Title: Pain in Fish
1Pain in Fish?
- Jennie Lofgren DVM, MS
- MIT Division of Comparative Medicine
- 2/1/08
2Overview
- Why does it matter?
- Approaches to discussion
- Neuroanatomical
- Physiological
- Behavior
- Current guidelines and recommendations.
- Conclusion
- References
3Why does it matter?
- Roughly 2.8 million fish are used for research
and education in the United States each year. - Fish represent nearly 25 of all animals used for
research and education in North America only
mice are used in higher numbers.
Grey and Vincent, 2006.
4 Why does it matter?
- The number of fish being used in research is
increasing due to - Ethical awareness phylogenetically lower
species - replacement for warm-blooded animals.
- Compared to transgenic mice, inexpensive and easy
to maintain. - Ideal species in which to study carcinogenesis,
renal and cardiac regeneration, environmental
toxicology and proteomics.
Borski and Hodson, 2003.
5Why does it matter?
- Zebrafish have a well characterized genome.
- According to the Foundation for Biomedical
Research, Zebrafish are one of the most promising
models for studying early vertebrate development
and gene function.
Grey and Vincent, 2006.
6Why does it matter?
- Common techniques used in research with fish
include fin, gill, or organ biopsies, skin
scrapings, and tissue or cell implantation. - With the increasing use of fish, IACUCs have
begun to struggle with the question of pain and
distress in fish and what they should require of
investigators.
7Approach to Discussion
- The academic debate over whether fish experience
pain is well documented. - Recent literature can be divided into three main
approaches - Neuroanatomical
- Physiological
- Behavioral
8Neuroanatomical
- Fish share some but not all of the
neuroanatomical structures utilized in human pain
perception. - Does this then mean that they cannot feel pain?
9Neuroanatomical
- 2 main aspects of pain perception
- Nociception conversion of a noxious stimulus
into an electrical impulse appreciated at the
spinal level and forwarded to the brain. - Conscious and emotional appreciation of pain
providing a suffering component.
10Nociception
- C fiber neurons unmyelinated
- long-lasting "noxious pain"
- associated with longer-term, dull, aching,
throbbing pain. - A delta fiber neurons myelinated
- immediate "adaptive pain," such as that
experienced milliseconds after slamming your
fingers in a door. - Both of these fiber types were identified in the
trigeminal nerve of trout.
Sneddon, Braithwaite, and Gentle. 2003a, 2003b.
11Neuroanatomical
Primary sensory cortex
Limbic system, which includes the amygdala.
Thalamus
Second-order nocioceptive neuron that crosses the
midline and ascends up the spinothalamic tract
http//www.mayoclinicproceedings.com/inside.asp?AI
D795UID
Nocioceptive fibers (A and C) cell bodies are
in the Dorsal root ganglion (DRG)
12Neuroanatomy a tale of 2 theories
13Neuroanatomy a tale of 2 theories
- Theory One
- Consciousness is a prerequisite for pain
suffering. - Consciousness requires a neocortex.
- Fish lack a neocortex, therefore they are
incapable of consciousness and cannot appreciate
pain nor suffer.
Neocortex
Rose, 2002, 2007.
14Neuroanatomy a tale of 2 theories
Forebrain (cerebrum, telencephalon)
Hind brain (brainstem)
Mid brain (diencephalon)
Rose, 2002.
15Neuroanatomy theory 1
- Nightmare (human)
- elevated heart and respiratory rates
- increased blood pressure
- release of stress hormones (cortisol)
- dilated pupils
- may even scream or thrash
- Not conscious- cannot be in pain nor suffer.
- How would an IRB approach a study that, as a
sequelae, induced nightmares? - Would an intervention be preferential if it would
prevent the nightmare ?
Rose. 2002.
16Neuroanatomy a tale of 2 theories
- Theory 2 3 parts.
- First Homologous structures with mammalian
neuroanatomy. - Fish telencephalon with mammalian limbic system.
- Limbic system anticipation, expectation, and
ultimately goal directed activity. - Amygdala (component of the limbic system)
emotional response and memory. - Only brain component consistently demonstrated to
respond during conscious experience of pain in
humans as demonstrated with imaging studies.
Chandroo, 2004a, 2004b.
17Neuroanatomy a tale of 2 theories
18Neuroanatomy a tale of 2 theories
- Theory 2
- Second
- Dynamic core hypothesis
- Consciousness is not the function of a single
structure with strict neuroanatomical boundaries - Consciousness is generated through shifting
functional connectivity of neurons.
Chandroo, 2004b.
19Neuroanatomy a tale of 2 theories
- Theory 2
- Third
- Neuroanatomical development in fish is extremely
different from that of humans - The ability to definitively identify structures
responsible for consciousness in fish have yet to
be determined.
Rodríguez, Broglio, Durán, Gómez, and Salas.
2006. Broom. 2007.
20Neuroanatomy a tale of 2 theories
- Third
- Ex
- mammalian neural tube folds in on itself
- the bony fish neural tube folds out
- in comparison to a mammalian brain, the neural
structures of the fish forebrain are reversed in
order - Ex
- fish brains put greater emphasis on different
stimulus that mammals (lateral line vs. sight) - change in priorities is reflected in the
composition of the brain.
Rodríguez, Broglio, Durán, Gómez, and Salas.
2006. Broom. 2007.
21Neuroanatomy
- One definitive statement made by The Fisheries
Society of the British Isles. - The lack of a neocortex does not mean that fish
cannot experience some kind of suffering. Recent
studies suggest that fish have the capacity to
perceive painful stimuli and that these are
strongly aversive states that we associate with
pain and emotional distress.
Fisheries Society of the British Isles. 2002.
22Physiology
- Stress
- Neuroendocrine
- Response to exogenous drugs
- Anxiolytics
- Analgesics
23Stress
- Acute stress can an adaptive mechanism which aids
in survival - Chronic, unavoidable stress is detrimental
- leads to increased illness and generally poor
welfare
Huntingford. 2006.
24Stress
- Hypothalamic-pituitary-interrenal response
- almost identical to the mammalian
hypothalamic-adrenal-pituitary axis. - Elevated cortisol is an indicator of fish
welfare. - Measured
- invasively through a blood stick
- non-invasively in tank water or feces.
Iwama. 2007. Chandroo. 2004a. Huntingford. 2006.
25Stress
- Study
- Salmon conditioned to tank emptying
- demonstrated by decreasing amounts of secreted
cortisol - On transportation, the conditioned fish had a
higher survival rate than those that had not been
conditioned.
Chandroo. 2004a.
26Stress
- Chronic stress has been demonstrated in
subordinate fish. - More closely related to threat imposed by the
presence of the dominant fish rather than actual
aggressive encounters. - Appreciation of danger without having experienced
actual physical insult. - Demonstrates fear?
http//en.wikipedia.org/wiki/ImageGeorgia_Aquariu
m_-_Giant_Grouper_edit.jpg
Chandroo. 2004a.
27Neuroendocrine
- Substance P
- peptide found in mammals
- known to modulate and transmit pain
- identified via immunohistochemistry in the fish
brain. - FMRFamide
- peptide neurotransmitter found in mammals
- functions in analgesic and aversive responses
- identified in the trout brain.
Chandroo. 2004a.
28Neuroendocrine
- Serotonin
- In human brains is naturally produced in the
limbic system (mediates psychological and social
stress) - In fish, serotoninergic activation in the
telencephalon (homologous to the mammalian
limbic system) identified during socially
stressful events - being exposed to a dominant
fish.
Chandroo. 2004a.
29Dopamine
- Mammals influence learning and behavior
resulting from emotion - Fish exposure to dopamine agonists
- amphetamine (intracranial injection)
- apomorphine (immersion)
- demonstrated dose dependent changes in reward
behavior and reduced aggression
Chandroo. 2004a.
30Benzodiazepam
- Fish given anti-anxiety medications that bind to
benzodiazepine receptors in the brain secreted
less alarm pheromone.
Chandroo. 2004a.
31Opioids
- Opioid receptors have been located in the
zebrafish limbic structure. - Multiple studies have demonstrated intriguing
affects of opioid administration.
Chandroo. 2004a.
32Opioids Study 1
- Goldfish withstood increasing electric shocks as
morphine was added to their tank water. - Effect was reversed when opiate antagonists were
administered. - Criticism- possible that the goldfish were not
alleviated of pain but rather their mental
capacity was diminished in the presence of
opioids.
Ehrensing, Michell, Kastin . 1982.
33Opioids Study 2
- Carp given butorphanol or a sham dose IM
post-operatively - Sham dose
- displayed decreased activity
- lower in the water column
- decreased feeding intensity
- Butorphanol
- no significant alterations from pre-surgical
behavior i.e. retained normal behavior.
Sneddon. 2003a.
34Opioids Study 3
- Normal fish behavior neophobic
- Fish avoid a new object placed in the center of
tank - Fish administered acetic acid (an irritant)
- Spent more time closer to the novel item
- Avoidance behavior was greatly reduced
- Given morphine
- Regained normal avoidance behavior
Sneddon. 2003a.
35Behavior
- Asks fish to demonstrate desire to avoid pain
and distress through preference testing. - Insight into an animals priorities.
- Measure of motivational affected state.
- Assumes that given a choice between discomfort
and comfort, an animal will not freely choose
discomfort.
Volpato. 2007 Dunlop, Millsopp, Laming.
2006. Braithwaite, Boulcott. 2007.
36Behavior
- Are the fish are making a choice or just
responding reflexively? - Two studies in particular address this concern.
37Reflex vs. Choice Study 1
- Simulated predator presented when fish swam
across the median of the tank - Elicited a fast start response
- Fast start behavior is a documented reflex in
fish seen in feeding behavior and in response to
predators.
Chandroo. 2004a.
38Reflex vs. Choice Study 1
- Fish soon changed their behavior
- avoided the center of tank.
- if forced to swim through the center of the tank
fish adopted a zig-zag motion (evasive but not
reflexive maneuver). - Study conclusion fish anticipated a
frightening stimulus and developed an escape
response that was motivated by an affective state
of fear.
Chandroo. 2004a.
39Reflex vs. Choice Study 1
- If fish were only capable of a reflexive response
they would display repetitive behavior fast
start response over and over without the
possibility of behavior modification.
Chandroo. 2004a.
40Reflex vs. Choice Study 2
- Trout placed in a shuttle tank.
- Exposed to plunging net.
- elicited a fast start response (reflex) to
shuttle to the safe chamber. - A light was then shown prior to the net.
- After conditioning
- When shown the light, fish would shuttle at a
slower (non-reflex) pace to the safe
compartment.
Yue. 2004
41Reflex vs Choice Study 2
- Conclusions
- trout recognized the light as a neutral stimulus.
- presence of the light initiated behavior that
would allow avoidance of the negative stimulus,
the net - Author extrapolated that trout were less stressed
when allowed to adopt an effective strategy to
avoid negative stimuli.
Yue. 2004
42Behavior
- Study how intensely fish prefer or dislike a
given experience through - complex cognitive abilities
- stress induced changes in memory
Volpato. 2007.
43Cognitive Ability
- Trout and goldfish trained to associate areas of
a tank with increasing levels of electric shock. - Both species learned to avoid areas of the tank
associated with shock. - In 2nd part of the study, fish were shown a
conspecific in the area of the tank with greatest
shock.
Dunlop, Millsopp, Laming. 2006
44Cognitive Ability
- Trout chose to sustain mild electric shock to be
closer to their companion. - Goldfish chose to stay as close to the
conspecific as possible without sustaining any
shocks. - Neither chose to be in the area of the tank
furthest from both the shock and conspecific. - May demonstrate that fish can choose what level
of negative stimuli is acceptable given the
possibility of positive stimuli.
Dunlop, Millsopp, Laming. 2006
45Stress, Memory, and Behavior
- Declarative memories
- A memory with a specific significance attached to
it (positive or negative), can be recalled to
influence future behavior. - Behavior gives insight into the internal state of
the fish while it was experiencing a stimulus.
Chandroo. 2004 Yue. 2004.
46Stress, Memory, and Behavior
- Paradise fish
- recognize a given conspecific for 7 days
- encounter with a fish of another species is
remembered for 3 months - Carp
- pass up bait for up to 3 years after being hooked
just once.
Yue. 2004
47Stress, Memory, and Behavior
- Siamese Fighting Fish
- Shown a fight between two fish
- If then put in the same tank as the winner they
were less likely to engage in battle - If the put in the same tank as the loser were
more likely to engage in battle -
- Rainbow trout
- Recognize past opponents
- Will change their fighting behavior depending on
the outcome of the prior fight
Chandroo. 2004
48Behavior Studies
- Provides an alternative to the notion that fish
are merely responsive, passively reacting to
stimuli as they encounter them with little or no
ability for cognition.
Braithwaite, Boulcott. 2007.
49Current Guidelines
- On going debate regarding pain in fish
- Current guidelines are allusive and require
individual interpretation. - Fish are not covered under the Animal Welfare
Act.
Animal Welfare Act as Amended in United States
Code, Title 7, Sections 2131 to 2156. (1996).
50PHS Policy
- covers all vertebrate species
- Requires
- avoidance or minimization of discomfort,
distress, and pain when consistent with sound
scientific practices - Unless the contrary is established,
investigators should consider that procedures
that cause pain or distress in human beings may
cause pain or distress in other animals. - Procedures with animals that may cause more than
momentary or slight pain or distress should be
performed with appropriate sedation, analgesia,
or anesthesia
Office of Laboratory Animal Welfare of the
National Institutes of Health. PHS Policy on
Humane Care and Use of Laboratory Animals. Public
Health Service. p 27. (1986).
51American Fisheries Societies Guidelines for the
use of Fishes in Research (1988)
- Recommended
- application of a local anesthetic, such as
benzocaine, to any area that will undergo tissue
removal or modification - general anesthesia during prolonged restraint
American Fisheries Society (AFS), American
Society of Ichthyologists and Herpetologists
(ASIH), and American Institute of Fishery
Research Biologists (AIFRE). AFS Policy Statement
16, also published as Guidelines for Use of
Fishes in Field Research. Fisheries. 13(2),
16-23 (1988).
52American Fisheries Societies Guidelines for the
use of Fishes in Research (2002)
- Primarily cites the Rose 2002 paper as the basis
for discussion of pain. - Fish are capable of experiencing physiological
stress and nociception but not pain or suffering - Recommendations are focused on reducing
physiological stress through good husbandry
practices.
http//www.fisheries.org/afs/publicpolicy/guidelin
es2004.pdf (2004).
53European Regulations
- Fisheries Society of the British Isles
- Pain in fish is not only possible, but likely.
- Recommendations encompass good husbandry
practices and preventing or addressing
potentially painful stimuli. - According to an article discussing the German
animal welfare law, fish are capable of feeling
pain and they are able to suffer...
Fisheries Society of the British Isles. Fish
Welfare. Briefing 2. Granta Information Systems,
82A High Street, Swanston, Cambridge CB2 4H.
2002 Oidtmann, Hoffmann. 2001.
54Veterinary Texts
- Admit that there is no clear answer
- Recommend using analgesia as there is evidence
that it is possible that fish feel pain.
Brown. 1992 Machin. 2001
55Recommendations from Veterinary Texts
- Anesthetics
- Benzocaine, lidocaine, and tricaine
methanesulfonate (MS-222) in tank water - also provides localized analgesia
- recovery from anesthesia requires removal from
immersion - the analgesic effect is only present while the
fish is in the anesthetic solution
Machin. 2001 Carpenter. 2005.
56Recommendations from Veterinary Texts
- Morphine, dissolved in tank water, has
demonstrated analgesia in response to electric
shock. - Butorphanol has also been given intramuscularly
(IM) for post operative analgesia.
Machin. 2001 Carpenter. 2005
57A special note on opioids
- Fish are ectotherms.
- Study the same dose of morphine was increasingly
efficacious with decreasing temperature of the
fish. - As the fishs temperature drops, the blood pH
increased, which increased the amount of free
base drug. - Therefore, the temperature of the fish should be
considered when deciding on an appropriate dose.
Stevens, Balahura. 2007.
58Recommendations from Veterinary Texts
- Ketamine, alone or in combination with
medetomidine, have been known to provide both
anesthesia and analgesia in fish. - The medetomidine can be reversed with atipamezole
(Antisedan, Pfizer).
Carpenter. 2005
59Studies using analgesia
- Koi carp
- given 0.4mg/kg butorphanol IM intra-operatively
- experienced significantly reduced recovery time
compared to the control group (not given pain
medication).
Harms. 2005.
60Studies using analgesia
- Carp
- Ketoprofen at 2mg/kg IM intra-operatively
- significantly lower post-surgical creatine kinase
levels - did not have significantly different
post-operative recovery time compared to the
control group - Ketoprofen at this dose did not provide
sufficient analgesia.
Harms. 2005.
61Studies using analgesia
- Carp
- Tramadol at 10 and 100 nmol/g IM
- achieved statistically significant analgesia
compared to carp without pain medication - Analgesia measurable within 5-15 minutes
- Analgesic effect was stable for 1-2 hours.
- Naloxone at 100nmol/g blocked the analgesic
effect.
Chervona, Lapshin. 2000.
62Studies using analgesia
- Non-opioid analgesics effective in amphibians.
- Chlorpromazine, chlorodiazepoxide (a
benzodiazepine), and diphenhydramine (Benadryl)
were roughly 50 as affective as morphine. - NSAIDs such as indomethacin and ketorolac were
roughly 40 as effective as morphine. - May be useful in fish.
Stevens, Maciver, Newman. 2001.
63Table 1. Analgesic Agents in Fish
Machin. 2001., Carpenter. 2005.
64Summary
- Research using fish as animal models has
demonstrated great promise. - Their increasing use is bringing with it a need
to understand their experience as research
animals.
Hirschler. 2007. Snyder. 2006.
65Summary
- Minimizing pain and distress is an inherent
responsibility with animal use. - Fulfilling this responsibility is especially
difficult in a species so different from mammals. - Studies designed to answer the question of
whether fish feel pain and therefore require
analgesia fall into three main categories - the neuroanatomical capabilities of fish
- their physiological responses
- behavioral adaptations to stress and pain
66Summary Neuroanatomy
- Fish are capable of nociception.
- Do not possess a neocortex which some believe is
solely responsible and required for
consciousness. - Possible through homologous structures or several
structures acting in concert, that fish
experience some form of conscious pain.
67Summary neuroanatomy
- No definitive studies to determine if fish
possess the neuroanatomy necessary to consciously
perceive pain. - Advanced imaging and molecular techniques may
allow visually identification of changes in the
fish brain in response to noxious stimuli.
Huntingford, 2006. Braithwaite, Boulcott. 2007.
68Summary Physiology
- Fish share much of the same physiological
responses to stress and pain as mammals. - Most interestingly, fish seem to benefit from
post operative analgesia.
69Summary Behavior
- Demonstrated difference between reflexive
response and escape behavior from aversive
stimuli. - If fish have the choice to avoid danger or pain
they do so. - Ability to weigh positive and negative stimuli
and adjust their behavior accordingly.
70Conclusion
- It may not matter whether fish are able to
consciously experience pain in the human sense. - There are a myriad of studies that conclusively
demonstrated that stimulation that would be
painful in a mammal can be extremely aversive to
fish. - As well as evidence that fish seem to benefit
from pain relief.
71Conclusions
- It is the responsibility of scientists,
veterinary and animal care staff committed to
excellent stewardship and welfare of laboratory
animals to minimize experiences that would cause
pain and distress.
72References
- Diseases of Aquatic Organisms
- Special 2 Welfare of Aquatic Organisms
- May 2007.
- Open Access articles
- http//www.int-res.com/abstracts/dao/v75/n2/
73References
- 1. Grey, Melissa. Vincent, Amanda C.J. Extent
and possible conservation implications of fish
use for research, testing and education in North
America. Aquatic Conserv Mar. Freshw. Ecosyst.
16, 569578 (2006). - 2. Borski, Russell J. Hodson, Ronald G. Fish
Research and the Institutional Animal Care and
Use Committee. ILAR Journal. 44(4), 286-294
(2003). - 3. Rose, James D. Anthropomorphism and mental
welfare of fishes. Dis Aquat Org. 75, 139154
(2007). - 4. Iwama, George K. The welfare of fish. Dis
Aquat Org. 75, 155158 (2007). - 5. Volpato, Gilson Luiz. et al. Insights into
the concept of fish welfare. Dis Aquat Org.
75, 165171 (2007). - 6. Bekoff, Marc. Aquatic animals, cognitive
ethology, and ethics questions about sentience
and other troubling issues that lurk in turbid
water. Dis Aquat Org. 75, 8798 (2007). - 7. Sneddon LU, Braithwaite VA, Gentle MJ. Do
fish have nociceptors? Evidence for the evolution
of a vertebrate sensory system. Proc R Soc Lond
B. 270, 11151121 (2003a). - 8. Sneddon LU, Braithwaite VA, Gentle MJ. Novel
object test examining pain and fear in the
rainbow trout. J Pain. 4,431440 (2003b). - 9. Chandroo, K.P., et al. Can fish suffer?
perspectives on sentience, pain, fear and
stress. Applied Animal Behaviour Science. 86,
225-250 (2004a). - 10. Rose, JD. The neurobehavioral nature of
fishes and the question of awareness and pain.
Rev Fish Sci. 10, 138 (2002).
74References
- 11. Chandroo, K.P., et al. An evaluation of the
current perspectives on consciousness and pain in
fishes. Fish and Fisheries. 5, 281-295 (2004b). - 12. Rodríguez F, Broglio C, Durán E, Gómez A,
Salas C. Neural mechanisms of learning in
teleost fish. In Brown C, Laland K, Krause J
(eds). Fish cognition and behaviour. Blackwell,
Oxford, p 243277 (2006). - 13. D. M. Broom. Cognitive ability and
sentience Which aquatic animals should be
protected? Dis Aquat Org. 75, 99108 (2007). - 14. Fisheries Society of the British Isles. Fish
Welfare. Briefing 2. Granta Information Systems,
82A High Street, Swanston, Cambridge CB2 4H.
(2002) - 15. Iwama, George K. The welfare of fish. Dis
Aquat Org. 75, 155158 (2007). - 16. Huntingford, F.A., et al. Current issues in
fish welfare. Journal of Fish Biology. 68,
332372 (2006). - 17. Ehrensing RH, Michell GF, Kastin AJ. Similar
antagonism of morphine analgesia by MIF-1 and
naloxone in Carassius auratus. Pharmacol Biochem
Behav. 17, 757761 (1982). - 18. Volpato, Gilson Luiz , et al. Insights into
the concept of fish welfare. Dis Aquat Org.
75, 165171 (2007). - 19. Dunlop R, Millsopp S, Laming P. Avoidance
learning in goldfish (Carassius auratus) and
trout (Oncorhynchus mykiss) and implications for
pain perception. Appl Anim Behav Sci. 97,
255271 (2006). - 20. Braithwaite, V.A., Boulcott, P. Pain
perception, aversion and fear in fish. Dis Aquat
Org. 75, 131128 (2007).
75References
- 21. S. Yue, et al. Investigating fear in
domestic rainbow trout, Oncorhynchus mykiss,
using an avoidance learning task. Applied
Animal Behaviour Sciences. 87, 343-354 (2004). - 22. Animal Welfare Act as Amended in United
States Code, Title 7, Sections 2131 to 2156.
(1996). - 23. Office of Laboratory Animal Welfare of the
National Institutes of Health. PHS Policy on
Humane Care and Use of Laboratory Animals. Public
Health Service. p 27. (1986). - 24. American Fisheries Society (AFS), American
Society of Ichthyologists and Herpetologists
(ASIH), and American Institute of Fishery
Research Biologists (AIFRE). AFS Policy Statement
16, also published as Guidelines for Use of
Fishes in Field Research. Fisheries. 13(2),
16-23 (1988). - 25. American Fisheries Society (AFS), American
Society of Ichthyologists and Herpetologists
(ASIH), and American Institute of Fishery
Research Biologists (AIFRE). Guidelines for the
Use of Fishes in Research. http//www.fisheries.
org/afs/publicpolicy/guidelines2004.pdf (2004). - 26. Oidtmann B, Hoffmann RW. Schmerzen und
Leiden bei FischenPain and suffering in fish.
Berliner und Munchener Tierarztliche
Wochenschrift. 114(7-8), 277-82 (2001). (In
German with an English summary) - 27. Brown, Lydia. Anesthesia and Restraint. In
Stoskopf, Michael K. Fish Medicine. W.B. Saunders
Co, Philadelphia, p 79-90 (1992).
76References
- 28. Machin, Karen L. Fish, Amphibian, and
Reptile Analgesia. In Heard, Darryl J. The
Veterinary Clinics of North America Exotic
Animal Practice Analgesia and Anesthesia. W.B.
Saunders Co, Philadelphia, 4(1) p 19-33 (2001) - 29. Carpenter, James W. Exotic Animal Formulary
Third Edition. Elsevier Saunders, St. Louis, p
18-21 (2005). - 30. Harms, CA et al. Behavioral and clinical
pathology changes in koi carp (Cyprinus carpio)
subjected to anesthesia and surgery with and
without intra-operative analgesics. Comp Med.
55(3) 221-226 (2005). - 31. Chervona, L.S., Lapshin, D.N. opioid
Modulation of Pain Threshold in Fish. Doklady
Biological Sciences. 375, 590-591 (2000). - 32. Stevens, Don E., Balahura, Robert J. Aspects
of Morphine Chemistry Important to Persons
Working with Cold-blooded Animals, Especially
Fish. Comp Med. 57(2) 161-166 (2007). - 33. Stevens, Craig W., Maciver, Donald N.,
Newman, Leslie C. Testing and Comparison of
Non-Opioid Analgesics in Amphibians.
Contemporary Topics. 40(4) 23-27 (2001). - 34. Hirschler, Ben. Zebrafish study may point
way to blindness cure. Reuters.
http//uk.reuters.com/article/topNews/idUKL3139081
320070801. Wed 1 Aug 2007. - 35. Snyder, Alison. Secret of Heart Regeneration
Uncovered. ScientificAmerican.com.
http//www.sciam.com/article.cfm?articleIDAAFFD0D
5-E7F2-99DF-3C4DEA936E6D7CD6. November 02, 2006
77Questions?