Sex Differences in Intelligence - PowerPoint PPT Presentation

1 / 50
About This Presentation
Title:

Sex Differences in Intelligence

Description:

Mackintosh (1998) in an analysis of the research literature on sex differences ... Mackintosh argued that the sex differences are, generally, small, amounting to ... – PowerPoint PPT presentation

Number of Views:518
Avg rating:3.0/5.0
Slides: 51
Provided by: joh9158
Category:

less

Transcript and Presenter's Notes

Title: Sex Differences in Intelligence


1
Sex Differences in Intelligence
2
Key Themes
  • Sex differences in intelligence
  • General intelligence
  • Spatial intelligence

3
Learning Outcomes
  • Outline the extent of sex differences between men
    and women on general intelligence and specific
    aspects of intelligence
  • Describe possible biological and environmental
    variables and considerations that may explain sex
    differences in intelligence

4
Sex Differences in Intelligence
  • Sex differences on measures of general
    intelligence
  • Sex differences in specific intelligences.

5
Sex differences on measures of general
intelligence
  • In the first instance we are going to look
    whether men and women score differently on
    general measures of intelligence, such as IQ
    scores, or scores on intelligence tests such as
    the Weschlers tests and the Ravens Progressive
    Matrices.

6
Sex differences on measures of general
intelligence
  • Louis Terman (1916) was the academic who decided
    to use the Binet-Simon test among Californian
    school children, and, consequently, developed the
    Stanford-Binet test. Terman reported that when
    comparing nearly 1,000 4- to 16 year olds, girls
    had a slightly higher score than boys for overall
    IQ scores on the Stanford-Binet test.
  • Charles Spearman (1921), the English
    psychologist, who introduced the way of
    conceptualising intelligence as a single factor,
    g, argued that there were no sex differences in
    intelligence.
  • Raymond B. Cattell, who theorised about the
    differences between fluid intelligence and
    crystallised intelligence, suggested that there
    were a number of studies that show no significant
    differences between men and women on these two
    dimensions of intelligence (of all ages including
    children).

7
Sex differences on measures of general
intelligence
  • A first systematic review of evidence on sex
    differences in intelligence was presented by J.H.
    Court (Court, 1983).
  • In his review, Court reviewed nearly 120 studies
    that had provided information on sex differences
    on the Ravens Progressive Matrices. He found
    that some studies suggested that women scored
    higher than men in other studies men scored
    higher than women, and in the majority of studies
    there was no difference.
  • Mackintosh (1998) in an analysis of the research
    literature on sex differences on the Ravens
    Progressive Matrices. Mackintosh argued that the
    sex differences are, generally, small, amounting
    to no more than 12 IQ points in favour of males
    or females.
  • Anderson (2004) reviewed the literature and
    concluded that on the Weschler and the Ravens
    tests, no significant differences occurred
    between males and females.

8
However..
  • Richard Lynn and Paul Irwing, over the course of
    two studies (Lynn Irwing, 2004 2005), argued
    that there had been no actual statistical
    meta-analysis of sex differences in general
    intelligence.
  • They pointed out that reports, such as that made
    by Court, are known as a narrative analysis.
  • Narrative analysis is where the researcher weighs
    up the evidence presented across a number of
    studies, but the size of samples of these studies
    vary, can be taken from a number of different
    samples, and results are only analysed in terms
    of whether the study supports, or doesnt support
    the main hypothesis
  • Sample sizes used by Court were relatively small
    (i.e. much less that n 500).

9
Lynn and Irwing
  • To remedy this situation, Lynn and Irwing set
    about collecting data on sex differences for
    scores on the Ravens Progressive Matrices. In
    all, Lynn and Irwing collected the data for large
    population samples (i.e. greater than n 500)
    from 1939 to 2002.
  • In all, Lynn and Irwing collected data from 57
    studies from 30 countries covering 195 samples,
    numbering a total excess of 80,000 people.

10
Sex Differences in General Intelligence
  • Reporting of Effect Size is evident in this
    area..Comparing differences you get d..
  • d .2 is a small effect size,
  • d .5 is a medium effect size and
  • d .8 is a large effect size.

11
Effect Size of Lynn and Irwings Findings
Regarding Sex Differences in Intelligence
Table 14.1 Effect size of Lynn and Irwings
findings regarding sex differences in intelligence
12
Sex differences in specific intelligences
  • In what is considered as a seminal study of the
    literature on sex differences, Maccoby and
    Jacklin (1974) concluded that men, on average,
    perform better on tests of spatial ability than
    women.
  • Spatial ability is the ability to visualise
    spatial relationships and to mentally manipulate
    objects. It is a mental process, which is
    associated with the brains attempts to interpret
    accurately incoming information.
  • On the other hand, they argued that women, on
    average, do better on verbal abilities, such as
    comprehension (The act of understanding the
    meaning, nature, or importance, of things) and
    language vocabulary (a knowledge of words in a
    particularly language)

13
Stop and think. What is spatial ability?
  • a. Spatial perception requires participants to
    identify the horizontal or the vertical object
    (usually a line) in a display while ignoring
    distracting information. One example of this task
    is the Piaget water level task, which requires
    participants to draw in the water level in a
    picture of a tilted glass that is half filled
    with water (see below).

14
Stop and think. What is spatial ability?
(Continued)
  • b. Spatial visualisation refers to analysing
    spatial information. Examples of tests that tap
    spatial visualisation are the embedded figures
    test (see Figure below), in which a target figure
    is hidden in the contours of a larger figure
    and the participant is expected to find it.
    Another example is a paper-folding task, in which
    participants have to imagine what the result is
    of folding a piece of paper in several ways.

15
Stop and think. What is spatial ability?
(Continued)
  • c. Mental rotation involves being able to
    visualise objects from different angles and
    different positions. Examples of this sort of
    tests include tasks where participants are
    presented pairs of drawings and asked people
    whether the objects were the same, only rotated,
    or mirror images of each other (see Figure below).

16
Stop and think. What is spatial ability?
(Continued)
  • d. Spatiotemporal ability involves the
    participants making judgments about moving visual
    stimuli. Often responses are asked to guess when
    certain objects will arrive at a certain point
    (see Figure below).

17
Stop and think. What is spatial ability?
(Continued)
  • Mechanical reasoning measures are the ability to
    understand basic mechanical principles of
    machinery, tools, and motion. These tasks
    comprise principles that involve reasoning rather
    than specialised knowledge or training. The task
    illustrated below is an example of mechanical
    reasoning. Participants would be asked to
    indicate which direction vehicle A would go if it
    collided with vehicle B at point X.

18
Summary of the Findings From FiveMeta-Analyses
Examining Sex Differences on Specific
Intelligences
Table 14.2 Summary of the findings from five
meta-analyses examining sex differences on
specific intelligences
19
Summary of the Findings From FiveMeta-Analyses
Examining Sex Differences on Specific
Intelligences (Continued)
Table 14.2 Summary of the findings from five
meta-analyses examining sex differences on
specific intelligences (Continued)
20
Sex differences in specific intelligence
  • In terms of men scoring higher on spatial
    abilities, we can see that across all ages, men
    score higher on measures of spatial perception,
    though it is a small to medium effect size.
    There is a larger effect size (medium to large)
    for mental rotation. Among adolescents, the
    greatest effect for men scoring higher than women
    is for mechanical reasoning (See Figure 14.3 if
    you need to be reminded of what these
    intelligence tasks involve).
  • In terms of women scoring higher than men in
    verbal ability, the only effect size, of above
    .2, is for verbal production (i.e. 0.33).
    However, among adolescents, there is evidence for
    girls doing better than boys (effect size gt .2)
    on verbal abilities for spelling, language, and
    perceptual speed.

21
Sex differences in specific intelligence Summary
  • There is a small effect for men over women on
    measures of general intelligence (IQ scores)
    (based on Lynn and Irwings findings)
  • There is a medium effect for men over women for
    spatial intelligence
  • There is no evidence for a general effect of
    women over men for verbal intelligence abilities

22
Looking for explanations for sex differences in
measures of intelligence
  • Biological explanations for sex differences in
    measures of intelligence (IQ scores).
  • Environmental explanations for sex differences in
    measures of intelligence.
  • Going a different route to Chapter 13 which talks
    about problems with biological explanations and
    differences..

23
Biological Explanations for Sex Differences in
Intelligence
  • The effect of brain size and maturity rates on
    general intelligence
  • Biological variables for sex differences in
    spatial intelligence
  • Evolutionary perspectives and sex differences in
    spatial ability
  • Brain functioning and sex differences in spatial
    intelligence
  • Testosterone and sex differences in spatial
    intelligence.

24
Biological variables for sex differences in
measures of general intelligence
  • Lynns theory of the effects of brain size and
    maturity rates on general intelligence seeks to
    explain why there are differences in measures of
    general intelligence (IQ scores) between men and
    women in adulthood, but not in adolescence.
  • Willerman et al. (1991) examined brain size and
    IQ scores among 40 U.S.A. university students and
    reported a correlation of r .35. While Raz et
    al. (1993) reported a correlation of r .43
    between brain size and IQ scores among 29 adults
    aged between 18 and 78 years of age. In a
    meta-analysis of 37 samples across 1530 people
    (McDaniel, 2005), U.S.A. psychologist Michael
    McDaniel estimated the correlation between brain
    size and IQ was 0.33. The correlation was higher
    for females than males and higher for adults than
    children. However, across both sexes and all
    ages, it was clear that brain volume is
    positively correlated with intelligence.
  • Lynn (1994) points out that, on average, men have
    10 larger brains than women by about 10 percent,
    and larger brains suggest larger brain power,
    this explains the difference between men and
    women on measures of general intelligence (IQ
    scores).

25
Biological variables for sex differences in
measures of general intelligence
  • However, why no differences among adolescents?
    Well, Lynn (1994) has proposed a developmental
    theory of sex differences in intelligence, as
    boys and girls mature physically and mentally at
    different rates
  • Lynn suggests boys and girls mature at the same
    rate up to the age of seven years. However,
    during crucial times during development girls
    mature faster than boys. This growth spurt starts
    at 8 years and slows down at fourteen and fifteen
    years. Therefore, girls brain size may be
    similar to boys around the start of adolescence
    at twelve to thirteen years and this leads to
    girls evening up with boys in intelligence scores
    in adolescents. Then, at the age of sixteen
    onwards into adulthood, while growth rates
    continue, boys start to develop larger average
    brain sizes and the differences in general
    intelligence, that last into adulthood, start to
    develop.

26
Summary of Effect Sizes of Sex Differences on
Intelligence Across the Age Range
Table 14.3 Summary of effect sizes of sex
differences on intelligence from Lynn and Irwing
across the age range Source Lynn and Irwin (2004)
27
Evolutionary perspectives and sex differences in
spatial ability
  • Jones, Braithwaite Healy (2003) summarised
    three evolutionary hypotheses that might explain
    why men might be better at spatial abilities
    Foraging, Range Size and Warfare.

28
Evolutionary perspectives and sex differences in
spatial ability
  • Foraging (see also Silverman Eals, 1992).
    Foraging is the act of looking or searching for
    food or provisions. Men took the skills such as
    finding their way around the countryside,
    demonstrating awareness of the physical
    environment to be able to hunt and intercept
    animals, and throwing weapons to kill animals,
    all require spatial abilities.
  • Range (see also Gray Buffery, 1971 Gaulin,
    1995). This hypothesis focuses on the view of
    mating roles in evolutionary theory, and the
    theory that while females seek out monogamous
    relationships (single relationships), males seek
    out multiple relationships (polygynous). To
    pursue and maintain multiple relationships,
    polygynous males need to cover a larger area of
    land and therefore has increased range size.
  • Warfare (see also Geary, 1995 Sherry Hampson,
    1997). To some extent this theory is a
    combination of the first two points. Sex
    differences in human spatial ability as a result
    of direct male-to-male small-scale warfare. Men
    travel long distances in order to ambush other
    men, compete for resources and also compete for
    females. In fighting men develop their spatial
    abilities.

29
Brain Functioning and sex differences in spatial
intelligence
  • Richard J. Haier, Professor of Psychology in the
    Department of Pediatrics at the University of
    California, presented findings suggesting men and
    women achieve intelligence through different
    areas of the brain (Haier, Jung, Yeo, et al.
    2005).
  • They used MRI (Magnetic Resonance Imaging) to
    find out where intelligence was in the brain. An
    MRI scan is able to provide clear pictures of
    parts of the body that are surrounded by bone
    tissue, so the technique is useful when examining
    the brain and spinal cord. They used a particular
    technique called voxel-based morphometry that
    looks for grey and white matter around the brain.
    Grey matter can be understood as the parts of
    the brain responsible for information processing,
    whereas, white matter is responsible for
    information transmission.
  • Participants were given the Wechsler Adult
    Intelligence Scale battery and underwent the MRI
    scan. The analysis (voxel-based morphometry) was
    used to examine whether IQ scores were related to
    brain areas where grey matter and white matter
    occurred. Haier et al. found two interesting
    findings.

30
Summary of Haier et al. (2005) FindingUsing
Brodmann Definitions of Areas of the Brain
Figure 14.1 Summary of the Haier et al. (2005)
finding using Brodmanns definitions of areas of
the brain Source Haier et al. (2005)
31
Brain Functioning and sex differences in spatial
intelligence
  • The second finding was that women have more white
    matter and fewer grey matter areas related to IQ
    scores compared to men. This finding suggests
    that intelligence is related to white matter in
    women, and grey matter in men. Haiers et al.s
    findings suggest that, among men, IQ score is
    related to those aspects of the brain responsible
    for information processing (grey matter)
    whereas, for women, intelligence is generally
    related to those aspects responsible for
    information transmission (white matter). This
    finding suggests that intelligence in men is
    related to information processing. Consequently,
    this might explain mens superior abilities in
    spatial intelligence.

32
Testosterone and sex differences in spatial
intelligence
  • Hormones are the substances that travel around
    the human body to effect physiological activity,
    such as growth or metabolism.
  • Gonadal hormones are those substances that create
    physiological growth in organs in animals that
    produce sex cells, e.g. ovaries in females (part
    of the female reproductive system) and testes in
    the male (part of the male reproductive system).
  • Testosterone (a gonadal hormone) is the male sex
    hormone. Testosterone is necessary for the
    individual in the fetus, for the development of
    male genitalia. Additionally, increased levels of
    testosterone at puberty are responsible for
    further growth of male genitalia and for the
    development and maintenance of what are known as
    male secondary sex characteristics, such as voice
    changes and facial hair.
  • Estradiol (also a gonadal hormone) is the most
    potent naturally occurring oestrogen. Oestrogen
    is produced chiefly by the ovaries and
    responsible for promoting oestrus and the
    development and maintenance of female secondary
    sex characteristics, such as pubic hair and
    breasts.
  • Males and females have both hormones, but greater
    concentrates of testosterone are found in men and
    greater concentrates of estradiol are found in
    women.

33
Testosterone and sex differences in spatial
intelligence
  • Choi and Silverman (2002) starts from a general
    finding in the literature, that male and female
    humans and non-humans use different strategies to
    learn routes.
  • Routes through the environment require
    individuals to use spatial ability. However,
    there is a sex difference in the way men and
    women use routes (Choi Silverman, 1996).
  • Women tend to use relative directions (left,
    right, front, and back) and landmarks (buildings,
    bridges, traffic lights etc) to learn routes (for
    example, you turn left at the bridge and then
    right at the next traffic lights).
  • Men use distance (mileage, metres) and cardinal
    (the four principal compass points north, south,
    east, and west) directions (for example, You
    head down there for about 100 metres and then
    head west until you get to the next turning 50
    metres down that road, and then head north).

34
Testosterone and sex differences in spatial
intelligence
  • The authors gave 46 male and 60 female
    undergraduate students a route-learning task,
    which comprised of a map that depicted a
    fictional town, with various landmarks and
    streets, and a compass indicator and distance
    scale.
  • Participants were asked to learn the shortest
    route from one point to another on the map in 2
    minutes, and then were asked to recall the route
    back to the experimenter.
  • The experimenter then coded the number of times
    the participants made a reference in their route
    to landmarks, relative direction, cardinal
    directions and distance.
  • Respondents testosterone and estradiol levels
    were measured by taking some saliva from the
    participants.

35
Testosterone and sex differences in spatial
intelligence
  • The authors gave 46 male and 60 female
    undergraduate students a route-learning task,
    which comprised of a map that depicted a
    fictional town, with various landmarks and
    streets, and a compass indicator and distance
    scale.
  • Participants were asked to learn the shortest
    route from one point to another on the map in 2
    minutes, and then were asked to recall the route
    back to the experimenter.
  • The experimenter then coded the number of times
    the participants made a reference in their route
    to landmarks, relative direction, cardinal
    directions and distance.
  • Respondents testosterone and estradiol levels
    were measured by taking some saliva from the
    participants.

36
Testosterone and sex differences in spatial
intelligence
  • Choi and Silverman found that though estradiol
    was not related to strategies for route learning.
    However, they found among men, but not women,
    that the testosterone level was positively
    related to the use of male-biased route-learning
    strategies (the use of distance and cardinal
    rules).

37
Environmental Explanations for Sex Differences in
Intelligence
  • Schemas
  • Influence of stereotypes on sex differences in
    intelligence outside of education
  • Influence of stereotypes on sex differences in
    intelligence within education
  • Interactions between technology and
    socio-economic status and their influence on
    intelligence in the classroom.

38
An Illustration of the Three Aspects to Consider
When Considering Environmental Influences on Sex
Differences in (spatial) Intelligence
Figure 14.2 An illustration of the three aspects
to consider when considering environmental
influences on sex differences in (spatial)
intelligence
39
Stereotypes on sex differences in intelligence
outside of education
  • Susan Levine (Levine, Vasilyeva, Lourenco,
    Newcombe Huttenlocher, 2005) suggests that,
    although not a lot is known about what type of
    toys can be used to increase spatial skills,
    playing with building blocks, puzzles and video
    games are related with spatial skills, and boys
    spend more time with these toys than girls.
  • Evidence among infants suggests that there are
    differences in the toy choices of boys and girls,
    which may affect their intelligence, particularly
    spatial intelligence. Anne Campbell and her
    colleagues (Campbell, Shirley, Heywood Crook,
    2000) found that sex differences in preferences
    for toys started to appear as young as 9 months.
    Girls were found to want to play with dolls,
    while boys preferred trucks.

40
Stereotypes on sex differences in intelligence
within education
  • Subject Choice
  • Halpern and LaMay (2000) suggest that while men
    and women nowadays reach similar levels of
    intelligence and achievement at school, their
    choice of subjects at school tend to follow a
    pattern based on their sex. At school, there is
    a large distinction in sex choices in science
    subjects (and to some extent aspects of
    mathematics that involve spatial reasoning). This
    can comprise positive subject choice, as in
    selecting particular subjects, or negative
    subject choice, as in avoiding particular
    subjects.

41
Stereotypes on sex differences in intelligence
within education
  • Subject Choice Plucker (1996) notes that one key
    idea on how sex differences may occur in
    intelligence is that self-fulfilling prophecies
    occur in the classroom. This is where positive or
    negative feedback influences a pupils ideas
    about their own abilities, and despite their real
    potential regarding a particular ability, their
    own ideas determine their attainment in that
    ability.
  • Stereotype threat (Steele, 1997). Stereotype
    threat has been used to explain why certain
    groups may perform poorly in test situations,
    particularly in tests of intelligence .

42
Interactions between technology and
socio-economic status and their influence on
intelligence in the classroom
  • Sex differences in spatial abilities may become
    more apparent as teaching relies more on learning
    techniques that use visual and technical
    environments (e.g. computers and the internet).
  • Kirby Boulter(1999) compared two groups of
    students on a new spatial ability task. One group
    was given the task via pen and paper, and the
    other was given the task via visual stimuli.
    Kirby and Boulter found that one of the factors
    of predicting success at the visual stimuli task
    was spatial ability before the task. Kirby and
    Boulter suggest that people who are weak at
    spatial ability before the task may be
    disadvantaged by the use of complex visual
    stimuli used as a teaching method, and this will
    further exacerbate their problems with spatial
    ability.

43
Interactions between technology and
socio-economic status and their influence on
intelligence in the classroom
  • Levine, Vasilyeva, Lourenco, Newcombe
    Huttenlocher, 2005). In a longitudinal study,
    children were administered two spatial tasks
    requiring mental transformations and a task of
    verbal comprehension. As with previous studies
    mentioned above, no sex difference was found
    between boys and girls in their verbal ability.
  • However, on the two tasks, boys from the middle
    and high-socio-economic class did better than
    girls on both spatial tasks, whereas boys and
    girls from the low-socio-economic group did not
    differ in their performance on the spatial task.
    It is important to note that the individuals from
    the low socio-economic classes didnt find the
    test any harder.
  • Susan Levine suggests that the main reason for
    this finding is that children from lower
    social-economic backgrounds spend less amounts of
    time engaged in activities that promote spatial
    intelligence outside the classroom and this gives
    them disadvantages in the modern-day classroom
    that emphasises the use of spatial abilities

44
A final consideration of sex differences in
measures of intelligence.
  • Stereotype emphasis
  • Placing the extent of the sex differences in
    intelligence within its proper context.

45
Stereotype emphasis
  • Over-emphasised stereotypes (i.e. the estimated
    size for the sex difference for the variable was
    greater than the real difference between men and
    women on that variable). This might provide
    evidence that stereotypes might be overly
    attributed to difference between the sexes. For
    example, Swim found that we over-emphasised how
    aggressive men are.
  • Under-emphasised stereotypes (i.e. the estimated
    size for the sex difference for the variable was
    smaller than the real difference between men and
    women on that variable). This may provide
    evidence that real sex differences are being
    underestimated in our stereotypes. For example,
    Swim found we might under-emphasise mens helping
    behaviour in an emergency.
  • Accurate stereotypes (i.e. the estimated size for
    the sex difference for the variable is the same
    as the real difference between men and women on
    that variable). For example, we may be accurate
    in terms of viewing men as more restless.

46
Stereotype emphasis
  • In terms of intelligence, Swims (1995) study
    suggested that we are accurate in our perceptions
    of men being better in their mathematical
    abilities, but subjects tended to overestimate
    women's verbal abilities (a finding that is
    consistent with our earlier discussion regarding
    research that suggests that there are no sex
    differences in verbal abilities).

47
Placing the extent of the sex differences in
intelligence within its proper context
  • Yoder Kahn, 2003) have suggested that there are
    two ways in which sex differences are treated in
    the literature, the first is an alpha bias, in
    which there is assumed to be large differences,
    and beta bias, in which it is assumed that there
    a minimal differences (also known as the gender
    similarity hypothesis).
  • Though research on sex differences in
    intelligence does tend to speculate on reasons
    for differences, we must consider to what extent
    sex differences in intelligence are important.
    Two points are important to such a consideration

48
Placing the extent of the sex differences in
intelligence within its proper context
  • There is a small effect size of around .3 for
    general intelligence scores, and a medium to high
    effect size of between .5 and .7 for two aspects
    of spatial ability (mental rotation and
    mechanical reasoning among adolescents).
  • However, U.S.A. psychologist Janet Shibley Hyde
    (Hyde, 2005) notes that when the ranges of these
    effect sizes for sex differences in intelligence
    scores are compared against the effect sizes for
    sex differences for other variables, their
    importance diminishes.
  • For example, the effect sizes of the majority of
    aggression variables range from .33 to .84, the
    majority of sexuality variables range from .30 to
    .96, personality variables (particularly
    extraversion and agreeableness) range from .35 to
    .91. Within this context, the sex differences
    between men and women seem a lot less important.

49
Two Comparison Lines of Difference. Comparingthe
Range of Variability in IQ scores within
Population Groups of Men and Women with Average
Difference Between Men and Women (drawn to scale)
Figure 14.3 Two comparison lines of difference.
Comparing the range of variability in IQ scores
within population groups of men and women with
average difference between men and women (drawn
to scale)
50
Summary
  • Outline the extent of sex differences between men
    and women on general intelligence and specific
    aspects of intelligence
  • Describe possible biological and environmental
    variables and considerations that may explain sex
    differences in intelligence
Write a Comment
User Comments (0)
About PowerShow.com