Visual, Visuospatial, and RightBrain Disorders - PowerPoint PPT Presentation

1 / 75
About This Presentation
Title:

Visual, Visuospatial, and RightBrain Disorders

Description:

The agnosia involves all visual stimuli including faces, the environment, and objects. ... Visual Illusions and Hallucinations. Visual illusions and ... – PowerPoint PPT presentation

Number of Views:950
Avg rating:3.0/5.0
Slides: 76
Provided by: pamgu
Category:

less

Transcript and Presenter's Notes

Title: Visual, Visuospatial, and RightBrain Disorders


1
Visual, Visuospatial, and Right-Brain Disorders
2
  • Perception refers to the ability to sense a
    stimulus regardless of whether the stimulus is
    recognized. Lesions of the eye or the optic
    nerve produce ipsilateral blindness (partial or
    total) lesions behind the optic chiasm result in
    homonymous visual field defects pathological
    changes in the inferior calcarine cortex produce
    achromatopsia (central color blindness) medial
    occipitotemporal lesions of the right hemisphere
    are associated with prosopagnosia (defective
    recognition of familiar faces) and environmental
    agnosia (impaired recognition of familiar
    places) and bilateral lesions of the medial
    occipitotemporal cortex or inferior longitudinal
    fasciculi cause visual object agnosia.

3
Monocular Blindness
4
  • Ocular disorders including diseases of the lens
    (cataracts), retina, or macula produce total or
    partial blindness of the involved eye. Likewise,
    diseases of the optic nerve (such as glaucoma
    affecting the nerve head, ischemic optic
    neuritis, and optic neuritis) produce unilateral
    blindness ipsilateral to the lesion. Pupillary
    responses are usually compromised in diseases of
    the eye and optic nerve because there is
    diminished light conduction, and this sign may
    help distinguish blindness as a manifestation of
    ocular or neurological disease from feigned
    blindness or blindness as a manifestation of a
    conversion reaction.

5
  • In addition, patients with conversion disorders
    or who are malingering have intact optokinetic
    nystagmus (nystagmus that is normally induced by
    passing a striped cloth in front of the eyes of a
    sighted person).

6
  • Long-standing bilateral blindness may result in
    pendular nystagmus with spontaneous to-and-fro
    movements of the eyes. Visual hallucinations
    (release hallucinations) are not uncommon in
    patients with ocular disorders and may be either
    formed or unformed. Phosphenes (sudden flashes
    of light) often accompany retinal or optic nerve
    disease, and some patients may experience
    synesthesia characterized by phosphenes occurring
    in response to loud sounds or noises.

7
Homonymous Hemianopsia
  • Lesions of the optic tract, optic chiasm,
    geniculate nucleus of the thalamus, or the
    geniculocalcarine radiations produce homonymous
    visual-field defects, defective vision or
    blindess in the right or left halves of the
    visual fields of both eyes. Temporal lobe
    lesions are associated with asymmetric
    (incongruent) field defects occipital lesions
    cause symmetric (congruent) defects.

8
  • Lesions involving the entire radiation produce
    homonymous hemianopsias (usually with sparing of
    the central few degrees of vision), whereas
    lesions affecting only some of the fiber tracts
    produce quadrantanopsias or other incomplete
    field defects. Pupillary responses are intact in
    patients with homonymous visual-field defects,
    and visual acuity is normal.

9
  • Hallucinations are common in the period
    immediately after an injury to the
    geniculocalcarine radiations (e.g. in the first
    few days after a stroke) and may be unformed
    (lights), semiformed (tire track or herringbone
    patterns), or formed (complex scenes, animals, or
    people). The hallucinations are commonly
    confined to the area of the visual-field defect.

10
Achromatopsia
  • Achromatopsia refers to central color blindness.
    The patient loses the ability to distinguish
    color in the contralateral visual field. The
    associated lesions involve the cortex inferior to
    the calcarine fissure of the medial occipital
    lobe. There is often an associated partial
    hemianopsia. The color blindness is difficult to
    demonstrate except when it is bilateral. Color
    blindness is assessed with color-naming tests,
    tests requiring sorting of colors by shades, and
    Ishihara pseudoisochromatic plates.

11
Simultanagnosia
  • Agnosias are recognition deficits rather than
    perceptual disturbances. Simultanagnosia is a
    misnamed exception. In this disorder, when the
    patients are shown two objects simultaneously,
    they fail to perceive one of them. For example,
    when shown a figure consisting of a circle and a
    cross, they see the circle or the cross but not
    both when shown a number drawn by juxtaposing
    many small numbers (for example the number 2
    drawn by using many little number 3s), they see
    the number 2 or the number 3s but not both.

12
  • Simultanagnosia is often observed as part of
    Balints syndrome. The latter consists of optic
    ataxia (the inability to touch objects accurately
    using visual guidance), sticky fixation or
    occular apraxia (difficulty volitionally
    redirecting gaze), and simultanagnosia.

13
Prosopagnosia
  • Prosopagnosia refers to the inability to
    recognize familiar faces. Visual perception is
    intact the patient sees normally and can
    describe the unrecognized face in detail.
    However, familiar faces, such as the faces of
    family members and famous people, cannot be
    recognized, and the patient cannot deduce the
    individuals identity without the aid of
    nonvisual clues. In some cases, specific facial
    features (moustache, glasses of a specific type)
    are used to facilitate recognition, but the
    patient is easily fooled if someone with similar
    characteristics appears.

14
Environmental Agnosia
  • Environmental agnosia is a clinical syndrome
    characterized by the inability to recognize
    familiar places. The patient sees normally and
    is able to describe the surroundings but has no
    sense of familiarity. Patients commonly adopt
    verbal strategies to compensate for the deficit
    for example, they may find their homes by using
    the street signs and house numbers.
    Environmental agnosia and prosopagnosia
    frequently coexist.

15
Visual Object Agnosia
  • Two principal types of visual agnosia have been
    identified apperceptive agnosia and associative
    agnosia. The two identified syndromes reflect
    disruption of different stages of processing of
    complex visual information, and variations of the
    syndromes are common. Apperceptive visual
    agnosia is characterized by intact visual acuity
    but an inability to recognize objects.

16
  • Patients can distinguish shades of light
    intensity, identify colors, determine line
    orientation, exhibit depth perception, see
    movement, and distinguish thin from thick lines.
    They can negotiate their surroundings but cannot
    recognize, describe, or match perceived objects.
    They cannot draw objects they see and cannot
    point to objects named by the examiner. The
    agnosia involves all visual stimuli including
    faces, the environment, and objects.
    Apperceptive agnosia may occur as a phase of
    recovery from cortical blindness after anoxia,
    carbon monoxide poisoning, or bilateral posterior
    hemispheric strokes.

17
Apperceptive visual agnosia is a failure in
higher level perception, also known as
visual-form agnosia. ?The person has normal
visual acuity, but cannot recognize objects based
on their shape. The person cannot recognize
objects, line drawings of objects, nor copy
drawings of objects. They do not know what they
are seeing. A person here could describe a rose
in intimate detail but not know it was a rose
until smelling it. ?Prosopagnosia is a form of
apperceptive visual agnosia in which the person
cannot recognize a face visually, but can do when
hearing their voice. Here, some individuals do
not recognize their own face.
18
  • Associative visual agnosia features intact
    elementary perception and preserved ability to
    describe, draw, and match visual stimuli. For
    example, when shown an object (shoe, toothbrush,
    etc.), patients can draw the object and match the
    object with an identical one from a group of
    objects, but they cannot recognize the object or
    describe its use.

19
?Associative visual agnosia refers to a
disconnection between perceptions and verbal
systems. The person cannot verbally label or name
what they see but they can label or identify it
nonverbally by gesture or behavior which
correctly discriminates the object. A person
here could not say an object was a car or not but
they could make gestures of using a steering
wheel to indicate they can correctly discriminate
the object but it cannot be verbally labeled. A
person here could copy a drawing of an object but
not name it but they could not produce a drawing
of an object if merely asked. A drawing of a car
could be reproduced but the person could not do
so in response to Draw a car.
20
Color Agnosia
  • Color agnosia (also called color anomia) is
    present in some patients who have alexia
    (inability to read) without agraphia (inability
    to write). The patients do not have
    abnormalities of color naming and can name the
    colors appropriate to specific settings (a banana
    is yellow, a fire engine is red) they also can
    sort and match colors. They do not have color
    blindness. They cannot point to named colors or
    name colors pointed to by the examiner.

21
Disturbances of Visuospatial Attention
22
Unilateral Neglect
  • Unilateral (or hemispatial) neglect refers to a
    lack of attention to events and actions in
    one-half of space. Neglect may involve all
    sensory modalities visual, auditory, and
    somatosensory as well as motor acts (motor
    neglect in-intention) and motivation.
    Unilateral visual neglect is the most commonly
    observed form of hemispatial neglect. In its
    most subtle form, the patient ignores the
    stimulus presented in the neglected hemifield.

23
  • This type of neglect can be demonstrated by
    holding one hand in equivalent portions of each
    visual field while facing the patient, quickly
    moving the fingers of each hand, and asking the
    patient to point to the hand whose fingers moved
    (the technique of double simultaneous
    stimulation). The patient will perceive movement
    only in the non-neglected visual field (this test
    requires that the patient not have a field
    defect).

24
  • In its more obvious form, unilateral visual
    neglect may be evidenced by failure to copy
    one-half of model objects, read one-half of
    words, dress one-half of the body, or shave
    one-half of the face. Tests for visual neglect
    include line bisection (the patient crosses the
    line in the middle of the nonneglected portion)
    and the line-crossing test, in which the patient
    is given a sheet of paper with randomly
    distributed lines and is asked to cross each line
    in the middle only those lines in the
    nonneglected field will be crossed.

25
  • In severe forms of neglect, all types of stimuli
    in the affected hemiuniverse are neglected.
    Somesthetic neglect is demonstrated by touching
    the patient simultaneously on both sides of the
    body and asking the patient where he or she was
    touched the patient perceives only the touch on
    the nonneglected side of the body.

26
  • Auditory neglect is assessed by standing behind
    the patient, snapping the fingers on either or
    both sides of the head, and asking the patient to
    point toward the side where a stimulus was heard.
    The patient points only toward the nonneglected
    side during double simultaneous auditory
    stimulation.

27
  • Motor neglect syndromes involve action-intention
    disorders affecting the half of space
    contralateral to frontal lobe or basal ganglia
    lesions. The patient may appear to have a
    hemiparesis because the limb with
    action-intention neglect is unused, but when
    strength and coordination are tested, they are
    found to be normal.

28
Anosognosia and Anosognosic Syndromes
  • Anosognosia is the clinical syndrome
    characterized by denial of hemiparesis. It
    classically occurs in patients with right
    parietal lesions and left unilateral neglect who
    deny their left hemiparesis. Variants of
    anosognosia include somatoparaphrenia (denial of
    ownership of the paralyzed limbs), anosodiaphoria
    (minimization of and unconcern about the weakness
    without complete denial), misoplegia (hatred of
    the paralyzed or weak limb), personification
    (naming the limb and giving it an identity), and
    supernumerary limbs (reduplication of limbs on
    the neglected side).

29
Visual Anosognosia
30
  • Also called Antons syndrome, visual agnosia
    (from the Greek, a negation nosos
    disease gnosis knowledge) is the denial
    of blindness in patients who have irrefutably
    lost their vision. Whether they walk into the
    walls under their own stream or are bluntly
    confronted with their disability by you or
    others, their response is indifference, am
    implausible explanation (e.g., I lost my
    glasses), or both.

31
  • Antons syndrome is distinct from phantom vision,
    in which enucleated patients or those with
    severed optic nerves claim to perceive light and
    shape. The existence of visual subception
    suggests two dissociable levels of seeing,
    instrumental behavioral responses to optical
    stimuli on the one hand, and subjective visual
    awareness on the other. These patients do not
    readily volunteer their experiences of seeing
    things. In fact, they feel ashamed at
    experiencing something that intellectually they
    know to be not real.

32
  • For example, after fifty-year-old patient ER had
    her left eye enucleated because of a melanoma,
    she began to see geometric figures in that eye.
    The shapes were external, close to the face, and
    consisted of metabolic blobs, spirals, and
    parallel lines. Her explanation was, This is
    involuntary. My brain is doing it. (This claim
    is most plausible. We have long known that
    migraineurs who have no eyes nonetheless have
    visual symptoms during their aura.

33
  • The inverse of Antons syndrome has also been
    reported namely, the denial of visual
    perception in a man with remarkably preserved
    visual skills in his upper right quadrant. When
    confronted with his success in correctly
    identifying colors, objects, faces, facial
    emotions, and words, he denied any awareness of
    visual perception. His explanation for his
    accurate performance was, I feel it.

34
Visual Illusions and Hallucinations
35
  • Visual illusions and hallucinations may be either
    positive or negative symptoms and can be seen in
    drug reactions or withdrawal, delirium, epilepsy,
    and mass lesions. They may be elementary or
    formed. Epileptic discharges in Brodmann areas
    18 and 19 (visual association cortex) (lateral
    occipital) are said to cause twinkling or
    pulsating lights. Striate lesions produce
    elementary visual sensations of dark shapes,
    phosphenes, and flashes that may be stationary or
    moving, colored or achromatic. Red is perceived
    most often, followed by blue, green, and yellow.
    These visions may appear straight ahead or in the
    visual field opposite the legion.

36
  • Elementary flashes of light, zigzags, or other
    geometric shapes are called photisms, and are
    distinct from phosphenes, the flashing lights one
    sees on firmly rubbing the eyes. Phosphenes are
    caused by mechanical deformation of the retina
    and are an example of an entopic perception
    (literally within the eye). These latter need
    to be distinguished from perceptions of cerebral
    origin as the eye and the occiput are poles apart.

37
  • Other entopic phenomena are the seeing of ones
    own retinal blood vessels, vitreous floaters,
    afterimages, or the muscae volitantes. Entopic
    perceptions appear to move with the eyes, whereas
    perceptions of cerebral origin are independent of
    eye movement. Afterimages are produced by
    fatigued retinal photoreceptors and are
    complimentarily colored images of cerebral
    origin are often chromatic. Retinal vessels and
    floaters look like cobwebs or blobs, are fixed in
    appearance, and can be viewed or ignored at will.
    The muscae volitantes are the actual corpuscles
    coursing through vessels near the macula. You
    should normally be able to see your own,
    particularly against a bright sky or a field of
    snow.

38
  • They travel in lines or arcs, then disappear.
    Awareness of your own normal retinal circulation
    is called Scheerers phenomenon. Traction of the
    vitreous or retina causes arch-shaped, achromatic
    phosphenes called Moores lightning streaks.
    Macular edema or hemorrhage will produce
    distortions such as heat waves, and glaucoma
    causes halos and rainbows around objects.
    Certain maculopathies can also cause
    metamorphopsia.

39
  • Release hallucinations are perceptions, in any
    modality, that occur in a deafferented field.
    Visual and auditory ones are most common.
  • Visual release hallucinations can wander out into
    the normal field. They are experienced in
    extrapersonal space, and patients almost always
    appreciate their unreal nature. Elementary
    hallucinations tend to occur with occipital
    lesions, whereas formed and more complex ones
    emanate from the temporal lobe. Hallucinations
    due to temporal lobe lesions tend to fill both
    visual fields, in contrast to those engendered by
    occipital lesions, which usually inhabit only the
    contralateral field.

40
  • Elementary visual hallucinations have many
    causes, and the ground is strewn with confounds.
    In addition to entopic and occipital lobe causes,
    hallucinations can also result from lesions of
    the anterior optic tract. These are special,
    however, in that they are not spontaneous but
    rather are induced by another sense, most often
    sound. This is an example of an acquired
    synesthesia. Sounds that induced photisms in
    these patients included clanking of the radiator,
    crackling of the walls as they cooled at night,
    the whoosh of a furnace ignition, a dogs bark,
    and slamming doors.

41
  • The photisms ranged from simple flashes of white
    light to colored forms that looked like a flame,
    amoebas, oscillating flower petals, a spray of
    bright dots, or kaleidoscopic effects. All
    lasted for just an instant. The monocular
    visual-evoked response from the scotomatous eyes
    showed conduction delays and reduced amplitudes.
    It is curious that photisms arising in one eye
    are perceived to be caused by sounds reaching
    only the ipsilateral ear. This is, of course,
    contrary to our conventional understanding as
    acoustic localization depends on differences in
    the sound reaching both ears.

42
Metamorphopsia and Allied Experiences
43
  • Many bizarre visual experiences are subsumed
    under the term metamorphosia, the essential
    features of which are (1) deformation of shape,
    (2) change in size, (3) the illusion of movement,
    or (4) all three. Its locus is imprecise, being
    vaguely in the occipitotemporo-parietal territory.

44
  • In metamorphopsia, objects may appear to advance
    or recede relative to the viewer, their vertical
    or horizontal orientation may suddenly skew, or
    objects may break apart as if they were painted
    on glass, the parts sliding over one another as
    in Cubism. One object may transform into
    another, sometimes with the two shapes
    alternating rhythmically back and forth. Visual
    field defects are frequently found in all these
    subjective visual experiences. Specific types of
    metamorphopsiae have earned their individual
    terminology.

45
  • In micropsia and macropsia, objects seem too
    small or too big, respectively.
  • Umkehrtsehen is German for inverted vision,
    meaning that things look as if you are standing
    on your head. Verkehrtsehen denotes the reversal
    of right and left. These aberrations usually
    appear and resolve suddenly, and the experience
    is transitory in all recorded cases.

46
  • Palinopia (also known as paliopsia, paliopia, and
    palinopsia) is visual preservation. For example,
    a patient saw his wife leave the hospital room,
    and then he saw her leave again a few moments
    later. Aside from static cerebral lesions,
    antiserotonergics such as LSD and mescaline can
    produce it. LSD users speak of trails,
    positive images that remain immediately behind an
    object as it moves across their visual field.

47
  • Polyopia signifies multiple images of the same
    object. For example, a patient looked at a
    single rose and, on turning away to look at the
    blank wall, saw multiple roses. Insect vision,
    or entomopia (from the Greek entomon meaning
    insect), connotes rows and columns of multiple
    images numbering in the hundreds, as might be
    experienced by looking through compound eyes.
    Polyopia is a form of cerebral diplopia.

48
  • Cerebral diplopia (double vision) may be vertical
    or concentric. Strict terminology would limit it
    to only two images (the above terms of polyopia
    and entomopia taking care of larger iterations),
    yet triple impressions are especially common.
    Migraine is probably the most common cause of
    cerebral diplopia.

49
  • Monochromatopsia refers to illusory coloration,
    such as erythropsia (red) or xanthopsia (yellow).
    Vitreous hemorrhage causes the former and
    digitalis intoxication the latter more often than
    do cerebral lesions.

50
  • The term achromatopsia, indicating no color
    seeing, is somewhat misleading because patients
    do perceive some color. The chroma that they see
    is pale and desaturated, however, as in a
    television whose color is turned down. Patients
    also describe a spatial gap between the
    normally saturated field and the achromatic one.
    The responsible lesion is in unimodal visual
    association cortex.

51
Auditory Illusions and Hallucinations
52
  • The term paracusia refers to an alteration of
    volume, timbre, or some other distortion of
    sound. This may be both unpleasant and
    persistent. Unlike tinnitus and other auditory
    perceptions caused by end-organ disease,
    paracusiae are cerebral in origin. This is
    analogous to the distinction between entopic and
    cerebrally based visual hallucinations. Auditory
    hallucinations may be elementary or complex,
    ranging from humming and buzzing to music,
    voices, and radio programs.

53
  • Their cause is not limited to the temporal lobe,
    however. Ipsilateral musical hallucinations, for
    example, also result from lesions in the pontine
    tegmentum. That such brainstem hallucinations
    usually occur in the context of hearing loss
    suggests that they may be an instance of release
    hallucinations.

54
Olfactory and Gustatory Hallucinations
55
  • Olfactory hallucinations are customarily
    associated with mass lesions or epileptic
    discharges in the inferior and medial segments of
    the temporal lobe, especially the hippocampal
    convolution or uncus hence the name uncinate
    fits when referring to the disagreeable smell
    that sometimes constitutes the aura of partial
    seizures. It is externally projected and
    experienced as coming from some nearby but
    unknown source. The smell is impossible to
    identify other than being described as foul,
    rancid, or vile. (This quality of
    indescribableness is characteristic of experience
    associated with temporal structures.

56
  • Gustatory hallucinations also arise from the
    temporal lobe. Intense and sudden hunger can be
    a symptom of temporal lobe epilepsy (TLE), and,
    paradoxically, also of lobectomy. A boy was
    reported to have experienced two episodes of
    right anterior temporal intracerebral hemorrhage,
    each preceded by exclamations of intense hunger.

57
Synesthesia
58
  • The term synesthesia derives from the Greek syn
    (meaning union) and aisthesis (sensation),
    and refers to an involuntary joining of one or
    more senses. That is, perception in one sense is
    accompanied by a parallel perception in another
    sense. Synesthesias medical and psychological
    history reach back 300 years.
  • Idiopathic synesthesia is not a disorder per se,
    but a perceptual curiosity found in roughly 1 in
    25,000 individuals. Women outnumber men by at
    least two to one, and left-handedness or mixed
    dominance is more common than expected.

59
  • Through permutations of the five senses yield
    twenty possible pair-wise combinations, some
    synesthetic combinations are much more common
    than others. The yoking of sight with sound is
    by far most frequent, touch and taste less so,
    and smell is least often involved. In colored
    hearing synesthesia (chromesthesia), words,
    voices, environmental sounds, or music will
    trigger the perception of an involuntary photism
    that is perceived in extrapersonal space. In a
    case that I called geometric taste (i.e.,
    taste-touch synesthesia) the taste of mint caused
    subject MW to palpate a cold, smooth, curved
    shaped in front of him.

60
  • We often emphasize the yoking of just two senses
    when speaking of idiopathic synesthesia, though
    polymodal synesthesia occurs as well.
    Individuals mention that a third or fourth sense
    sometimes participates, but not as often as the
    main two that are joined. It is important to
    distinguish that synesthetic percepts are neither
    metaphoric nor pictorial. They are concrete,
    generic, and unelaborated. By generic, it is
    meant that synesthetes see blobs, lines, spirals,
    and lattice shapes palpate smooth or rough
    textures agreeable or disagreeable taste
    flavors, such as salty, sweet, or metallic.

61
  • They do not see pastures and temples, taste
    chicken soup, or feel a sponge or some other
    specified object. Sensations never go beyond
    this elementary, unembroidered level to become
    specific exemplars.
  • Acquired synesthesia is classically seen in TLE,
    head trauma, and mass lesions involving the
    medial temporal lobe. Synesthesia may also be
    induced by sensory deprivation, antiserotonergic
    hallucinogens such as LSD and peyote, or direct
    electrical stimulation of subcortical limbic
    structures.

62
Other Subjective Experiences Dependent on the
Temporal Lobe
63
  • Extremely unusual subjective perceptions have
    been recorded in cases of TLE and in patients
    with other known temporal lobe pathology.
  • Time dilation or time contraction occurs with
    lesions of either temporal lobe. Changes in the
    perception of time are certainly cognitive and,
    because they emerge following temporal-lesions
    one would expect to find them associated with
    other interesting neurological conditions.

64
  • Déjà vu and deja vecu mean already seen and
    already experienced (the latter literally means
    already lived), and refer to ones sense that
    something novel is extremely familiar or has been
    experienced exactly so at some past time. Jamais
    vu and jamais vecu refer to the opposite sense
    that a familiar setting feels nonetheless alien
    and strange.

65
  • The feeling of presence is not an uncommon
    experience, yet its occurrence rarely is
    volunteered for fear of negative judgment.
    Sensing a presence can occur in pathological
    states as well as be part of normal experience
    in the latter case, it is most often experienced
    late at night or at times of great creativity.

66
  • Other odd personality characteristics that are
    said to be typical of those who suffer from
    temporal lobe seizures are a heightened interest
    in religion and cosmic matters, the keeping of
    diaries or voluminous writings, a sense of
    portentousness or the attribution of heightened
    significance to otherwise mundane events, and a
    viscous or sticky personality.

67
Cortical Sensory Disturbances
68
  • Where we once were content to call the single
    post-central gyrus the somatosensory cortex, we
    now know that five somatosensory cortical areas
    exist in humans, each with its distinct
    somatotopic representation, cytoarchitecture, and
    projections.

69
  • Knowing what relative roles the various
    somesthetic cortices play in human sensation is
    another matter. At present, we can say that
    dorsomedial lesions produce severe anesthesia and
    apraxia that eventually improve considerably.
    Ventrolateral lesions, in contrast, produce
    tactile agnosia that remains detectable years
    after brain injury.

70
  • Pain, though less well understood than other
    sensory systems, also has multiple cortical
    representations. Painful heat, for example,
    activates contralateral anterior cingulum, S1,
    and S2, a pattern distinct from the predominant
    S1 activation caused by vibrotactile stimulation.
    Because the forebrain usually is thought to
    regulate emotion, the specific cingulate
    representation of pain is unexpected.

71
  • Categorization of sensory loss on clinical
    grounds is far less confusing. Patients with
    loss of one or more elementary sensations (touch,
    pain, temperature, vibration) in a
    face-arm-leg-trunk pattern have inferior-anterior
    parietal lesions involving the parietal operculum
    and insula. Theirs is a pseudothalamic sensory
    syndrome in that it mimics a lesion of lemniscal
    or spinothalamic projections. Other individuals
    who lose the ability to discriminate sensation
    (stereognosis, graphesthesia, proprioception)
    have superior-posterior parietal lesions. Theirs
    is called a cortical sensory syndrome.

72
  • A somatic hallucination that bears a superficial
    resemblance to release hallucinations is
    alloesthesia, a condition in which a noxious
    sensory stimulus given on one side of the body
    (where a sensory deficit exists) is perceived at
    the corresponding locus on the opposite side.
    This aberration occurs with lesions in the
    putamen and spinal cord, represents an elementary
    disturbance of sensory pathways, and is not to be
    mistaken for a higher cortical dysfunction.
    Segmental sensation or pain can also be referred
    to a different and quite distant dermatome via
    spinal mechanisms.

73
Asomatognosia
74
  • Asomatognosia is a disturbance of
    awareness/knowledge about ones own body and
    bodily conditions, the perception of ones own
    physical self, the relations of its parts to the
    whole, and its orientation and extension into
    extrapersonal space. One form would be asymbolia
    for pain in which normal reactions to painful
    stimuli are not evidenced or autotopagnosia, the
    inability to localize, name, or orient correctly
    different body parts

75
Asomatognosia/Autotopagnosia
  • Fails to point to correct body part upon command
  • Fails to correctly imitate body positions,
    postures or movements
  • Fails to correctly name body parts
Write a Comment
User Comments (0)
About PowerShow.com