Social influence Conformity, obedience - PowerPoint PPT Presentation

About This Presentation
Title:

Social influence Conformity, obedience

Description:

Social influence Conformity, obedience A large part of psychology is devoted to the study of the various ways that people influence each other when they are together. – PowerPoint PPT presentation

Number of Views:167
Avg rating:3.0/5.0
Slides: 44
Provided by: MarekF8
Category:

less

Transcript and Presenter's Notes

Title: Social influence Conformity, obedience


1
Social influenceConformity, obedience
2
  • A large part of psychology is devoted to the
    study of the various ways that people influence
    each other when they are together.
  • It has been established for quite a long time
    that people can have an effect on each other's
    behaviour, just by being there.
  • Some people feel that they can truly be
    'themselves' only when they are alone, while
    other people feel happier if they are amongst
    others in a crowd or a group of friends.

3
  • Triplett (1898) set up one of the first
    experimental studies of social influence.
  • He asked children to turn a fishing reel as fast
    as they possibly could, and measured how quickly
    they could do it. Then he arranged things so that
    they would either be doing it alone, or with a
    friend or another child.
  • When the children were working in coaction
    (together) he found that they worked very much
    faster even just the presence of another person
    seemed to have a stimulating effect on their
    behaviour.

4
  • Do we agree with others, even we have a different
    opinion?
  • Do we obey commands of others, even if we do not
    strongly agree with them?
  • Conformity
  • Obedience

5
Conformity
  • Asch (1951).
  • He set up an experiment in which subjects were
    told that they were participating in a perception
    study.
  • They were asked to judge the correct length of a
    given line, by identifying which of three sample
    lines was identical to it.
  • Asch arranged that the subjects would be tested
    as members of a group, all of whom would report
    their judgments openly.
  • Unknown to the subjects, the rest of the group
    consisted of 'stooges (assistants of the
    experimenter), who, from time to time,
    deliberately gave prearranged but obviously wrong
    answers.

6
  • Subjects clearly found it uncomfortable when they
    were in a position of disagreeing with the
    majority
  • Many of them at one time or another during the
    study gave answers which they knew to be wrong,
    but which conformed with the majority judgments.

7
  • In total, 74 of Asch's subjects conformed at
    least once, and 32 of them conformed all the
    time.
  • Asch reported that the anxieties being
    experienced by the subjects as they heard others
    giving the wrong judgments.

8
  • A more recent study by Perrin and Spencer (1980)
    involved replicating Asch's experiments with a
    new set of subjects.
  • They found that, despite the clear existence of
    anxiety by the subjects, subjects did not conform
    as they did in Asch's study.
  • Suggestion the Asch effect might be 'a child of
    its time' rather than a general effect.

9
  • However, Doms and Avermaet (1981) questioned this
    conclusion.
  • They argued that a possible reason was because of
    their effort to obtain subjects who had not heard
    of the original Asch experiments.
  • In doing so, they had asked students from
    disciplines such as engineering, chemistry, and
    medicine to take part in the studies. Students
    from scientific disciplines were likely to
    emphasise the need for correct judgements of
    measurements.
  • Even though they disliked going against the
    majority, they would feel obliged to do so,
    because they would see accuracy as being
    important.

10
Reasons for conformity
  • An effort to avoid a conflict.
  • Subjects believed that a group harmony is more
    important than the correct answer.
  • Subjects had feel anxiety during incorrect
    answers.

11
Asch (1951)
12
Asch (1951)
13
Types of conformity
  • Kelman (1958) identified three main forms of
    conformity
  • (1) Compliance - going along with the majority,
    however we do not change our opinions.
  • (2) Identification a change of opinions and
    behaviors, identification with an another person.
  • (3) Internalization we agree that opinions of
    majority are better than our one.

14
(1) Compliance
  • This form of conformity which is the most
    superficial,
  • Going along with the majority in order to avoid
    rejection,
  • or to gain rewards such as social acceptance or
    approva1.
  • The distinguishing feature of compliance,, is
    that the conforming behaviour will only last as
    long as the situation does.
  • Once the inf1uencing 'agents' are no longer
    present, the individual will stop conforming.

15
(2) Identification
  • The individual conforms at particular times and
    in particular ways because these are part of a
    general overall series of relationships which he
    or she is trying to maintain.
  • In this type of conformity, the particular
    behavior is not an important thing in itself but
    only as it forms part of a whole system of
    behavior which establishes or maintains some kind
    of relationship.

16
  • Example
  • A shop assistant who wishes to establish a
    positive working relationship with a supervisor
    will tend to conform to the behavior expected of
    a 'good' assistant - being pleasant with
    customers, accurate and fast when serving, polite
    and attentive to other employees, and so on.
  • Although the individual will actually be likely
    to believe in each of these acts of conformity as
    a 'right way to behave, none of the individual
    acts will matter much on their own. They all
    arise from the person's identifying with the role
    of the assistant, and conforming to that.

17
(3) Internalization
  • This form is concerned with an individual's own
    personal value system, their way of understanding
    the world and morality in both small and large
    matters.
  • Someone may accept another person's inf1uence,
    and conform to their demands or expectations
    because they fully agree with the principles
    involved.

18
Milgrams study
  • Milgram
  • Milgram, S. (1963). Behavioral study of
    obedience. Journal of Abnormal and Social
    Psychology,52, 500-510.
  • Experiment influence of pain on learning

19
  • A subject was informed that he/she takes part in
    an experiment. The goal of a study is help people
    to improve their memory (altruistic goal).
  • Teacher was asked to read a couple of words to
    a student and than to test his memory. The
    teacher said the first word from a pair, the
    student was required to say the second word from
    the pair.. Incorrect answers were punished with
    an electrical shock.

20
  • Teacher controlled a shock generator with 30
    switches in units of 15 volts. The switches were
    labeled with increasing voltage designations,
    from 15 to 450 volts.
  • The switches also had descriptive labels. For
    example, slight shock (15-60 V), very strong
    shock (195-240 V), dangersevere shock
    (375-420 V). The 435 to 450 V range very simply
    labeled XXX.
  • Teacher had to punish the first recall error
    with a mild 15-volt shock and then increase the
    shock by 15 volts for each succeeding error.

21
  • Thestudent was middle-aged, mild-mannered, and
    likable man, He was escorted to a nearby room
    where he was strapped to a chair and hooked up to
    an electrode that transmits the electric shock.
  • The student, a bit worriedly, mentions that he
    has a slight heart condition. But the
    experimenter assures him that although the
    shocks may be painful, they cause no permanent
    tissue damage.

22
Milgrams study
23
Milgrams study
24
  • The subjects were aware that they were
    administering extremely high levels of shock, and
    that these could prove fatal.
  • They could hear the 'stooge' subject, who they
    had seen strapped into a chair, giving
    increasingly loud cries of pain at first and then
    becoming silent as if he had passed out or died.
  • They were extremely disturbed by what they were
    being asked to do, and frequently argued with the
    experimenter.
  • But, nonetheless, 65 of them gave electric
    shocks up to the maximum possible level. All of
    Milgram's subjects found it very difficult to
    disobey the experimenter by refusing to
    participate any further.

25
Explanation
  • Milgram attributed his findings directly to the
    power of social roles.
  • By entering into the experiment and adopting the
    role of participant, the subjects felt that they
    had entered into a social contract which involved
    their behaving obediently.
  • Their view of the contract was also that it
    involved the experimenter behaving responsibly,
    and many afterwards stated that they had gone
    along with it because it took place at a
    high-status university, Yale, where they assumed
    the experimenter would be a responsible
    scientist.

26
  • The original studies were conducted at Yale
    University
  • When they were repeated in downtown building
    indicated as Research Associates of Bridgeport
    the full obedience rate dropped from 63 to
    48.
  • When an ordinary man gave the orders, obedience
    fell to about 20 .
  • But when the research was conducted at Princeton
    University, obedience increased to 80.
  • Obedience decreased with the decrease of the
    authority of the experimenter or an institution.

27
Milgrams study
28
Milgrams study
  • The study was replicated in other countries.
  • Shanab and Kahya (1977) in Jordan. The level of
    obedience 80
  • Kilham and Mann (1974) in Australia. The level of
    obedience only 50.

29
Milgrams theory of obedience
  • Milgrams theory of obedience (1973)
  • Two modes of social consciousness
  • Autonomous state individuals behave on the
    basic of their own conscience, attitudes, norms.
    The majority of people is fair, gentle, and in
    aggressive.
  • Substitute state individuals think and act as
    substitutes of an other person.

30
Obedience in a work
  • Another study which shows the power of social
    roles and expected behaviour in a more real-life
    setting was performed by Hofling (1966) in a
    hospital.
  • The aim of the study was to see if nurses would
    comply with a doctor's instructions (which, of
    course, is a strong part of their expected role
    behaviour) even if it went against the hospital
    regulations.
  • While on duty, a nurse would receive a phone call
    from a doctor who claimed to be Dr. Smith from
    psychiatry, about a particular patient. The nurse
    would be asked to give the patient a particular
    drug called Astroten.

31
  • At first, they would be asked to go to the drug
    cabinet, check that the drug was there and to
    report back.
  • This gave the nurse a chance to see the bottle,
    which was clearly labelled 'maximum doseage 10
    mg'. Dr. Smith would then ask the nurse to
    administer 20 mg to the patient.
  • This request required the nurse concerned to
    contravene hospital regulations in two ways,
    firstly by administering a dose which was above
    the maximum considered safe, and secondly, by
    taking instructions from an unknown person, which
    was also forbidden.
  • Despite this, 21 out of 22 nurses involved in the
    study poured out the medication, and were
    prepared to administer it to the patient.

32
  • Social pressure elicited by inequality causes
    that a nurse prefers to endanger a patient than
    do not respect the order of a physician.

33
  • Milgram (1963) - experiments in the New York
    subway system.
  • He found that 50 of the people asked to give up
    their seat to another person with no explanation
    would comply.
  • It seems that for many people, refusing a direct
    request from someone is very difficult.

34
Bystander intervention
  • Some kind of situation arises in which we feel
    that we ought to help out a total stranger,
    either because they have had an accident or
    because they are being attacked. Although most
    people know what they ought o do in such
    situations, there have been several cases in
    which crimes or even murders have been committed
    in full view of the public, but no-one
    intervened.

35
  • The bystander apathy has been studied by many
    psychologists, in an attempt to identity the
    factors which bring about people's reluctance to
    intervene.
  • There would appear to be three main factors
  • the influence of other people's presence
  • the way that people comprehend what is going on
  • the effects of other people's example.

36
The presence of other people
  • The presence of other people is thought to have
    an effect because each person expects the next
    one to act or, in some cases, assumes that they
    already have.
  • In the case of a murder reported by Rosenthal
    (1964), of the 40 or so witnesses to the murder
    (it took place in front of a block of flats and
    went on for a considerable time) each assumed
    that someone else had called the police, which
    meant that in the end no body did.

37
  • Latane and Rodin (1969)
  • Subjects were seated in a room, with only a
    curtain separating them from another room in
    which a 'secretary' was working. They heard her
    climb on a chair to reach the bookcase shelf,
    fall, and cry for help, saying that her foot was
    trapped and that she was in pain.
  • 70 of the subjects who were waiting alone came
    to her rescue,
  • whereas only 40 of the subjects who were waiting
    in pairs came to help.
  • When they were waiting with other people, it
    seemed, the subjects considered the other person
    equally responsible for helping, and if the other
    person didn't help then they didn't either.

38
Diffusion of responsibility
  • This factor is usually called diffusion of
    responsibility, whereby the more people who are
    considered able to help, the less any particular
    individual feels responsible.

39
  • The way that people understand the situation
    seems to result also in different kinds of
    behaviur.
  • People tend to define situations for themselves
    and if a situation is defined as an emergency,
    they will be far more ready to help than if they
    have defined the situation to themselves as a
    non-emergency.
  • This factor also tends to work in terms of other
    people's responses. If someone appears to be very
    calm in a particular situation, we may assume
    that they have defined the situation as a non
    emergency.

40
  • Latane and Darley (1968) asked experimental
    subjects to sit in a waiting room, waiting for an
    interview.
  • As they sat there, a wall vent began to pour
    smoke into the room.
  • Subjects were observed through a one-way mirror
    to see how long it would be before they reported
    the smoke to someone else outside the room.
  • The subjects were tested either singly or in
    groups.

41
  • 75 of the single ones reported the smoke within
    two minutes of it starting.
  • When the subjects were in groups, less than 13
    of them reported the smoke at all, despite the
    fact that the little room filled up with it over
    a six minute period!
  • Those subjects said afterwards that they had not
    really thought of the smoke as indicating a fire
    but as some other harmless phenomenon, such as
    steam or smog.
  • It seems that the presence of other people, and
    their unwillingness to act, had meant that they
    all defined the situation as a non-emergency, and
    so acted accordingly.

42
How other people's examples may infIuence our
behaviour
  • Bryan and Test 1967.
  • They set up a motorway study, in which a 'model'
    scenario was acted out by the side of a motorway,
    and a bit further on drivers had the chance to
    imitate the 'model'.
  • Drivers passed a broken-down car, with a man
    repairing it by the roadside, and a woman
    standing watching.
  • A bit further on down the road, they came to the
    'test' car, which had a flat tyre, and a woman
    apparently unable to change it.

43
  • A control group of drivers saw the 'test'
    situation, but not the 'model' beforehand.
  • Out of 4,000 passing cars, 93 drivers who had
    seen the 'model' stopped to help, but only 58 of
    the 'control' group stopped.
  • Although this might not seem like much of a
    difference, it was one which was very unlikely to
    have happened by chance alone, and implies that
    seeing another person helping does affect how
    likely we are to help other people ourselves.
Write a Comment
User Comments (0)
About PowerShow.com