ed.)
    Subject and Topic, 151-188. New York Academic
    Press.
  • Greenberg, Joseph 1978. How does a language
    acquire gender markers? In Joseph Greenberg
    (ed.), Universals of Human Language 3, 47-82.
    Stanford University Press.

89
  • Harris, Martin 1978. The Evolution of French
    Syntax. London Longman.
  • Heine, Bernd, Ulrike Claudi, Friederike
    Hünnemeyer 1991. Grammaticalization A conceptual
    framework. Chicago University of Chicago Press.
  • Heine, Bernd Tania Kuteva 2005. Language
    Contact and Grammatical Change. Cambridge
    Cambridge University Press.
  • Hengeveld, Kees 1992. Non-verbal Predication.
    Berlin Mouton de Gruyter.
  • Hodge, Carleton 1970. The Linguistic Cycle.
    Linguistic Sciences 13 1-7.
  • Hoeksema, Jack 2009. Jespersen Recycled. In Elly
    van Gelderen Cyclical Change, 15-34. Amsterdam
    John Benjamins.
  • Hopper, Mike Elizabeth Traugott 2003.
    Grammaticalization. Cambridge Cambridge
    University Press.
  • Huang, James to appear. On Syntactic Analyticity
    and Parametric Theory. In Handbook of Chinese
    Languages. Wiley-Blackwell.
  • Humboldt, Wilhelm von 1836. Über die
    Verschiedenheit des menschlichen Sprachbaus und
    seinen Einfluss auf die geistige Entwicklung des
    Menschengeschlechts.
  • Ingham, Richard Pierre LArrivée (eds.). to
    appear. The Evolution of Negation Beyond the
    Jespersen Cycle. Berlin Mouton de Gruyter.

90
  • Jack, George 1978. Negative adverbs in early
    Middle English. English Studies 59 295-309.
  • Jespersen, Otto 1917 1966. Negation in English
    and other Languages. Copenhagen A.F. Høst.
    second edition
  • Jespersen, Otto 1922. Language. London Allen
    Unwin.
  • Katz, Aya 1996. Cyclical Grammaticalization and
    the Cognitive Link between Pronoun and Copula.
    Rice Dissertation.
  • Kiparsky, Paul 1965. Phonological Change. MIT
    PhD.
  • Kiparsky, Paul 1996. Indo-European Origins of
    Germanic Syntax. In Ian Roberts and Adrian Battye
    (eds.), Clause Structure and Language Change,
    140-167. Oxford Oxford University Press.
  • Kiparsky, Paul 2011. Grammaticalization as
    Optimization. In Dianne Jonas, John Whitman, and
    Andrew Garrett, Grammatical Change Origins,
    Nature, Outcomes, 15-51. Oxford Oxford
    University Press.
  • Kiparsky, Paul Cleo Condoravdi (2006). Tracking
    Jespersens Cycle. In Mark Janse, Brian Joseph
    A. Ralli (eds.). Proceedings of the 2nd
    International Conference of Modern Greek Dialects
    and Linguistic Theory, 172-197. Mytilene Doukas.
  • Kemenade, Ans van 1987. Syntactic Case and
    Morphological Case in the History of English.
    Dordrecht Foris.
  • Kemenade, Ans van Nigel Vincent (eds) 1997.
    Parameters of Morphosyntactic Change. CUP.
  • King, Robert 1969. Historical Linguistics and
    Generative Grammar. Prentice Hall.
  • Klima, Edward 1965. Studies in Diachronic
    Transformational Syntax. Harvard PhD.
  • Kroch, Anthony Anne Taylor 1996. Verb movement
    in Old and Middle English dialect variation and
    language contact. In Ans van Kemenade and Nigel
    Vincent (eds.), Parameters of Morphosyntactic
    Change, 297-325. Cambridge Cambridge University
    Press.
  • Labov, William 1972. Language in the Inner City.
    Philadelphia University of Philadelphia Press.
  • Lakoff, Robin 1968. Abstract Syntax and Latin
    Complementation. MIT Press.
  • Lambrecht, Knud 1981. Topic, Antitopic, and Verb
    Agreement in Non Standard French. Amsterdam John
    Benjamins.
  • Landau, Barbara 1994. Wheres what and whats
    where the language of objects in space. Lingua
    92 259-296.
  • Lightfoot, David 1974. The diachronic analysis of
    English modals. Historical Linguistics ed. by J.
    M. Anderson C. Jones, 219-49. Amsterdam North
    Holland Publishing.
  • Lightfoot, David 1979. Principles of Diachronic
    Syntax. Cambridge Cambridge University Press.

91
  • Li, Charles, and Sandra Thompson. 1977. A
    mechanism for the development of copula
    morphemes. In Charles Li (ed.), Mechanisms of
    syntactic change, 414-444. Austin University of
    Texas Press.
  • Los, Bettelou 2012 The loss of verb-second and
    the switch from bounded to unbounded systems. In
    Meurman-Solin, Anneli et al. (eds), Informationa
    Structure and Syntactic Change in the History of
    English, 21-46. Oxford Oxford University Press.
  • McWhorter, John 1997. Towards a new model of
    creole genesis. New York Peter Lang.
  • Medeiros, David 2012. Economy of Command.
    University of Arizona PhD.
  • Meillet, Antoine. (1921 1912) "L'évolution des
    formes grammaticales". In Linguistique
    historique et linguistique générale (ed. Édouard
    Champion). Paris Librairie Ancienne Honoré
    Champion, 130-148. reprint 1958
  • Nesselhauf, Nadja 2012. Mechanisms of language
    change in a functional system the recent
    semantic evolution of English future time
    expressions. Journal of Historical
    Linguistics 2.1 83-132.
  • Pustet, Regina. 2003. Copulas Universals in the
    Categorization of the Lexicon. Oxford Oxford
    University Press.
  • Roberts, Ian 1993. Verbs and Diachronic Syntax.
    Dordrecht Kluwer.
  • Roberts, Ian 2009. Grammaticalization, the
    clausal hierarchy, and semantic bleaching. In
    Graeme Trousdale Elizabeth Traugott (eds),
    Gradience, Gradualness, and Grammaticalization,
    45-73. Amsterdam John Benjamins.

92
  • Roberts, Ian Anna Roussou 2003. Syntactic
    Change A Minimalist Approach to
    Grammaticalization. Cambridge CUP.
  • Robins, R.H. 1967. A Short History of
    Linguistics. London Longman.
  • Schlegel, August Wilhem von 1818. Observations
    sur la langue et la literature provençales.
    Paris.
  • Schwegler, Armin 1990. Analyticity and
    Syntheticity. Berlin Mouton de Gruyter.
  • Sigurðsson, Halldór. 2011. Conditions on Argument
    Drop. Linguistic Inquiry 42.2 267-304.
  • Stassen, Leon. 1997. Intransitive Predication.
    Oxford Oxford University Press.
  • Tauli, Valter 1958. The Structural Tendencies of
    Languages. Helsinki.
  • Tauli, Valter 1966. Structural Tendencies in
    Uralic Languages. Den Haag Mouton.
  • Tooke, John Horne 1786-1805. The Diversion of
    Purley. London. .
  • Traugott, Elizabeth Bernd Heine 1991.
    Grammaticalization. Amsterdam John Benjamins.
  • Trudgill, Peter Jenny Cheshire 1998. The
    sociolinguistics reader volume 1. London Arnold.
  • van der Auwera, Johan 2009. The Jespersen Cycles.
    In Elly van Gelderen (ed.) Cyclical Change,
    35-71. Amsterdam John Benjamins.
  • Vance, Barbara 1997. Syntactic Change in Medieval
    French. Dordrecht Kluwer.
  • Vitral, Lorenso Jânia Ramos 2006.
    Gramaticalização uma abordagem formal. Rio de
    Janeiro Tempo Brasileiro.
  • Zribi-Hertz, A. (1994). La syntaxe des clitiques
    nominatifs. Travaux de Linguistique et
    Litterature 131-147.
Write a Comment
User Comments (0)
About PowerShow.com