Title: Growing up Bilingual: One System or Two
1Growing up BilingualOne System or Two?
- Language differentiation and speech perception in
infancy
2Extending the models to Phonology
- ULS (Unitary Language System)
- A common, undifferentiated storage system for all
languages (Volterra Taeschner) - In Phonology, Flege (1987) suggests a single
system with category boundaries influenced by
each language - DLS (Differentiated Language Systems)
- Language systems differentiated from the
beginning (Genessee) - In phonology, Grosjean (1997). One phonology for
each language. - One language dominant
- Research with adult bilinguals showing faster
phonetic perception of L1 contrasts
(Sebastian-Galles, et. al).
3Organization of the Lecture
- Examining the Language Differentiation hypothesis
with research on language discrimination - Examining the Language Differentiation hypothesis
with research on phonetic perception
4 Language Discrimination
- At birth, monolingual infants prefer native over
non-native continuous speech (Moon, et. al, 1994)
can discriminate the filtered speech of
rhythmically distinct languages (Mehler, et .al.,
1988 Nazzi, et. al., 1998) - By 4-5 months they not only discriminate
rhythmically distinct languages, but can
discriminate their own language from rhythmically
similar languages, including dialectal variations
of the same language (Nazzi, et. al., 2000
Ramus, et. al., 2000) -
5Extending to Bilinguals
- Bosch Sebastian-Galles (1997) tested
Monolingual Spanish and Monolingual Catalan
infants on their ability to discriminate their
native language from the unfamiliar one - Interesting because both from the same rhythmical
class - Still some differences (more vowel reduction in
Catalan)
6The Orientation Latency Procedure
Picture of a womans face over each loud speaker.
7Monolingual 4-month infants orient faster to
native language
Bosch Sebastian-Galles, Cognition, 1997
8Bilingual infants orient more SLOWLY to one of
their native languages over an unfamiliar language
9Bilingual infants show no difference in
orientation latency to either of their languages
10Summary
- Infants of 4 months can discriminate their
language from a rhythmically similar language,
and show this by orienting faster to the native
language - Spanish-Catalan bilingual infants aged 4-5 months
discriminate both of their native languages from
an unfamiliar language. - But, they show this discrimination of native vs.
unfamiliar language by responding slower to the
native language, the opposite pattern as
monoliguals - This suggests different organization for speech
processing even at this early age - No evidence in the orientation latency procedure
that bilinguals can discriminate their two
languages
11Implications
- On the basis of the Bosch Sebastian-Galles 1997
work, would suggest that bilinguals might be
listening to language differently from
monolinguals - But no evidence of differentiation of their two
language
12Can Bilingual Infants discriminate their two
languages?
- Lack of discrimination of the two languages,
particularly when they are discriminable by
monolinguals, is supportive of the ULS - B SG decided to use a more appropriate task to
see if bilinguals can discriminate - Used the HTPP, with a familiarization phase
13Procedure Famliarization Phase
- First familiarized infants to sentences from one
of their languages - Present a flashing light at the centre
- Once the infant looked, the image disappeared,
and a slightly different image appeared on the R
or L screen - After the infant looked toward that, sentence
presentation began and lasted 28 sec or until the
infant looked away - Required the infants to accumulate 2 minutes of
sustained attention to the sentences (1 min to
each)
14Procedure Test Phase
- New sentences
- Half in familiar language, half in the other
language - Same procedure as before
- Did the infants choose to look/listen longer to
the sentences in the new language?
15Sequence of Experiments
- 1st wanted to make sure the results from the
Orientation Latency procedure would replicate
with the Familairization/Switch task - So first tested monolingual Spanish and Catalan
infants on their ability to discriminate their
native language from the other
16Familiarization/Switch Monolingual 4-monthers
discriminate Spanish vs. Catalan
Bosch, et. al., 2001
17Control Study
- Worried that recovery in Study 1 could have been
due to the fact that the sentences were new
(although seems that was controlled for in the
last experiment as well) - And to ensure that there wasnt just spontaneous
recovery - Tested infants with only materials from one
language - Familiarized to one set of sentences from one
language, and then tested them with a new set
from the same language
18Control Experiment rules out simple recovery to
new sentences
19Can Bilinguals discriminate their two languages?
- Finally ready to conduct the key experiment
- Familiarized bilinguals to sentences in one
language - Tested on new sentences from that language vs.
new sentences from the other language
20Bilingual 4 monthers DO discriminate their two
languages!
21Implications
- These results provide very strong evidence for
the DLS - If bilingual infants as young as 2 months can
discriminate their two languages, hard to argue
for a ULS - How about even earlier?
22ProcedureHigh Amplitude Sucking (HAS)Werker
Burns, in prep.
23Design
- Used alternating (Cowan) version of HAS
procedure, but as a test of preference - Baseline minute to set HA suck value
- Presentation of speech contingent on HA suck
- Collected 10 minutes of sucking, 5 minutes for
Tagalog and 5 minutes for English, with the
languages alternating by minute - Order counterbalanced
- Preference DV Do infants select to listen, i.e.
deliver more HA sucks to Tagalog or to English?
24(No Transcript)
25Preliminary Conclusions Tag/Eng Preference Study
- English infants do show a preference for English
over Tagalog - BFLA Tag/Eng newborns do NOT prefer one of their
languages over the other - Instead, they choose to listen equally to both
Tagalog and English, and do so significantly more
than the English babies listen to Tagalog - This suggests that both languages are equally
dominant at birth in the BFLA infant
26Unanswered questions future research
- Do BFLA infants differentiate their two
languages, or is there initially one
undifferentiated language space? - To test, need to see if BFLA newborns can
discriminate their two first languages - Do BFLA infants encode enough detail about
language to prefer a familiar language over an
unfamliar one? - To test, need to see if BFLA newborns will show a
preference for one of their native languages over
an unfamiliar language
27Part IIConsonant Discrimination in BFLA Infants
(with Tracey Burns)
- In previous work we have shown there to be a
reorganization in consonant discrimination in the
first year of life - Look at voicing, in a BFLA population and compare
to French-only and English-only groups
28Age changes in non-native discrimination(Werker
Tees, 1984)
29Vowel Perception in Bilinguals
- Bosch Sebastian-Galles (2003) tested Catalan
and Spanish-Catalan bilingual infants on their
ability to discriminate the Catalan only /e/-/E/
contrast - At 4 months, both groups discriminated
- At 8 months, only the Catalan infants did
- By 12 months, both groups discriminated again
- Does the data at 8 months indicate confusion
(ULS) or language dominance? - DLS by 12 months
30Extensions to consonants
- Test monolingual and bilingual infants on their
ability to discriminate a b-p difference that is
instantiated differently in each of their
languages - Do they confuse all three sounds? (ULS)
- Do they choose the category distinction for one
of their languages (language dominance) - Or do they use all three categories and maintain
both boundaries (DLS)
31VOT in French English
- English has two categories of VOT, short lag, and
long lag with aspiration - French, also has two categories of VOT, but they
are long lead and short lag - -------1------------------1------------------1----
----- Fr. /ba/ Fr./pa/-Eng /ba/
Eng /pa/
32Design
- -------1------------------1------------------1----
----- Fr. /ba/ Fr./pa/-Eng /ba/
Eng /pa/ - Habit infants to middle stimulus, test on both
others with order counterbalanced - Test English, French, and Fren/Eng BFLA infants
33Procedure
- Language exposure assessed using an adapted
version of the questionnaire developed by Bosch
and Sebastián-Gallés (1997) - Infants tested in a visual habituation procedure
- Auditory stimuli presented with a checkboard
display - Pre and post tests
34Visual Habituation Procedure
35Results 6-8 Month Olds
Looking Time (seconds)
36Results 68 Month Olds
- Both groups of infants dishabituate to ba but
not significantly to pha - 6 8 month olds show the same pattern of
response regardless of home language environment
37Experiment 2
- Stimuli
- same as Experiment 1
- Procedure
- same as Experiment 1
- Participants
- 1012 month old infants being raised in
monolingual English or bilingual English/French
households
38Results 10-12 Month Olds
Looking Time (seconds)
39Results 1012 Month Olds
- Infants being raised in English speaking homes
dishabituate to pha but not to ba - This is the pattern expected given their native
boundary and previous research (Werker and Tees,
1984) - Infants being raised in English/French homes do
not show a significant change in looking times to
either of the two test stimuli - Data are not uniform and suggest within-group
differences - These results suggest that the timecourse, and
possibly nature, of bilingual phonetic
representation is distinctly different than that
of monolinguals
40Experiment 3
- Stimuli
- same
- Procedure
- same
- Participants
- 14-21 month old infants being raised in
monolingual English, monolingual French, or
bilingual English/French households
41Results 14 Month Olds
Looking Time (seconds)
42Results 14 Month Olds
- Infants being raised in monolingual homes
dishabituate to stimuli that cross the category
boundary in their native language - Infants being raised in bilingual homes do not
dishabituate to either stimuli - Bilingual data are not normally distributed
some infants appear to be responding as
monolinguals, while others dishabituate equally
to both stimuli - Age does not predict pattern of response
- Suggest that it is not the timecourse that
differs but rather the nature of the
representation for those infants dishabituating
to both stimuli
43Conclusions
- 6 month olds separate the stimuli into two
discrete phonetic categories - Category boundary is the same regardless of
language input - 10 month olds from monolingual homes place the
category boundary in the appropriate location for
their native language - 10 month olds from bilingual homes do not appear
to categorize the stimuli - This suggests that the timecourse, and possibly
the nature, of bilingual phonetic representation
is distinctly different than that of monolinguals
44Conclusions
- 14 month olds from monolingual homes place the
category boundary in the appropriate location for
their native language - 14 month olds from bilingual homes appear to
divide into two groups - Infants who categorize the stimuli as
monolinguals in one of their two languages - Infants who maintain both English and French
category boundaries equally
45- This suggests that for some bilinguals, it is the
nature, not the developmental time course, of
phonetic representation that is distinctly
different than that of monolinguals
46Bilingual Infants Only
47Theoretical Implications
- Is it language mixing at 10-12 months for
bilinguals? Maybe - But by 14 months there are 3 distinct patterns
- Some infants show language dominance
- Others show two differentiated systems
- No evidence of language mixing
48Conclusions
- Taken together, these studies provide pretty
convincing support against the ULS hypothesis - There is little evidence of sustained language
mixing - Stronger evidence for either DLS or one dominant
language