Title: Conversational Skills in Children With
1Conversational Skills in Children With Without
Cochlear ImplantsFara E. Ditkowsky, M.A., C.F.
Katrina Fulcher M.A., C.F.
-
- DISCUSSION
- Children with (CI) employ more invitation
questions than follow-up questions. Invitation
questions encourage the conversational partner to
share information on a previously established
topic, whereas a follow-up question allows for
elaboration. - Children with CI also employed more repair
questions, meant to request a repetition or
clarification of a previously stated utterance. - Several instances of overlapping speech were
observed between the children with cochlear
implants, while no evidence of overlapping speech
was seen in the typically developing dyad. - LIMITATIONS,
- CLINICAL IMPLICATIONS
- FUTURE RESEARCH
- Limitations of this research study include
small dyad number, short conversational sample
length. A less naturalistic conversation task
construction was facilitated. - Clinical implications of this research study
focus on conversational flexibility via
therapeutic/diagnostic implications. For children
who have deficits in conversational ability,
learning how to be flexible communication
partners is crucial. - Future research in the area of conversational
skills, especially regarding cochlear implant
recipients, should focus on conversational skills
in naturalistic environments. - In addition, it would be beneficial to conduct a
similar study using longer conversational samples
with extended utterance production to analyze
more variables and provide a more holistic view
of conversation.
BACKGROUND
- Pragmatic language skills, including
conversational skills, support childrens ability
to establish and maintain acceptance into peer
groups (Schley Snow, 1992). - Pragmatics and conversational skills of children
with cochlear implants have received limited
empirical examination (Lloyd at al., 2007). - Analyses of conversations between adults and
children with cochlear implants (CI) reveal that
children - may be less likely to understand when a
conversational breakdown has occurred (Toe,
2008), - are more likely to provide inadequate or
unrelated information within a conversation
(Elfenbein, Hardin-Jones, Davis, 1994), - tend to produce more comments than questions,
- are more likely to initiate turns by behaviorally
touching (McKirdy Bank, 1982), and may rely on
simple responses (Toe et al, 2007). - Whereas these differences have been documented in
adult to child conversations (Blum-Kulka,
Huck-Taglicht, Anvi, 2004), much less is known
about the conversational skills of children with
CI in peer-to-peer interactions.
RESULTS
Typically Developing Conversational Data Analysis
Cochlear Implant Conversational Data Analysis
- PURPOSE
- To examine peer-to-peer conversational skills in
school-aged children with CI and compare these
skills to the interactions of typically
developing school-age children. - Specific aims included
- To determine the ratio of comments to questions
that exhibit contingency, - To analyze the types of questions produced within
the interaction, - To quantify the number of interruptions and topic
shifts.
Chart 2 Data analysis (averaged) for the
children with cochlear implants conversational
dyad.
Chart 1 Data analysis (averaged) for the
typically developing children conversational
dyad.
EXPERIMENTAL TASKS
- A conversational topic picture card task was
created to elicit a minimally structured small
talk conversation. Topics included birthday,
favorite television show/movie, favorite holiday,
family, favorite toy, friends, bullying, and bee
sting. - A snack task was created to elicit a negotiation
situation meant to facilitate a longer, more
naturalistic utterance production
PARTICIPANTS
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
Typical Cochlear Implants
(n2)
(n2)
Age (Year Months) Gender (Boys/Girls)
Appreciation is extended to Dr. Geralyn Timler,
Dr. Jeff Higginbotham, Dr. Kristi Buckley,
Gretchen Swartzenberg and the rest of the staff
at the University at Buffalo for their guidance
and knowledgeable input. A special thank you is
extended to the children and their families that
participated in this study.
65/610 95/910 1/1
1/1
The children with cochlear implants were
bilaterally implanted between 10-60. The
following standardized measures were obtained to
determine that each child had sufficient speech
and language skills to be an appropriate
candidate for this study Test of Narrative
Language (TNL Gillam Pearson, 2004), the
Kaufman Brief Intelligence Test - Second Edition
(KBIT-2 Kaufman, A. Kaufman N., 2006),
Childrens Communication Checklist-2 (CCC-2
Bishop, 2006) and The Screening Instrument for
Targeting Educational Risk (S.I.F.T.E.R.
Anderson, 1989). Scores were required to fall
within normal limits
CONTACT INFORMATION
Chart 4 Overlapping speech produced by both
participant populations. Numbers presented
represent the frequency of occurrences.
Chart 3 Comparison of the varied question
functions between the typically developing
children and children with cochlear implants.
Numbers presented represent the frequency of
occurrences.
References and a handout copy of this poster
are available from Fara Ditkowsky at
fditkowsky_at_aol.com or Katrina Fulcher at
katrina.fulcher_at_gmail.com. Please contact Fara
and/or Katrina with any further questions and
comments.
Utterances were transcribed using the Systematic
Analysis of Language Transcription (S.A.L.T.
Miller Chapman, 1986), and organized by subject
code. Reliability measures were conducted for
both general transcription and coding procedures.