Li6 Phonology and Morphology - PowerPoint PPT Presentation

1 / 34
About This Presentation
Title:

Li6 Phonology and Morphology

Description:

Cena 1978, Jaeger 1980, McCawley 1986. Pierrehumbert 2002 ... Amsterdam: John Benjamins. Janda, Richard, Brian Joseph, and Neil Jacobs. 1992. ... – PowerPoint PPT presentation

Number of Views:420
Avg rating:3.0/5.0
Slides: 35
Provided by: bert61
Category:

less

Transcript and Presenter's Notes

Title: Li6 Phonology and Morphology


1
Li6 Phonology and Morphology
  • Rules

2
Lecture plan
  • Key point of tension between symbolic
    rationalists and numerical reductionists
  • Do humans extract generalisations from the data
    in their perceptual worlds?
  • Put differently, is the mind a Turing Machine or
    a recurrent switching network?
  • Evidence for rules
  • What form rules take
  • Degree of specificity
  • Formalism

3
Turing machine vs switch network
  • memory -
  • rules/algorithms/generalisations -

4
Arguments for Turing machine (or against
connectionism)
  • Gallistel 2006
  • Dead reckoning
  • Bee dances
  • Temporal learning in conditioning experiments
  • truly random control (Rescorla 1968)
  • Blocking (Kamin 1969)
  • Minsky and Papert 1969 on 2-layer networks
  • Exclusive OR (thanks to Marc)
  • Cant correctly indicate at its output neuron (or
    neurons) whether there are an even or an odd
    number of neurons firing in its input layer
  • Berent et al. 2006 on plurals in English
    compounds
  • Vaux
  • MSCs, as well see later

5
UR?SR mappings and rules
  • We saw in lecture 1 that humans store both
    abstract underlying representations (URs) and
    more concrete surface representations (SRs)
  • How does one get from one type of representation
    to the other?
  • Hypothesis 1 each is simply memorized
  • Hypothesis 2 UR?SR mappings encoded in
    associative/connectionist network
  • Hypothesis 3a All URs are transformed into SRs
    (and perhaps vice versa) by an ordered series of
    rules
  • Hypothesis 3b Only regular UR?SR mappings
    involve rules

Why favor this one?
6
Animals extract generalisations
7
Generalisation by animals
Gallistel, C. 2003. Conditioning from an
information processing perspective. Behavioural
Processes 61.31234 1-13.
8
Generalisation by infants
  • Marcus et al 1999
  • Question
  • Do infants extract linguistic generalisations,
    and in what form?
  • Method
  • 16 infants randomly assigned to one of two
    groups, each familiarized with 2-minute speech
    sample
  • ABA group 3 reps of each of 16 3-word sentences
    from ABA grammar (ga ti ga, li na li, etc.)
  • ABB group same with ABB grammar (ga ti ti, etc.)
  • After habituation, testing on sentences of 3
    novel nonce words
  • test sentences varied as to whether they were
    consistent or inconsistent with the grammar of
    the habituation sentences.
  • Because none of the test words appeared in the
    habituation phase, infants could not distinguish
    the test sentences based on transitional
    probabilities, and because the test sentences
    were the same length and were generated by a
    computer, the infant could not distinguish them
    based on statistical properties such as number of
    syllables or prosody.
  • Results
  • The infants attend longer to sentences with
    unfamiliar structures.
  • Conclusions
  • Results suggest that infants can represent,
    extract, and generalize abstract algebraic rules.

Mean time spent looking in the direction of the
consistent and inconsistent stimuli in each
condition for experiments 1, 2, and 3.
9
Conclusions about generalisation extraction
  • Ample evidence that humans extract
    generalisations from patterns of data in the real
    world
  • These are directly captured in rules
  • These are not captured insightfully (or sometimes
    at all) by switch-network models (surface
    constraints, connectionist networks)

10
Evidence for rules
11
Internal evidence
  • A typical line of argumentation
  • When does glottalization occur?
  • sat
  • Atlantic, atmosphere, coat-tails
  • tap, atrocious
  • Since glottalization/unrelease is predictable, we
    dont want this to be part of the underlying
    representation, under the assumption that
    speakers dont store redundant information.
  • If this is the case, we need a rule to glottalize
    stops in the appropriate environments.
  • What form should this rule take?

12
External evidence
  • Productivity
  • Child and adult Wug tests, e.g. Pinker and
    Ullmann on novel plurals
  • Speech therapy
  • Click girl undoing her problem with lightning
    quickness
  • Syllable deletion in speech errors
  • unanímity jun??nIm?Ri ? unámity ju?nQm?Ri
  • treméndously tHr??mEnd?sli ? trémenly
    ?tHrEm?nli
  • specifícity spEs??fIs?Ri ? specífity
    spE?sIf?Ri
  • What is the error in each case?
  • We need a rule to assign a new stress in these
    words if there were no rules, we should expect
    the forms to be stressless
  • What sort of rules do we need to account for the
    outcome of these errors?
  • First-language acquisition phenomena
  • Over-regularization (goed for went, etc.)
  • Transfer in second-language acquisition
  • Speakers have trouble suppressing L1 rules
  • Japanese/Korean palatalization, epenthesis
  • English aspiration
  • Hard to explain this phenomenon (L1
    non-suppression) without rules

13
What about extraction of generalisations from
less clear patterns?
  • Morphophonemic rules
  • Static patterns

14
What about generalisations that have exceptions?
  • English Vowel Shift is productive for some
    speakers for some vowels
  • Cena 1978, Jaeger 1980, McCawley 1986
  • Pierrehumbert 2002
  • English /k/ ? s / _ i in Latinate contexts
  • electric-ity vs cheek-y ? cheesy
  • Is the rule active, or just a historical remnant?
  • Method
  • ADJ?N (back formation)
  • In Pierres entire career as a curator, he had
    never before seen such a perfect example of
    hovacity. It was an electrifyingly ______
    sculpture.
  • N?ADJ (forward formation)
  • Before Pierre stood an electrifyingly hovac
    sculpture. In his entire career as curator, he
    had never before seen such a perfect example of
    ______.
  • Results
  • The alternation was productive, but only for
    Latinate and semi-Latinate targets.

15
What about generalisations that show no
alternations?
  • Esper 1925
  • Test subjects break up nonce words into morphemes
    based on phonotactics of their L1
  • Moreton 1999
  • Speakers have active knowledge of constraint on
    monosyllables ending in lax vowel, which they use
    in speech perception
  • Pater and Tessier 2003
  • toy grammars easier to acquire when their
    alternations conform to phonotactic
    generalizations in their L1
  • Dell et al 2000
  • speech errors conform to phonotactics of data in
    toy language
  • Cebrian 2002
  • native English speakers, and Catalan learners of
    English, use this restriction in interpreting the
    morphological composition of nonce words.
  • Vaux 2003
  • Productivity of MSCs
  • Kaun and Harrison 1999 on Tuvan reduplication

16
Tuvan overwriting reduplication
  • Common assumption among phonologists
  • Non-alternating structure is stored as such in
    underlying forms.
  • Alternating structure is not stored in URs.
  • Alternation Condition (Kiparsky 68), Lexicon
    Optimization (PS 93)
  • Kaun and Harrison 1999
  • Observation Tuvan VH all vowels in a root agree
    wrt ?back
  • Question does vowel harmony apply to
    non-alternating forms?
  • Method teach subjects Jocular Reduplication see
    if new V triggers root harmony
  • Replace first vowel of root with a nom book ?
    nom-nam
  • If root vowel is a, replace it with u at
    name ? at-ut
  • Results harmonic forms reharmonize, disharmonic
    forms dont
  • Harmonic words idik boot ? idik-adik (not
    adik)
  • Disharmonic words maina car ? maina-muina
    (mui/una)

17
Tuvan overwriting reduplication
  • Conclusions
  • Disharmonic forms are fully specified
    underlyingly
  • Harmonic forms are not (Free Ride, McCarthy
    2004)
  • Theoretical implication
  • Generalisations can be formed over
    non-alternating phonological material

i d i k m a i n a
-bk b -b b
18
Technical aspects of rule formalism
19
The formal statement of rules
  • Rules take the general form A ? B /X_Y
  • A target of rule, an element in UR
  • ? becomes
  • B what the segment containing A becomes
  • / in the environment of
  • _ position of the target A
  • X element left-adjacent to A (can be absent)
  • Y element right-adjacent to A (can be absent)
  • word boundary
  • Ø zero/nothing
  • /X/ underlying form
  • X surface form
  • stray segment
  • (X) optional segment
  • a,ß,? variables

20
Key rule types
  • Insertion
  • Ø ? A / B _ C
  • Insert A between any BC sequence
  • Ø ? A / _
  • Insert A word-finally
  • Deletion
  • A ? Ø / B _ C
  • Delete A between B and C
  • A ? Ø / _
  • Delete A word-initially
  • Alpha Rule
  • aX ? -aX / B _ s
  • Invert the feature specification for X when it
    occurs after B at the end of a syllable

21
Desiderata in rules
  • Keys
  • Elsewhere Case UR
  • Use as few rules as possible
  • This includes trying to collapse rules dealing
    with (seemingly) separate phenomena, such as the
    English plural and other voice assimilation
    processes
  • Be as general as possible
  • E.g. try stops rather than p t
  • Be as predictive as possible
  • a rule that merely describes the facts is
    essentially useless
  • The last two points normally boil down to the
    same thing (use as few features as possible,
    etc.)
  • Linguists generally temper their rule
    formulations with consideration of what is
    (typologically) plausible

22
Choosing a UR
  • Relevance of Elsewhere Case
  • English aspiration
  • Insertion of material is less common than
    deletion
  • Generalisation avoid insertion/creation of
    arbitrary elements
  • Consideration of rule typology
  • Final devoicing, palatalization, etc.
  • An example
  • Sound X occurs only at the ends of words, while
    sound Y occurs anywhere but at the ends of words.
    Which of the following rules is most likely to be
    involved?
  • X ? Y / ___
  • Y ? X / ___
  • X ? Y everywhere but / ___

23
Use as few rules as possible
  • English aspiration
  • p ? ph, t ? th, k ? kh 3 rules
  • -voice, -cont ? spread glottis 1 rule

24
Use as few features as possible
  • Voicing neutralization
  • Russian voiced obstruents become voiceless
    word-finally
  • Voiced obstruents voice, -sonorant,
    consonantal
  • Relatively specific formulation
  • voice, -son, cons ? -voice, -son, cons / _
  • More general/predictive formulation, using fewer
    features
  • -son ? -voice / _
  • voiceless obstruents vacuously undergo the rule

25
Spanish spirantization
  • Noun definite gloss
  • banca baNka la banca la BaNka bank
  • demora demoRa la demora la DemoRa delay
  • gana gana la gana la ?ana desire
  • What are the segments targeted by the rule?
  • In what environment(s) do they undergo the rule?
  • The set of sounds that undergoes this change is
    the voiced stops, i.e. the natural class of
    consonantal, -sonorant, -continuant, voice
    segments.
  • The set of sounds produced by the rule is the
    voiced fricatives, i.e. the natural class of
    consonantal, -sonorant, continuant, voice
    segments.
  • The set of sounds that triggers the change is
    vowels and r, i.e. the natural class of
    continuant segments.
  • We could therefore say
  • cons, -son, -cont, voice ? cons, -son,
    cont, voice / cont_
  • However, we want to be as general and efficient
    as possible. Therefore
  • voice ? continuant / continuant _

26
English plural formation
  • Formation of regular plurals of nouns in English
  • cat cats
  • dog dogz
  • ash ash?z
  • Possible analyses
  • 1. Memorize each word and its plural form.
  • 2. Memorize 3 plural endings assign each word
    to class 1, 2, or 3.
  • 3. plural ?
  • s after p t k ...
  • z after b d g ...
  • ?z after
  • 4. several general rules (holding over domains
    broader than the plural)
  • Plural selection plural ? /-z/
  • Epenthesis Ø ? ? / _ cf. knish
  • Voicing Assimilation -son ? avoice / _
    avoice s cf. fif-th
  • Predictions?
  • Analyses 1 and 2 predict that speakers will be
    unable to deal with foreign and made-up words.

27
What does each model predict?
  • Some theories of English plural formation
  • rule-based
  • pl ? -z / aeioubdgmn?ð _
  • -s / ptk?f _
  • -?z / szcj _
  • pl ? -z / voice, -strident _
  • -s / -voice, -strident
    _
  • -?z / strident _
  • pl ? -?z / strident _
  • -s / -voice _
  • -z / elsewhere
  • rule 1 pl ? /-z/
  • rule 2 Ø ? ? / _
  • rule 3 cons ? -voice / -voice _
  • probabilistic (? analogical, connectionist)
  • wug PL ? 70 of g-final words take -z ? 70
    wugz
  • wug PL ? 70 of g-final words take -z ? 100
    wugz
  • memory-based

unordered
ordered
28
Conclusions
29
Avoiding insertion/creation
  • Proto-Polynesian C ? Ø / _
  • Synchronic analysis
  • Passive /-ia/, gerundive /-a?a/
  • V ? Ø / V _
  • Better to have C-deletion rule than to have many
    allomorphs for the passive and the gerundive
  • The allomorphy analysis also incorrectly predicts
    the existence of roots selecting -tia but -ma?a
  • NB Maori actually did later choose the allomorphy
    analysis, and then made -tia its default form

Hale, Ken. 1973. Deep-surface canonical
disparities in relation to analysis and change
An Australian example. Current Trends in
Linguistics 11401-458.
30
References
  • Armbruster, Thomas. 1978. The Psychological
    Reality of the Vowel Shift and Laxing Rules
    Dissertation Abstracts International.
    391516A-17A.
  • Aske, Jon. 1990. Disembodied Rules versus
    Patterns in the Lexicon Testing the
    Psychological Reality of Spanish Stress Rules
    Berkeley Ling. Soc.. Berkeley 30-45. Proceedings
    of the Sixteenth Annual Meeting of the Berkeley
    Linguistics Society, February 16-19, 1990
    General Session and Parasession on the Legacy of
    Grice. Hall, Kira (ed.) Koenig, Jean-Pierre
    (ed.) Meacham, Michael (ed.) Reinman, Sondra
    (ed.) Sutton, Laurel A. (ed.).
  • Berent, Iris, Steven Pinker, G. Ghavami, and S.
    Murphy. 2006. The Dislike of Regular Plurals in
    Compounds Phonological Familiarity or
    Morphological Constraint? Manuscript, Harvard
    University.
  • Bernstein Ratner, N. 1984 Phonological rule usage
    in mother-child speech. Journal of Phonetics
    12245-254.
  • Cena, R. 1978. When is a phonological
    generalization psychologically real? Bloomington
    Indiana University Linguistics Club.
  • Dell, Gary, Reed, K.D., Adams, D.R., Meyer, A.
    2000. Speech errors, phonotactic constraints, and
    implicit learning A study of the role of
    experience in language production. Journal of
    Experimental Psychology Learning, Memory, and
    Cognition 61355-1367.
  • Gallistel, C. Randy. 2003. Conditioning from an
    information processing perspective. Behavioural
    Processes 61.31234 1-13.
  • Gallistel, C.Randy. 2006. The nature of learning
    and the functional architecture of the brain. In
    Q. Jing, et al (Eds) Psychological Science Around
    the World, vol 1. Proceedings of the 28th
    International Congress of Psychology. Sussex
    Psychology Press.
  • Hale, Ken. 1973. Deep-surface canonical
    disparities in relation to analysis and change
    An Australian example. Current Trends in
    Linguistics 11401-458.
  • Hauser, Marc, Daniel Weiss, and Gary Marcus.
    2002. Rule learning by cotton-top tamarins.
    Cognition 86B15B22.
  • Hetzron, Robert. 1972. The Shape of a Rule and
    Diachrony. Bulletin of the School of Oriental and
    African Studies 35.3451-475.
  • Iverson, Greg. 1994. The Reality of Linguistic
    Rules (Studies in Language Companion Series, no.
    26), ed. with S. Lima R. Corrigan. Amsterdam
    John Benjamins.
  • Janda, Richard, Brian Joseph, and Neil Jacobs.
    1992. Systematic hyperforeignisms as maximally
    external evidence for linguistic rules. In
    Iverson et al, the reality of linguistic rules.
  • Marcus, Gary, S. Vijayan, S. Bandi Rao, and P.
    Vishton. 1999. Rule learning by seven-month-old
    infants. Science 283.5398.
  • Minsky, Marvin and Seymour Papert. 1969.
    Perceptrons. Cambridge MIT Press.
  • Moreton, Elliott. 1999. Evidence for phonological
    grammar in speech perception. In J. J. Ohala, Y.
    Hasegawa, M. Ohala, D. Granville, and A. C.
    Bailey (eds.), Proceedings of the 14th
    International Congress of Phonetic Sciences, San
    Francisco, pp. 2215-2217.
  • Pater, J. and A.-M. Tessier. 2003. Phonotactic
    Knowledge and the Acquisition of Alternations. In
    M.J. Solé, D. Recasens, and J. Romero (eds.)
    Proceedings of the 15th International Congress on
    Phonetic Sciences, Barcelona. 1777-1180.
  • Pierrehumbert 2002, an unnatural process. LabPhon
    8.
  • Pinker and Prince. 1994. Regular and irregular
    morphology and the psychological status of rules
    of grammar. In S. D. Lima, R. L. Corrigan, G. K.
    Iverson (eds.), The reality of linguistic rules,
    321-51. Amsterdam Benjamins.

31
Truly random control
  • Shows that
  • statistical correlations of the sort if CS then
    US do not drive generalisation formation
  • Categorical generalisations can be extracted from
    gradient distributions
  • vs 33 response as one might expect for Group 1

32
Blocking
  • Shows that something beyond statistical
    association is taking place

33
Esper 1925
  • Method
  • Ss learn names of 16 objects, each having one of
    four different shapes and one of four different
    colors
  • Ss trained on 14 object-name associations but
    tested on 16 to see if they generalize what they
    learned
  • 3 experimental conditions
  • names presented to Group 1
  • naslig, sownlig, nasdeg, sowndeg, where nas- and
    sown- coded color and -lig and -deg coded shape
  • Since these names consisted of two phonologically
    legal morphemes, this group could simplify their
    task by learning not 16 names but 8 morphemes (if
    they could discover them) plus the simple rule
    that the color morpheme preceded the shape
    morpheme in each name.
  • Names presented to Group 2
  • bi-morphemic names, as with Group 1
  • unlike group 1, the morphemes were not
    phonologically legal for English, e.g., nulgen,
    nuzgub, pelgen, pezgub (where nu- and pe- were
    color morphemes and -lgen and -zgub were shape
    morphemes, the latter two violating English
    morpheme structure constraints)
  • Names presented to Group 3 (a control group)
  • names with no morphemic structure
  • no recourse but to learn 16 idiosyncratic names
  • Results
  • As expected, group 1 learned their names much
    faster and more accurately than group 3.
  • Performance of Group 2 was similar to (and
    marginally worse than) that of group 3
  • Analysis of the errors of group 2, including how
    they generalized what theyd learned to the two
    object-name associations excluded from the
    training session, revealed that they tried to
    make phonologically legal morphemes from the
    ill-formed ones. 
  • Demonstrates (i) psychological reality of MSCs
    (ii) ability to conduct morphological analysis

34
Korean borrowing of Coda t
  • Korean word-final t ? /t, th, t, c, ch, c,
    s, s/
  • Surface word-final postvocalic t in loans and
    nonce words invariably assigned to /s/ (Martin
    1992, Kang 1998, Hayes 1998, Iverson Lee 2004)
  • supermarket ? nom. sup?makhet, dat.
    sup?makhese
  • What appears to be involved in the Korean case is
    that speakers know that surface word-final ts
    most often come from underlying /s/ in their
    native lexicon, and they therefore assign all new
    words to the same pattern.
Write a Comment
User Comments (0)
About PowerShow.com