Culture, Learning, and the Chore Curriculum - PowerPoint PPT Presentation

1 / 28
About This Presentation
Title:

Culture, Learning, and the Chore Curriculum

Description:

Culture, Learning, and the Chore Curriculum – PowerPoint PPT presentation

Number of Views:80
Avg rating:3.0/5.0
Slides: 29
Provided by: davidl176
Category:

less

Transcript and Presenter's Notes

Title: Culture, Learning, and the Chore Curriculum


1
Culture, Learning, and the Chore Curriculum
  • David F. Lancy
  • Program in Anthropology

2
  • Among well-educated families throughout the
    modern world, there is the compelling directive
    to stimulate infants intellectually to
    appropriately scaffold their burgeoning cognitive
    development. Such stimulation continues in early
    childhood and segues into formal instruction in
    pre-school. Scientific and applied literature in
    human development explicitly supports this
    perspective and warns of adverse consequences to
    the child of under-stimulation. This presentation
    will survey a wide body of work among pre-modern
    societies which fails to uncover evidence of
    infant or childhood stimulation or instruction
    and fails to affirm western benchmarks for
    cognitive development. In more recent
    researchcarried out in a cross section of
    agrarian and foraging societiesthere is little
    compelling evidence that childhood is a period of
    intense skill acquisition and practice. On the
    contrary, children acquire adult competencies at
    a very slow, episodic pace, they learn them
    through play and through social learning. Folk
    theories of childhood stress the folly of trying
    to accelerate development or instruct children
    who dont yet have any sense.

3
(No Transcript)
4
(No Transcript)
5
(No Transcript)
6
Taekyo
7
Invisible Babies
  • Birth Huts
  • Seclusion
  • Cradleboard
  • Swaddling
  • Manta pouch
  • Silence well-being

8
swaddling immobilizes the child. Parents can
hang the bound infant up on a nail and go about
their business, secure in the know-ledge that he
cannot crawl into the fireplace or fall down a
well. A swaddled baby, like a little turtle in
its shell, could be looked after by another, only
slightly older child without too much fear of
injury, since the practice of swaddling
madechild care virtually idiot proof.Calvert,
Karin. 1992. Children in the House The Material
Culture of Early Childhood, 1600-1900. Boston,
MA Northeastern University Press.
Swaddling
9
Uzbek Cradle
10
Ideal Quiet Child
Among the Punan Bahthe baby ishardly
considered human the child is like an unripe
fruit, it must ripen, only then will you
know the taste of it. Nicolaisen, Ida. 1988.
Concepts and learning among the Punan Bah of
Sarawak, In Acquiring Culture Cross cultural
studies in child Development, Edited by Gustav
Jahoda and Ioan Lewis, (pp. 193-221). London, UK
Croom Helm.
11
(No Transcript)
12
(Denver, Colorado)
Training Toddlers to Become Students
  • Executive functions (EF), also called cognitive
    con-trol, are critical for success in school and
    life. Poor and minority children lack these
    skills but they can be taught. Core EF skills
    are
  • (1) inhibitory control (resisting habits,
    temptations, or distractions), (2) working memory
    (mentally holding and using information), and (3)
    cognitive flexibility (adjusting to change).
  • Diamond, Adele, Barnett, W. Steven, Thomas,
    Jessica, and Munro, Sarah 2007. Preschool program
    improves cognitive control. Science,
    3181387-1388.

13
China
Ghana
14
(Gbarngasuakwelle, Liberia)
They are Un-educable
On Ifaluk Island, before the age of two, children
have no thoughts or feelings they just eat and
play. Since they lack sense or morals, it is
useless to get angry or to try and control their
behavior. Children do not acquire adult-like
intelligence repiyuntil they are about five or
six years old. Burrows, Edwin G. and Shapiro,
Melford E. 1957. An Atoll culture. Westport, CT
Greenwood Press.
15
(Liberia and Malawi)
The 5-7 Transition
  • From the ethnographic record the notion emerges
    that children, before the age of five, roughly,
    arent worth teaching because they are too
    immature to really absorb important lessons. It
    isnt until ages 5-7 that children really begin
    to tune their radar to the world of adult
    competencies and to begin to emulate their
    parents and more competent older siblingsin
    earnestand not just in make-believe.
  • Weisner, Thomas. 1996. The 5-7 transition as an
    ecocultural project. In The Five to Seven Year
    Shift Reason and Responsibility The Passage
    Through Childhood, Edited by A.J. Samaroff and M.
    M. Haith, (pp. 295-326). Chicago University of
    Chicago Press.

16
(Fish Market Hodeidah, Yemen)
Chore Curriculum-
Marketing
17
(Liberia)
Chore Curriculum-
Marketing
(Mexico)
18
(Local market Otavalo, Ecuador)
Chore Curriculum-
Marketing
19
Chore Curriculum-Farming
  • Among the Bamana in Mali four-year-old Bafin
    has already grasped the meaning of sowing and is
    able to perform the various movementshe is
    entrusted with an old hoe as well as with some
    seeds so that he can gain some practice in this
    activity. Howeverhe has to be allocated a
    certain part of the field where he neither gets
    in the way of the others nor spoils the rows they
    have already sown. Also, the others have to keep
    an eye on his attempts and point out his mistakes
    to him from time to time. As a rule, his rows
    have to be re-sown. At harvest..three-year-old
    Daolebegins to pluck beans from the tendrils.
    After he has filled the lid with a handful of
    beans, his interest fades. He carelessly leaves
    the lid with the beans lying on the ground and
    goes looking for some other occupationFive year
    old Sumaèlalooks out for a corner not yet
    harvested and picks as many beans as will fill
    his calabash.he keeps on doing this for more
    than one and a half hours.Eleven year old Fase
    has been busy harvesting beanssince morning. He
    works as fast ashis father and grown-up
    brotherand only takes a rest when they do.
    Fase is a fully competentwith regard to
    harvesting beans. He even takes on the role of
    supervising his younger brothers and checks their
    performance from time to time.
  • Polak, Barbara. 2003. Little peasants on the
    importance of reliability in child labour, In Le
    travail en Afrique noire Représentations et
    Pratiques à lépoque Contermporaine. Edited by
    DAlmeida-Topor, Hèléne, Monique Lakroum, and
    Gerd Spittler, (pp. 125-136). Paris, France
    Karthala.

20
(Niger)
Chore Curriculum-Herding
(Ladakh)
21
(Mary, Turkmenistan)
Chore Curriculum-Herding
  • Spittler, Gerd. 1998. Hirtenarbeit. Köln Rüdiger
    Köppe

22
(Lake Issyk Kul, Kyrgyzstan)
Chore Curriculum-Crafts
23
(Khiva)
Chore Curriculum-Crafts
(Bokhara, Uzbekistan)
  • Crown, Patricia 2001. Learning to make pottery in
    the Prehispanic American Southwest.
  • Journal of Anthropological Research 57(4).

24
(Western China)
Chore Curriculum-Crafts
(India)
25
(Kewa Tribe, Papua New Guinea)
Task Demands-Time Frame
  • Among Kewa horticulturalists, children are
    competent gardeners by 9.
  • Mer Island children are fairly proficient reef
    foragers by 6.
  • In Tibet, mixed herds tended by 6-7 year-olds.
  • 10 year old Aka pygymies have mastered some 50
    foraging skills.
  • Ache forest dwellers can recognize a kuere or
    signs of human or animal trails by age 8.
  • Ache hunters do not become fully proficient until
    their 30s or 40s.
  • Mongongo nut processing efficiency peaks in the
    30s for women living in the Okavango delta.

26
Implications for Schooling
27
School vs. work
  • The Touaregs refuse to send their children to
    school even though attendance is compulsory. Some
    even go so far as to purchase potions from the
    shaman to make them appear stupid. Others claim
    that only poor parents without the means to
    employ children in herding or fruit harvesting
    send them to school.
  • Spittler, Gerd. 1998. Hirtenarbeit. Köln
    Rüdiger Köppe
  • Among the Lowland Mayathere is currently almost
    no opportunity to work in jobs that require an
    education, and parents would benefit little from
    investing in their childrens educationThe
    limited amount of training necessary to be
    successful at maize production coupled with the
    unavailability of skill-based wage labor result
    in a low payoff to parents to forgo their
    childrens work and formally educate or train
    them.
  • Kramer, Karen L. 2002. Variation in juvenile
    dependence Helping behavior among Maya children.
    Human Nature 13(2) 299-325.
  • Pandya describes the Indian government's
    institution of a public school in an Ongee
    Andaman Island village. In a multi-year
    account, we learn how the community struggles to
    find something useful or meaningful in the
    curriculum that's been imported in toto from
    Hindu-speaking, urban India. After twenty years
    five boys who had been in the school off and on
    for about eight yearscould not read or write
    The villagers are left completely without
    recourse as the foresttheir home and their
    livelihoodis rapidly cut in large-scale logging.
  • Pandya, Vishvanjit 2005. Deforesting among
    Adamanese Children. In Barry S. Hewlett and
    Michael E. Lamb, Hunter Gatherer Childhoods
    Evolutionary, Developmental, and Cultural
    Perspectives. Pp. 385-406. New Brunswick, NJ
    AldineTransaction.

28
Implications for Cognitive Development
  • Argument advanced that most of the cognitive
    skills proposed by western psychologists, like
    Piaget, are not at all universal. Instead, these
    skills are promoted by schooling and the
    academically-oriented, information-rich child
    rearing practiced in bourgeoisie Euromerican and
    East Asian societies. Where non-western societies
    promote the mastery of large bodies of
    informationsuch as a foragers knowledge of wild
    food sourcesthis would provide a similar press
    to cognitive development.

29
Conclusion
  • Predominant theory and popular opinion regarding
    child development is entirely dependent on a
    single core assumption. The assumption is that
    childhood is a period during which an enormous
    volume of critical information and skill must be
    acquired. But that assumption is tenable only
    where formal, academic education is highly valued
    and where families, communities, and governments
    willingly accept the enormous costs entailed. In
    societies where childhood is dominated by the
    chore curriculum, the nature of development,
    especially cognitive development, will be quite
    different.
Write a Comment
User Comments (0)
About PowerShow.com