Title: Culture, Learning, and the Chore Curriculum
1Culture, Learning, and the Chore Curriculum
- David F. Lancy
- Program in Anthropology
2- Among well-educated families throughout the
modern world, there is the compelling directive
to stimulate infants intellectually to
appropriately scaffold their burgeoning cognitive
development. Such stimulation continues in early
childhood and segues into formal instruction in
pre-school. Scientific and applied literature in
human development explicitly supports this
perspective and warns of adverse consequences to
the child of under-stimulation. This presentation
will survey a wide body of work among pre-modern
societies which fails to uncover evidence of
infant or childhood stimulation or instruction
and fails to affirm western benchmarks for
cognitive development. In more recent
researchcarried out in a cross section of
agrarian and foraging societiesthere is little
compelling evidence that childhood is a period of
intense skill acquisition and practice. On the
contrary, children acquire adult competencies at
a very slow, episodic pace, they learn them
through play and through social learning. Folk
theories of childhood stress the folly of trying
to accelerate development or instruct children
who dont yet have any sense.
3(No Transcript)
4(No Transcript)
5(No Transcript)
6Taekyo
7Invisible Babies
- Birth Huts
- Seclusion
- Cradleboard
- Swaddling
- Manta pouch
- Silence well-being
8swaddling immobilizes the child. Parents can
hang the bound infant up on a nail and go about
their business, secure in the know-ledge that he
cannot crawl into the fireplace or fall down a
well. A swaddled baby, like a little turtle in
its shell, could be looked after by another, only
slightly older child without too much fear of
injury, since the practice of swaddling
madechild care virtually idiot proof.Calvert,
Karin. 1992. Children in the House The Material
Culture of Early Childhood, 1600-1900. Boston,
MA Northeastern University Press.
Swaddling
9Uzbek Cradle
10Ideal Quiet Child
Among the Punan Bahthe baby ishardly
considered human the child is like an unripe
fruit, it must ripen, only then will you
know the taste of it. Nicolaisen, Ida. 1988.
Concepts and learning among the Punan Bah of
Sarawak, In Acquiring Culture Cross cultural
studies in child Development, Edited by Gustav
Jahoda and Ioan Lewis, (pp. 193-221). London, UK
Croom Helm.
11(No Transcript)
12(Denver, Colorado)
Training Toddlers to Become Students
- Executive functions (EF), also called cognitive
con-trol, are critical for success in school and
life. Poor and minority children lack these
skills but they can be taught. Core EF skills
are
- (1) inhibitory control (resisting habits,
temptations, or distractions), (2) working memory
(mentally holding and using information), and (3)
cognitive flexibility (adjusting to change). - Diamond, Adele, Barnett, W. Steven, Thomas,
Jessica, and Munro, Sarah 2007. Preschool program
improves cognitive control. Science,
3181387-1388.
13 China
Ghana
14(Gbarngasuakwelle, Liberia)
They are Un-educable
On Ifaluk Island, before the age of two, children
have no thoughts or feelings they just eat and
play. Since they lack sense or morals, it is
useless to get angry or to try and control their
behavior. Children do not acquire adult-like
intelligence repiyuntil they are about five or
six years old. Burrows, Edwin G. and Shapiro,
Melford E. 1957. An Atoll culture. Westport, CT
Greenwood Press.
15(Liberia and Malawi)
The 5-7 Transition
- From the ethnographic record the notion emerges
that children, before the age of five, roughly,
arent worth teaching because they are too
immature to really absorb important lessons. It
isnt until ages 5-7 that children really begin
to tune their radar to the world of adult
competencies and to begin to emulate their
parents and more competent older siblingsin
earnestand not just in make-believe. - Weisner, Thomas. 1996. The 5-7 transition as an
ecocultural project. In The Five to Seven Year
Shift Reason and Responsibility The Passage
Through Childhood, Edited by A.J. Samaroff and M.
M. Haith, (pp. 295-326). Chicago University of
Chicago Press.
16 (Fish Market Hodeidah, Yemen)
Chore Curriculum-
Marketing
17(Liberia)
Chore Curriculum-
Marketing
(Mexico)
18(Local market Otavalo, Ecuador)
Chore Curriculum-
Marketing
19Chore Curriculum-Farming
- Among the Bamana in Mali four-year-old Bafin
has already grasped the meaning of sowing and is
able to perform the various movementshe is
entrusted with an old hoe as well as with some
seeds so that he can gain some practice in this
activity. Howeverhe has to be allocated a
certain part of the field where he neither gets
in the way of the others nor spoils the rows they
have already sown. Also, the others have to keep
an eye on his attempts and point out his mistakes
to him from time to time. As a rule, his rows
have to be re-sown. At harvest..three-year-old
Daolebegins to pluck beans from the tendrils.
After he has filled the lid with a handful of
beans, his interest fades. He carelessly leaves
the lid with the beans lying on the ground and
goes looking for some other occupationFive year
old Sumaèlalooks out for a corner not yet
harvested and picks as many beans as will fill
his calabash.he keeps on doing this for more
than one and a half hours.Eleven year old Fase
has been busy harvesting beanssince morning. He
works as fast ashis father and grown-up
brotherand only takes a rest when they do.
Fase is a fully competentwith regard to
harvesting beans. He even takes on the role of
supervising his younger brothers and checks their
performance from time to time. - Polak, Barbara. 2003. Little peasants on the
importance of reliability in child labour, In Le
travail en Afrique noire Représentations et
Pratiques à lépoque Contermporaine. Edited by
DAlmeida-Topor, Hèléne, Monique Lakroum, and
Gerd Spittler, (pp. 125-136). Paris, France
Karthala.
20(Niger)
Chore Curriculum-Herding
(Ladakh)
21(Mary, Turkmenistan)
Chore Curriculum-Herding
- Spittler, Gerd. 1998. Hirtenarbeit. Köln Rüdiger
Köppe
22(Lake Issyk Kul, Kyrgyzstan)
Chore Curriculum-Crafts
23(Khiva)
Chore Curriculum-Crafts
(Bokhara, Uzbekistan)
- Crown, Patricia 2001. Learning to make pottery in
the Prehispanic American Southwest. - Journal of Anthropological Research 57(4).
24(Western China)
Chore Curriculum-Crafts
(India)
25(Kewa Tribe, Papua New Guinea)
Task Demands-Time Frame
- Among Kewa horticulturalists, children are
competent gardeners by 9. - Mer Island children are fairly proficient reef
foragers by 6. - In Tibet, mixed herds tended by 6-7 year-olds.
- 10 year old Aka pygymies have mastered some 50
foraging skills. - Ache forest dwellers can recognize a kuere or
signs of human or animal trails by age 8. - Ache hunters do not become fully proficient until
their 30s or 40s. - Mongongo nut processing efficiency peaks in the
30s for women living in the Okavango delta.
26Implications for Schooling
27School vs. work
- The Touaregs refuse to send their children to
school even though attendance is compulsory. Some
even go so far as to purchase potions from the
shaman to make them appear stupid. Others claim
that only poor parents without the means to
employ children in herding or fruit harvesting
send them to school. - Spittler, Gerd. 1998. Hirtenarbeit. Köln
Rüdiger Köppe - Among the Lowland Mayathere is currently almost
no opportunity to work in jobs that require an
education, and parents would benefit little from
investing in their childrens educationThe
limited amount of training necessary to be
successful at maize production coupled with the
unavailability of skill-based wage labor result
in a low payoff to parents to forgo their
childrens work and formally educate or train
them. - Kramer, Karen L. 2002. Variation in juvenile
dependence Helping behavior among Maya children.
Human Nature 13(2) 299-325. - Pandya describes the Indian government's
institution of a public school in an Ongee
Andaman Island village. In a multi-year
account, we learn how the community struggles to
find something useful or meaningful in the
curriculum that's been imported in toto from
Hindu-speaking, urban India. After twenty years
five boys who had been in the school off and on
for about eight yearscould not read or write
The villagers are left completely without
recourse as the foresttheir home and their
livelihoodis rapidly cut in large-scale logging.
- Pandya, Vishvanjit 2005. Deforesting among
Adamanese Children. In Barry S. Hewlett and
Michael E. Lamb, Hunter Gatherer Childhoods
Evolutionary, Developmental, and Cultural
Perspectives. Pp. 385-406. New Brunswick, NJ
AldineTransaction.
28Implications for Cognitive Development
- Argument advanced that most of the cognitive
skills proposed by western psychologists, like
Piaget, are not at all universal. Instead, these
skills are promoted by schooling and the
academically-oriented, information-rich child
rearing practiced in bourgeoisie Euromerican and
East Asian societies. Where non-western societies
promote the mastery of large bodies of
informationsuch as a foragers knowledge of wild
food sourcesthis would provide a similar press
to cognitive development.
29Conclusion
- Predominant theory and popular opinion regarding
child development is entirely dependent on a
single core assumption. The assumption is that
childhood is a period during which an enormous
volume of critical information and skill must be
acquired. But that assumption is tenable only
where formal, academic education is highly valued
and where families, communities, and governments
willingly accept the enormous costs entailed. In
societies where childhood is dominated by the
chore curriculum, the nature of development,
especially cognitive development, will be quite
different.