Schemadriven Memory and Structural Alignment - PowerPoint PPT Presentation

1 / 1
About This Presentation
Title:

Schemadriven Memory and Structural Alignment

Description:

... however, this line of study, and the questions it posed, ... first recall task, as well as the information relevant to their first perspective that they had ... – PowerPoint PPT presentation

Number of Views:86
Avg rating:3.0/5.0
Slides: 2
Provided by: smi9150
Category:

less

Transcript and Presenter's Notes

Title: Schemadriven Memory and Structural Alignment


1
Schema-driven Memory and Structural Alignment
C. Hunt Stilwell Arthur B. Markman
Experiment 3 Mean Number of Properties Recalled
Experiment 2 OLD Sentences Mean Recognition
  • Experiment 1 Mean Recall
  • Results For both sub-targets, participants
    recalled more when they received the
  • analogous Base Story (m 6.33 Target 1, 4.67
    Target 2) than when they received the
    non-analogous
  • Schematic Memory
  • Schematic memory research flourished in the
    1960s and 1970s
  • Researchers produced several findings relating
    to memory for thematic information.
  • Schemas select information for encoding
    participants recall schema-relevant information
  • better than schema-irrelevant information, and
    schemas facilitate recall for relevant info
  • (e.g., Bransford Johnson, 1972).
  • Schemas are abstract participants remember the
    gist, rather than the surface details
  • (Bransford Franks, 1971).
  • Schemas allow for inferences about instances,
    including false ones (e.g., Sulin Dooling,
  • 1974).
  • Memory takes place through an on-line
    reconstructive process from abstract gist
  • representations (Bartlett, 1935).
  • Lacking a concrete theoretical framework,
    however, this line of study, and the questions it
    posed,
  • gave way to the list-learning research of the
    1980s.
  • Structural
    Alignment
  • Analogy has been shown to influence memory by
    highlighting relevant information, and ignoring
  • irrelevant information (Markman Gentner,
    1997 Schustack Anderson, 1979). This is
    similar
  • to the role of schemas.
  • It is known that analogy, and comparisons in
    general, use the process of structural alignment
  • Recall Task 1 Mean Recall
    Target 1 Mean Recall Target 2
    Mean Recall
  • Results
  • In first recall, participants recall more when
    they have read the analogous Base Story (m 5.89
    Target 1,
  • 3.83 Target 2) than when they saw the
    non-analogous Base Story (m 4.64 Target 1, 3.11
    Target 2).
  • In second recall, participants recalled more
    from Targets for which the non-analogous story
    had been
  • read in encoding (m 5.26 Target 1 , 3.75
    Target 2) than they had in the first recall (m
    4.64 Target 1, 3.11 Target 2).
  • Conclusions Replicate Experiments 1 2
    replicate Anderson Pichert, with a change in
    analogical correspondence (change in
  • perspective) leading to recall of previously
    unrecalled information.

2a 2b
Experiment 2 New Sentences Mean Recognition
  • Summary of Results
  • Experiment 1 Replicate Bransford Johnson -
    memory is better for analogous, or
    schema-relevant information
  • than for non-analogous, or schema-irrelevant
    information.
  • Experiment 2a Replicate Sulin Dooling with
    short delay participants do not make many
    intrusions. Consistent with
  • Alba Hasher with short delay,
    schema-relevant and schema-irrelevant information
    recognized equally well in a recognition
  • task.
  • Experiment 2b Replicate Experiment 1
    Replicate Sulin Dooling with long delay-
    schema-relevant information
  • intrudes on memory for analogous (thematic)
    information frequently.
  • Experiment 3 Replicate Experiments 1 2b
    replicate Anderson Pichert - a change in
    analogical correspondence
  • (shift in perspective) yields recall of
    previously unrecallable information.
  • Conclusions
  • The goal of these studies was to replicate
    major findings from the schema-driven memory
    literature using an
  • analogical paradigm, allowing us to then carry
    over what we know about analogy, and the
    structural alignment
  • process, to the study of these memory
    phenomena. Experiments 1 2 replicated two major
    findings, and the
  • preliminary results from Experiment 3 are
    consistent with a third major finding. Thus, we
    can conclude that
  • schematic memory is driven by analogy.
  • We can now begin to carry over what we know
    about analogy to the domain of schematic memory.
  • Sulin Dooling, (1974)
  • During encoding presented participants with
    biographical characters in which the main
  • character was either given the name of a
    famous person (e.g., Hellen Keller) or given a
  • novel name (e.g., Carol Harris).
  • Delay of 5 minutes or 1 week
  • Recognition Task When the main character was
    given the name of a famous person,
  • participants mistakenly remembered having seen
    information about that character that was not
  • presented in the original biographical story
    with 1 week delay, but not with 5 minute delay.
  • Conclusion Schema-relevant, or thematic
    information intrudes on memory for thematic
  • information. This effect increased with the
    length of delay (5 minutes vs. 1 week).
  • Experiment 2a and b
  • Goal Replicate experiment 1 using a
    recognition task replicate Sulin Dooling
    (1974).
  • Prediction With a short delay (20-30
    minutes), participants will be able to
    distinguish
  • between old and new sentences (Sulin
    Dooling, 1974. In addition, they will not
  • show a preference for schema-relevant
    information (Alba Hasher, 1983). With a
  • long delay, participants will mistakenly
    recognize intrusion sentences that are
  • schema-relevant, and will remember
    schema-relevant old sentences better than

2a
2b
  • Conclusions
  • In Experiment 2a, there was no preference for
    schema-relevant over schema-
  • irrelevant information at a short delay with
    a recognition task, consistent with Alba
  • Hasher (1983).
  • In Experiment 2b, as in Experiment 1, memory
    for analogous, or schema-
  • relevant, information was better than memory
    for non-analogous, or schema-
  • irrelevant, information, this time in a
    recognition task.
  • Replicate Sulin Dooling using the analogical
    reasoning paradigm -
  • information relevant to the analogical match
    (schema-relevant information)
  • present in one domain intrudes on memory for
    the other domain after long delay (2b),
  • but not after a short one (2a).
  • Encoding
  • Comparison Task Participants were given pairs
    of stories, one story at a time, and asked to
  • rate their similarity.
  • Target Stories The second story in each pair
    served a target, that contained two
  • sub-stories (Sub-story 1 Sub-Story 2) that
    had different sets of relations and objects,
  • but the same characters. Each Target Story
    was given a title.
  • Base Stories The first story in each pair
    served a base. For each Target Story, there
  • were two separate Base Stories, each
    corresponding to one of the two sub-stories in
  • the Target Story.
  • For each Base-Target pair a participant
    received, the Base formed the Schema, and
  • the Target sub-story to which it corresponded
    was analogous, or relevant to the
  • Schema, while the Target sub-story to which
    it did not correspond, was irrelevant to
  • the Schema.
  • After the Encoding Task, participants received
    a Filler Task or Delay.
  • Retrieval
  • Participants were given the titles of the
    Target Stories from each pair, and either a
    recall or
  • recognition task for information from the
    Target Stories.
  • Anderson Pichert, (1978)
  • Presented participants with the story of a boy
    who skipped school, during encoding.
  • Participants were asked to read the story from
    the perspective of either a Home Buy or a
  • Burglar
  • First Recall Task Asked to recall as much as
    they could from the original story. Participants
  • recalled more information relevant to the
    perspective they took than information relevant
    to
  • the other perspective.
  • Perspective Shift Told to now think about the
    story that they had ready from the other
  • perspective.
  • Second Recall Task Asked participants to
    recall the original story again. Participants
    now
  • recalled information relevant to their new
    perspective that they had not remembered in the
  • first recall task, as well as the information
    relevant to their first perspective that they had
  • remembered in the first recall task.
  • Conclusion With a shift in perspective,
    participants are able to recall information the
    had
  • previously not remembered.
  • Experiment 3
  • Goal Replicate experiment 1 using a
    recognition task replicate Anderson Pichert
    (1978).
  • Prediction Memory for information from the
    analogous Target sub-story (schema-
  • relevant information) will be better than
    memory for information from the non-analogous
  • Bransford Johnson (1972)
  • Participants were given paragraphs containing a
    target sentence that does not fit with the
  • schema invoked by the story..
  • E.g., Peace March story containing a
    sentence about a craft landing on an alien
  • planet.
  • During retrieval task, participants fail to
    recall the schema-irrelevant sentence, unless
    they
  • had been given a title that facilitated the
    creation of a schema within which that sentence
    is relevant
  • (e.g., Landing on an alien planet.)
  • Conclusion Memory is better for
    schema-relevant than schema-irrelevant
    information.
  • Experiment 1
  • Goal Replicate Johnson, et a. (1972) using the
    analogical reasoning paradigm described
  • above.
  • Prediction Memory for information from the
    analogous Target sub-story (schema-
  • relevant information) will be better than
    memory for information from the non-analogous
  • Target sub-story (schema-irrelevant
    information).
  • Participants 60 undergraduates at the
    University of Texas, Austin. Participants
    received
  • course credit for their participation.
  • Method
  • Encoding Comparison Task
  • Alba Hasher (1983). Is memory schematic?
    Psychological Bulletin, 93(2), 203-231.
  • Anderson, R. C. Pichert, J.W. (1978). Recall
    of previously unrecallable information following
    a shift in perspective. Journal
  • of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior,
    17(1), 1-12.
  • Bartlett, F. C. (1932). Remembering.
    Cambridge, England University Press.
  • Bransford, J. D. Franks, J. J. (1971). The
    abstraction of linguistic ideas A review.
    Cognitive Psychology, 2(4), 331 - 350.
  • Bransford, J. D. Johnson, M. K. (1972).
    Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior,
    11(6), 717-726.
  • Gentner, D. (1983). Structure mapping A
    theoretical framework for analogy. Cognitive
    Science, 7, 155-170.
  • Markman, A. B. Gentner, D. (1993). Structural
    alignment during similarity comparisons.
    Cognitive Psychology,
  • 25(4), 431-467
  • Markman, A.B. Gentner, D. (1997). The effects
    of alignability on memory. Psychological
    Science, 8(5), 363-367.
  • Schustack, M. W. Anderson, J. R. (1979).
    Effects of analogy to prior knowledge on memory
    for new information.
  • Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal
    Behavior, 18(5), 565-583.
  • Stilwell, C. H. Markman, A. B. (2001). The
    fate of irrelevant information in analogy.
    Proceedings of the 23rd Annual Conference
  • of the Cognitive Science Society.
  • Sulin, R. A. Dooling, D. J. (1974). Intrusion
    of a thematic idea in retention of prose.
    Journal of Experimental
  • Psychology, 103(2), 255-262.
  • Method
  • Encoding Comparison Task. Each Base Story
    contains information relevant to the
  • theme which is not contained in the
    corresponding Target Story.
  • Filler Task (15-30 minutes)
  • Retrieval 1 Recognition Task
  • Change of Perspective Read the Base Stories
    they had not seen in the Comparison
  • Task
  • Retrieval 2 Recognition Task
Write a Comment
User Comments (0)
About PowerShow.com