Title: Emotion Regulation and Understanding in Three-
1Emotion Regulation and Understanding in Three-
Four-Year-OldsPamela M. Cole Tracy A.
DennisLaura H. CohenThe Pennsylvania State
UniversityThis research has been reviewed and
approved for compliance with the policy of The
Pennsylvania State University Institutional
Review Board, 00B0230-00, 3/10/2000
- DISCUSSION
- Â
- The puppet procedure appeared to tap very young
childrens recognition of effective emotion
regulatory strategies. Even the young 3-year-olds
showed recognition above that predicted by
chance. During the period between 36 to 48 months
this ability increases. Consistent with evidence
from the Theory of Mind literature (Wellman,
1992), some time after the 3rd birthday children
gain increasing awareness of mental states and
possibly their malleability. - Our data are limited by the fact that ours was a
first attempt at this procedure. We provided one
story for each emotion, and only three
effective strategies for each vignette. We
chose these limitations out of concern for a
young childs ability to tolerate the verbal
demands of the task. - Surprise! Even with these limitations,
childrens awareness of ER strategies was
correlated with maternal report of social
competence and observations of actual child
self-regulation during a mild frustration. - Maternal reports indicated that responsibility
was the skill associated with ER strategy
awareness. We plan to explore the role of
language development, given that items on that
scale focus on communicating responsibly. - Awareness of ER strategies appeared to be
related to autonomous and flexible efforts to
solve the problem (the locked box), pointing
toward the role of the childs developing sense
of self-efficacy and optimism. - REFERENCES
- Band, E. B., Weisz, J. R. (1988). How to feel
better when it feels bad Childrens perspectives
on coping with everyday stress. Developmental
Psychology, 24, 247-253. - Bartsch, K., Wellman, H. M. (1995). Children
Talk About the Mind. New York Oxford University
Press. - Calkins, S. D., Johnson, M. C. (1998). Toddler
regulation of distress to frustrating events
Temperamental and maternal correlates. Infant
Behavior and Development, 21(3), 379-395. - Denham, S.A. (1998). Emotional Development in
Young Children. New York Guilford. - Dunn, J., Brown, J. (1991). Relationships,
talk about feelings, and the development of
affect regulation in early childhood. In J.
Garber K. Dodge (Eds.), The Development of
Emotion Regulation and Dysfunction. Cambridge
Studies in Social and Emotional Development (pp.
89-108). New York Cambridge University Press. - Eder, R. A. (1990). Uncovering young childrens
psychological selves Individual and
developmental differences. Child Development, 61,
849-863. - Goldsmith, H. H., Rothbart, M. K. (1994,
November). The Laboratory Temperament Assessment
Battery (Version 2.03). Unpublished manuscript. - Gresham, F. M., Elliott, S. N. (1990). Social
Skills Rating System. Circle Pines, MN American
Guidance Service. - Measelle, J. R., Ablow, J. C., Cowan, P. A.,
Cowan, C. P. (1998). Assessing young childrens
views of their academic, social, and emotional
lives An evaluation of the self-perception
scales of the Berkeley Puppet Interview. Child
Development, 69, 1556-1576. - Wellman, H. M. (1992). The Childs Theory of
Mind. Cambridge Cambridge University Press.
RESULTS CONTINUED 2. Young 3-year-olds show
less frequent recognition of effective strategies
compared to older 3-year-olds and all
4-year-olds, F (3, 112) 5.00, p lt .005. The
effect is significant for happy and angry, but
not for sad, vignettes. 3. The number
of effective strategies is moderately but
significantly related to maternal report of
social competence, but particularly
responsibility. SSRS Scales Effective
Strategies Recognized All Emotions Anger
Sadness Only Social Skills .20 .16
Cooperation .06 .12m
Responsibility .33
.30 Assertion .12m .04
Self-Control .00 -.05 4. The number
of effective strategies is negatively, moderately
and significantly related to maternal report of
externalizing problems only. SSRS Scales
Effective Strategies Recognized All
Emotions Anger Sadness Only Problem Behaviors
- .01 -.05 Externalizing
-.15 -.17 Internalizing
-.03 -.05 5. The number of effective
strategies is associated with aspects of
preschoolers actual regulatory attempts during a
mild frustration (the lock box procedure).
Regulatory Activity Effective Strategies
Recognized All Emotions Anger Sadness
Only Persistence .20
.16 Flexibility .29
.25 Distraction -.17
-.07 Self-speech .11
.10 Self-soothing -.03
.02 Support-seeking -.20
-.33 Venting/acting out -.05
-.12
- STUDY AIMS
- to determine if a puppet procedure would engage
3- and 4-year-olds well enough to tap their
implicit awareness of strategies for regulating
emotion - to determine if 3- and 4-year-olds recognized
effective emotion regulation strategies - to determine whether this age period captures a
developmental transition in awareness of emotion
regulation strategies - to determine if awareness is related to social
competence or actual emotion regulation - METHOD
- Participants 116 3- and 4-year-old boys and
girls - Generation Recognition of Effective ER
Strategies - Two child puppets 1 mother puppet enacted 3
vignettes in which happiness, anger, and sadness
needed to be reduced. After each vignette, the
child puppets asked the 3- or 4-year old, How
can we stop feeling so ________? - A research assistant repeated the situation, the
question, the puppets need for the childs
help to facilitate the childs ability to manage
the cognitive load. The child was given a minute
to respond spontaneously (i.e., generate a
strategy) and then given 3 pairs of strategies -
one effective and one ineffective - and asked to
choose one from each pair. Sketches depicting
each strategy type (e.g., a child thinking a
different thought) aided the childs
understanding of choices.
- THE ANGRY VIGNETTE
- Red Brownie are happy content, playing with
some toys. Each is playing alone but sitting
next to the other. They both reach for the same
toy. - Red (looks over at Brownie, speaks with emphatic
irritation) I need that toy, Brownie. (Pulls the
toy). - Brownie (angrily protesting) HEY, no-oo! I need
that toy! - Red (very angry, yells) I NEED IT! (To child
subject) Childs name, Brownie wont give it
to me! - Brownie (also very angry, loud, jumping up)
NOOO!! I NEED IT! (They struggle with the toy,
then Brownie says to child subject in a sullen
voice) Childs name, Red wont give it to
me!!!!! - Red Brownie (both very angry, loud, tugging on
the toy) Its mine!!!!!!! - Brownie Im telling Mom. MOOOOM!!
- Mom (enters, very angry) You two STOP being so
angry! If you do not STOP being angry, Im
taking all the toys away! (Mom marches off.) - Red Brownie (so angry, both turning to child
subject) We are SO angry. Please childs
name, what can we do to STOP feeling so angry? - ASSESSMENT OF STRATEGY AWARENESS
- SPONTANEOUS STRATEGY GENERATION
- Lets see, Red Brownie need your help, childs
name. They must STOP feeling so angry or Mom
will take away the toys. Childs name whats
the best way to STOP feeling so angry? - RECOGNITION OF EFFECTIVE STRATEGIES
- ASSESSMENT OF ACTUAL REGULATORY STRATEGIES
- The Lock Box Task is a frustration task from the
LABTab Temperament Battery (Goldsmith Rothbart,
1990) designed to assess a childs ability to
regulate anger. A desired figurine is locked in
an acrylic box and the child tries to open the
box with the keys but has the wrong set of keys.
The task was 3 minutes long. - Observed Regulatory Strategies
- Persistence (continuing to try to open the box)
- Flexibility (trying alternate strategies to open
the box) - Distraction (focusing on a different task)
- Self-speech (talking to oneself about the task)
- Self-soothing (calming oneself through self-touch
or rest) - Support-seeking (trying to get an adult to help)
- Disruption venting (appearing frustrated)
- THE LOCKBOX
ABSTRACT Preschoolers awareness that emotions
can be regulated was tested using a new puppet
procedure. We interviewed 116 3- and 4-year-old
children via 3 puppet-enacted vignettes involving
happy, angry, or sad emotions. Recognition of
effective strategies was used as an index of
awareness. Young 3-year-olds endorsed fewer
effective strategies than older 3-year-olds and
4-year-olds. This effect was significant for
anger but not for sadness. It also appeared to
hold for one particular type of strategy, i.e.,
behavioral distraction but not for cognitive
distraction or problem solving. The number of
effective strategies endorsed by children was
positively associated with mother-reported social
social skills, and in particular, responsibility,
They were negatively associated with
externalizing symptoms. Gender effects did not
emerge. Implications for childrens actual
emotion regulatory behavior and associations with
the Theory of Mind literature are discussed.
INTRODUCTION Young childrens awareness that
they can regulate emotions appears implicit in
their behavior, but it has been difficult to
demonstrate that understanding. Puppet enactments
provide a method for tapping young childrens
understanding of social phenomena (Denham,
Eder, 1990 Measelle, Ablow, Cowan, Cowan,
1998). At about 2 years of age, children
explain their own and others behavior in terms
of emotions and desires (Bartsch Wellman, 1995)
and appear aware of the causes and consequences
of emotions, using this understanding to
influence others affective states (Dunn Brown,
1991). It is unclear whether very young
children recognize that emotions are states that
can be regulated. This knowledge is not trivial
as it may play a role in promoting adaptive
outcomes. It remains uncertain, however, whether
knowledge of emotion regulation actually relates
to social-emotional competence.
- TYPES OF EMOTION REGULARTORY STRATEGIES
- The coping literature dichotomizes strategies as
either problem-focused (effort is directed at
correcting the situation) or emotion-focused
(effort is directed at modifying the emotion
state). Developmental research suggests that
emotion-focused coping emerges later than
problem-focused coping (e.g., Band Weisz,
1988). - The literature on early emotion regulation,
however, suggests that very young children engage
spontaneously in self-distraction and
instrumental coping behaviors (e.g., Calkins
Johnson, 1998). - We examined distraction and problem-solving
strategies. Distraction strategies were
subdivided into those that involved a cognitive
distraction (thinking about something else) or a
behavioral distraction (doing something else).