Teaching the Control of Variables Strategy in Fourth Grade Classrooms - PowerPoint PPT Presentation

1 / 1
About This Presentation
Title:

Teaching the Control of Variables Strategy in Fourth Grade Classrooms

Description:

Teaching the Control of Variables Strategy in Fourth Grade Classrooms Robert F. Lorch, Jr., William J. Calderhead, Emily E. Dunlap, Emily C. Hodell, Benjamin Dunham ... – PowerPoint PPT presentation

Number of Views:74
Avg rating:3.0/5.0
Slides: 2
Provided by: LisaRin8
Category:

less

Transcript and Presenter's Notes

Title: Teaching the Control of Variables Strategy in Fourth Grade Classrooms


1
Teaching the Control of Variables Strategy in
Fourth Grade Classrooms Robert F. Lorch, Jr.,
William J. Calderhead, Emily E. Dunlap, Emily C.
Hodell, Benjamin Dunham Freer, Elizabeth P.
Lorch University of Kentucky
  • METHOD
  • Participants
  • Participants included in analyses were 543
    children from 36, 4th grade classrooms. Students
    were from high achieving (n 269) and low
    achieving schools (n 274) in the Fayette County
    school district, Lexington, KY.
  • Design
  • The experimental design included three factors
  • Students attended schools that were either high
    or low scoring on a statewide test of science
    achievement (Kentucky Core Content Test).
  • Classrooms were assigned at random to one of
    three instructional interventions (a) CVS
    instruction (Instruct), (b) hands-on manipulation
    (Manipulate), or (c) CVS instruction hands-on
    manipulation (Both).
  • Students were tested at three points (a) the day
    before instruction (i.e., Pretest), (b) the day
    after instruction (Posttest), and (c) 4-5 months
    after instruction (Delayed Test).
  • Materials
  • Comparison of Figures 2 and 3 shows several
    similarities between the performances of
  • students in the higher- vs. lower-achieving
    schools
  • Students in both environments improve from
    pretest to posttest
  • Students in both environments maintain their
    gains at the delayed test.
  • The pattern of relative performance as a function
    of instructional condition is the same for
    students in both environments.
  • However, comparison of Figures 2 and 3 also shows
    some differences between the
  • performances of students in the higher- vs.
    lower-achieving schools
  • The same instruction produced greater gains in
    the higher-achieving schools than in the
    lower-achieving schools.
  • The Manipulate condition produced significant
    learning only in the higher-achieving schools.
  • The addition of hands-on experience to the direct
    instruction produced significant gains relative
    to direct instruction alone for the
    higher-achieving schools, but not for the
    lower-achieving schools (compare Both vs.
    Instruct in each graph).
  • CONCLUSIONS
  • The teaching intervention developed by Klahr and
    his associates was successfully translated into
    4th-grade classrooms on a relatively large scale.
    Instruction in the Both condition resulted in
    substantial improvement from the pretest to the
    posttest. Further, the gains were maintained
    several months later on the delayed test.
  • RESULTS
  • Figure 1 displays mean performance on the paper
    pencil tests of CVS understanding.
  • The data are averaged across high- and
    low-achieving schools.
  • ABSTRACT
  • We investigated an intervention (Chen Klahr,
    1999) that was designed to teach the control of
    variables strategy (CVS) to young students.
    Four questions were addressed by the study (1)
    Can the teaching intervention be successfully
    translated to the classroom? (2) Is the
    intervention effective with students from
    lower-achieving schools, as well as
    higher-achieving schools? (3) Is direct
    instruction sufficient for teaching CVS in
    4th-grade classrooms? (4) Does hands-on
    experience conducting experiments contribute to
    learning CVS beyond the gains produced by direct
    instruction in CVS? Students from high- and
    low-achieving schools were taught CVS by direct
    instruction, or hands-on experimentation, or
    both. The results demonstrate that the basic
    intervention can be successfully translated to
    the classroom, but it is relatively less
    effective in lower-achieving classrooms.
    Further, direct instruction alone was effective
    in teaching CVS, but it was more effective when
    combined with hands-on manipulation. Hands-on
    manipulation unaccompanied by direct instruction
    was relatively ineffective.
  • INTRODUCTION
  • The experimental method is based on the logic
    that in order to demonstrate a causal
    relationship between two variables, the
    experimenter must manipulate the independent
    variable and find corresponding variation in the
    dependent variable while holding constant all
    other variables. Chen and Klahr (1999) developed
    a brief instructional intervention that they
    showed to be very effective at teaching CVS to
    4th-grade students . Klahr and his associates
    (Klahr Nigram, 2004 Toth, Klahr Chen, 2000)
    have successfully applied the teaching
    intervention in the classroom, albeit on a
    limited basis. The current study scales up the
    intervention by both applying the intervention in
    many 4th-grade classrooms (i.e., 36) and by
    testing its effectiveness in two, very distinct
    environments (i.e., high-achieving classrooms vs.
    lower-achieving classrooms).
  • In addition to examining whether Chen and
    Klahrs teaching intervention can be scaled up,
    the study addressed basic theoretical questions
    about the conditions necessary for successful
    teaching of CVS. The teaching intervention is
    comprised of both direct instruction in the logic
    of CVS, and a discovery component involving
    hands-on manipulation of the experimental
    apparatus. Klahr and associates have already
    demonstrated that relatively little learning
    results if students conduct hands-on manipulation
    in the absence of any direct instruction. Based
    on their results, they have argued that direct
    instruction is a necessary component of teaching
    of CVS. However, they have not demonstrated that
    direct instruction is sufficient for learning of
    CVS, nor have they shown that the hands-on
    component of their intervention produces no
    benefits beyond the gains of direct instruction.
  • Our study addresses four questions concerning
    the basic teaching intervention first developed
    by Chen and Klahr
  • Can we successfully translate the basic teaching
    intervention into 4th-grade classrooms on a
    relatively large scale (i.e., more than 1 or 2
    classrooms)?
  • Will the intervention be successful in both high-
    and low-achieving classrooms?


Write a Comment
User Comments (0)
About PowerShow.com