Title: Integrating ICALL into synchronous CMC
1Integrating ICALL into synchronous CMC
- Markus Dickinson, Rebecca Sachs, Yunkyoung Kang,
Soojeong Eom, Chong Min Lee - CALICO/IALLT Conference
- March 20, 2008
- San Francisco, CA
2Intelligent CALL (ICALL)
- Intelligent CALL, using natural language
processing technology, provides many promising
means of facilitating L2 development - Detailed information about learners L2
production errors can foster awareness of
language and encourage cognitive comparisons - Feedback can be precisely tailored to learners
proficiency levels, common mistakes, activity
goals, learning styles, cognitive abilities, etc. - Specific improvements can be tracked across
exercises through the use of learner models -
- (Amaral Meurers, 2006 Heift Schulze, 2007
Nagata, 2002)
3Limitations of ICALL
- However, there is a tension between
- Contextualized language use is increasingly
emphasized in ICALL, and with increasing success - (e.g., Amaral et al., 2006 Amaral Meurers,
2006 Nagata, 2002) - But actual communicative interaction remains
relatively unexplored in systems capable of
providing feedback - (though see Petersen, 2006)
- In order to manage computational complexity
- Exercises are often restricted to the sentence
level - Activities often do not simulate true
communication
The ability of an ICALL system to provide
meaningful, accurate feedback
The flexibility an ICALL system allows for in
terms of meaningful, communicative interaction
4Synchronous CMC
- Synchronous CMC (computer-mediated communication)
between L2 learners can also provide beneficial
contexts for language development - Activities can approximate target tasks
relevant to real-life communicative situations - Learners can function as language users as
opposed to simply displaying language or
manipulating L2 forms systematically without
attending to meaning (Ellis, 2003) - Possibly optimal psycholinguistic environment
for several interactional features linked to L2
development (Doughty Long, 2003)
5Pros of CMC
- Potentially beneficial features
- Negotiation of lexical meaning (Blake, 2000)
- Attention to form, monitoring of linguistic
output, self-correction (Salaberry, 2000) - Incorporation of others feedback (Pellettieri,
2000) - Use of more complex language (Warschauer, 1996)
- Drawing of form-meaning connections (Doughty
Long, 2003) - Less pressing time constraints and less ephemeral
language in the written modality, reducing
processing demands (Payne Whitney, 2002) - Comfortable environment for expression of ideas
(Warschauer, 1996)
6Limitations of CMC
- Concerns regarding the quality of
learner-to-learner interactions The blind
leading the blind? - Without feedback from a trusted authority,
learners might - reinforce each others errors
- not have the resources necessary for correcting
each other - naturally tend to focus on vocab without
attending to grammar - (Blake, 2000 Kern, 1995)
- ? Teachers may need to set explicit expectations
for grammatically correct language, while
simultaneously trying to balance this with a
primary focus on meaningful communication (Lee,
2001 Pellettieri, 2000)
7Combining the benefits
- Can ICALL and synchronous CMC be integrated in a
way that exploits the unique benefits of each
while avoiding their limitations?
detailed, informative, individualized feedback
communicative purpose
negotiation and interactional modifications
attention to form and meaning
approximation of real-world target tasks with
less pressing time constraints
development of functional L2 proficiency with
grammatical accuracy
Intelligent computer-generated feedback in
synchronous, task-based, computer-mediated
learner-learner interactions
8A balancing act
- We think so, but ICALL has certain limitations
- We want learners interactions to be as free as
possible, promoting authentic and meaningful
communication - But must constrain the communicative situation
somehow in order to make processing manageable - (cf. discussion in Amaral et al., 2006)
- We want to allow learners to make errors and then
provide them with detailed, informative
computer-generated feedback on nontargetlike
language use - But must reduce the amount of variability in
learner input so that accurate feedback can be
given
9Proposed study
- Questions
- What design features will allow an ICALL system
to provide detailed, accurate, individualized
metalinguistic feedback on L2 errors in
meaningful learner-to-learner CMC? - How can we make the system as user-friendly as
possible? - How effective will this sort of ICALL system be
in terms of promoting L2 development?
10We would argue
- Feedback on grammar can be provided in
synchronous CMC without sacrificing an undue
amount of communicative authenticity, as long as
we can design natural ways of - Controlling the activity specification
- Controlling the range of learner input
- Participants English-speaking university
students in first-year Korean classes - Target of feedback Korean postpositional
particles
11Activity specification
- Dyadic picture-based spot-the-differences task
- Each participant will see one version of a house
and have to exchange information in the L2 in
order to find similarities and differences
between the two pictures - A game record will provide additional guidance
and potentially increase motivation through
including an element of competition - Each participant must
- Record the activities and locations of all
characters in his/her partners house using a
provided chart - Indicate whether each of these represents a
similarity or difference - Following the activity, each dyad will be able to
compare their score to the average scores of
other dyads
12Picture 1
13Picture 2
14Activity specification (cont.)
- Guided and goal-oriented
- Constrains the vocabulary and domain, thereby
reducing many of the complexities involved in
generating feedback (computationally speaking) - Interactionally authentic
- Perhaps not so authentic in terms of real-world
relevance, but the sort of task often used in
interaction research to target specific areas of
language and promote negotiation and L2 learning
15Target of feedback Korean particles
- Korean has relatively free word order, so
postpositional particles are used to indicate
grammatical functions, thematic roles (e.g., who
is doing what to whom), and the locations of
people and objects. - Subject Verb Object
Location Time -
I
ate
pizza
last night
at a restaurant
?
?
??
?
?
?? ?
???
??
???
Subject
Time
Location
Object
Verb
16Target of feedback Korean particles
- Particles must be used even in simple sentences
thus, they are taught from the beginning of L2
Korean study - However, the system is quite complex and
difficult to master for adult learners of Korean - Korean particles make distinctions not made in
English - Some verbs have different argument structures
across the two languages - Several particles are ambiguous
- ? Particle errors account for a substantial
proportion of the mistakes made by beginning
learners (Ko et al., 2004), and errors persist
even at advanced levels
17Sources of difficulty for native speakers of
English
- No one-to-one correspondence between Korean
locative particles and English prepositions
18Sources of difficulty for native speakers of
English
- Korean has a topic marker, best translated as as
for or speaking of X, which English does not
have. - The distinction between subject and object
markers can be confusing because some English
transitive verbs are used as intransitive verbs
in Korean.
I (subject)
need
a book (object)
?? I (topic)
?? book (subject)
iss-ta (have) ? exist pilyoha-ta (need)
? be needed choh-ta (like) ? be
liked shil-ta (do not like) ? not be liked
19Examples of targetlike and non-targetlike
particle use
- Intransitive verbs need a subject marker
- ??-? ?-? ???? ??-? ?-? ????
- kitchen-LOC what-SUBJ is kitchen-LOC what-OBJ
is - What is in the kitchen?
- Transitive verbs need an object marker
- ???-? ??-? ???. ???-? ??-? ?? ??.
- father-SUBJ meat-OBJ grill father-SUBJ
meat-SUBJ is grilling - Father is grilling meat.
- A static location must be marked with a static
locative marker - ???-? ??-? ???. ???-? ??-?? ??.
- cat-SUBJ living room-LOC is cat-SUBJ living
room-LOC is - A cat is in the living room.
(TL examples are on the left with correct
particles non-TL are on the right with
asterisks)
20Other expected error types
- Missing particles
- ? ø ?? ? ø ??? ø ?? ø ???.
- I last night restaurant meat ate
- Incorrect particles (morphology)
- subject - i/ka sister (dongsang) i/ ka
- object - eul/rul rice (bab) eul/rul
- topic - un/nun elephant (kokkiri) un/nun
- comitative - wa/kwa rice (bab) wa/kwa meat
(gogi) - Incorrect particle order
- ????? (??) ?? ????
- restaurant-Loc-Top Who-Sub work-Ques
- As for a restaurant, who works (there)?
21Can beginning learners use CMC?
- We can expect problems with particles regardless
of the communicative situation - Unconstrained tasks might be stressful or
frustrating for beginning learners, making it
important to sequence, guide, and scaffold tasks
appropriately (cf. Doughty Long, 2003) - AND Beginning learners of Korean do not yet know
how to type in Korean - Since we wish to provide communicative practice
with particles, we need to ensure that the focus
of the task does not become that of simply
inputting Korean
22A possible solution
- Word and particle banks
- Learners can select the tokens they wish to use
simply by clicking on words and particles - For some morphophonological alternations, the
system will transform some adjacent characters
where necessary - Misspellings will be less of an issue
- Learners will be given positive evidence of
particle attachment in Korean
23The interface
- Spot-the-differences picture
- Word and particle banks
- Sentence drafting area
- Check and Send buttons
- Feedback-providing avatar
- Chat window
- Game record
24ParticipantA ??? ??? ???? ParticipantB ???
???. ParticipantA ???? ???? ParticipantB
S
S
D
D
??? ??? ?? ???.
S
S
D
D
S
D
S
D
S
D
S
D
CHECK
SEND
In your sentence, ?? is marked with the particle
?, which suggests that ?? is an object. Instead,
you need the particle ? attached to ?? in order
to indicate that ?? is the location of a dynamic
activity.
Word Bank
Particles
?
??
???
?
?
??
?
??
??
?
????
?
????
??
25ParticipantA ??? ??? ???? ParticipantB ???
???. ParticipantA ??? ???? ????
ParticipantB
TASK PICTURE Partners have slightly different
versions and must communicate to find
differences. They can scroll over the picture to
enlarge it.
S
D
S
D
??? ??? ?? ???.
S
S
D
D
S
D
S
D
CHECK
SEND
S
D
S
D
In your sentence, ?? is marked with the particle
?, which suggests that ?? is an object. Instead,
you need the particle ? attached to ?? in order
to indicate that ?? is the location of a dynamic
activity.
Word Bank
Particles
?
??
???
?
?
??
?
??
??
?
????
?
????
??
26ParticipantA ??? ??? ???? ParticipantB ???
???. ParticipantA ???? ???? ParticipantB
S
S
D
D
??? ??? ?? ???.
S
S
D
D
S
D
S
D
CHECK
SEND
S
D
S
D
In your sentence, ?? is marked with the particle
?, which suggests that ?? is an object. Instead,
you need the particle ? attached to ?? in order
to indicate that ?? is the location of a dynamic
activity.
Word Bank
Particles
WORD PARTICLE BANKS To create a sentence,
participants click on words and particles
?
??
???
?
?
??
?
??
??
?
????
?
????
??
27ParticipantA ??? ??? ???? ParticipantB ???
???. ParticipantA ???? ???? ParticipantB
which then appear in the sentence drafting area.
S
S
D
D
S
D
??? ??? ?? ???.
S
S
D
D
S
D
S
D
S
D
S
D
S
D
S
D
CHECK
SEND
S
D
In your sentence, ?? is marked with the particle
?, which suggests that ?? is an object. Instead,
you need the particle ? attached to ?? in order
to indicate that ?? is the location of a dynamic
activity.
Word Bank
Particles
?
??
???
?
?
??
?
??
??
?
????
?
????
??
28ParticipantA ??? ??? ???? ParticipantB ???
???. ParticipantA ???? ???? ParticipantB
If they want help with Korean particle usage,
they can request feedback on their sentences
before entering them into the conversation.
S
D
S
D
??? ??? ?? ???.
S
S
D
D
S
D
S
D
CHECK
SEND
S
D
S
D
In your sentence, ?? is marked with the particle
?, which suggests that ?? is an object. Instead,
you need the particle ? attached to ?? in order
to indicate that ?? is the location of a dynamic
activity.
Word Bank
Particles
?
??
???
?
?
??
?
??
??
?
????
?
????
??
29ParticipantA ??? ??? ???? ParticipantB ???
???. ParticipantA ???? ???? ParticipantB
S
D
S
D
??? ??? ?? ???.
S
S
D
D
S
D
S
D
CHECK
SEND
S
D
S
D
In your sentence, ?? is marked with the particle
?, which suggests that ?? is an object. Instead,
you need the particle ? attached to ?? in order
to indicate that ?? is the location of a dynamic
activity.
Word Bank
Particles
FEEDBACK AREA Here, participants receive
metalinguistic feedback with advice on particle
usage.
?
??
???
?
?
??
?
??
??
?
????
?
????
??
30ParticipantA ??? ??? ???? ParticipantB ???
???. ParticipantA ???? ???? ParticipantB
S
D
S
D
When they are ready, they click SEND to enter
their utterance into the conversation.
??? ??? ?? ???.
S
S
D
D
S
D
S
D
CHECK
SEND
S
D
S
D
In your sentence, ?? is marked with the particle
?, which suggests that ?? is an object. Instead,
you need the particle ? attached to ?? in order
to indicate that ?? is the location of a dynamic
activity.
Word Bank
Particles
?
??
???
?
?
??
?
??
??
?
????
?
????
??
31ParticipantA ??? ??? ???? ParticipantB ???
???. ParticipantA ???? ???? ParticipantB
S
D
S
D
CHAT WINDOW They can scroll up and down to
review the conversation so far.
??? ??? ?? ???.
S
S
D
D
S
D
S
D
CHECK
SEND
S
D
S
D
In your sentence, ?? is marked with the particle
?, which suggests that ?? is an object. Instead,
you need the particle ? attached to ?? in order
to indicate that ?? is the location of a dynamic
activity.
Word Bank
Particles
?
??
???
?
?
??
?
??
??
?
????
?
????
??
32ParticipantA ??? ??? ???? ParticipantB ???
???. ParticipantA ???? ???? ParticipantB
GAME RECORD When participants find similarities
or differences, they drag the relevant words for
locations and activities here to record
information about their partners pictures, then
click on S or D to indicate whether the
pictures match in those respects or not.
S
D
S
D
??? ??? ?? ???.
S
S
D
D
S
D
S
D
CHECK
SEND
S
D
S
D
In your sentence, ?? is marked with the particle
?, which suggests that ?? is an object. Instead,
you need the particle ? attached to ?? in order
to indicate that ?? is the location of a dynamic
activity.
Word Bank
Particles
?
??
???
?
?
??
?
??
??
?
????
?
????
??
33Is processing feasible?
- Learners sentence construction is guided by
- The nature of the picture-based task
- Instructions and the game record
- Word and particle banks, which
- Limit the types of argument structure by limiting
the verbs used - May be necessary for beginning learners who cant
type in Korean - May serve as a scaffold for using receptive vocab
in conversation - Intensive feedback is provided on one particular
error type
34Upshot
- Synergy between pedagogical and computational
constraints - Beginning learners will feel comfortable
communicating meaningfully in the L2 (with
familiar content and sufficient guidance) - The learners can still make mistakes in the L2
while attempting to express themselves - ICALL processing can focus just on detecting
particle errors in a known domain
35How can we detect ill-formed sentences?
- A combination of techniques will ultimately be
used to feed into an error diagnosis module - Linguistic processing will be kept separate from
error detection/diagnosis and feedback generation - Since general relations between elements of the
task pictures are fixed, fairly traditional
anticipation-based pattern matching (i.e.,
regular expressions) could be used - This will need to be augmented with basic
linguistic abstraction (part-of-speech tags and
syntactic chunks) - Partial parsing methods are extremely robust
provide information even when a full syntactic
parse is not possible - Linguistic abstraction ensures applicability to
new exercises
36Opportunity to experiment with different
techniques
- Particle errors will often result from a mismatch
between the argument relations of the sentence
and the morphological forms used by the learners - Could use multiple parsing models to check for
mismatches (cf. Metcalf Boyd, 2006) - One parser captures particle usage patterns from
real language - Another parser captures general argument
structure patterns between words, irrespective of
particles - Currently exploring this other techniques
(Dickinson Lee, 2008) - Regardless of the techniques, generating learner
data will provide evaluation material to help
advance the state-of-the-art in processing Korean
learner input
37Is focused feedback beneficial?
- Some have argued that intensive feedback on one
pre-selected error type may be more effective in
certain contexts than wide-ranging incidental
feedback on a variety of errors - (e.g., Lyster, 1998 Nicholas, Lightbown,
Spada, 2001) - In our study, we will inform learners that they
will be receiving feedback only on particles - Important for meaning (i.e., communicating who is
doing what to whom) in Korean - Will hopefully prevent them from mistaking
non-feedback for correctness - Leaves open the possibility of providing other
feedback, if needed
38Is meaningful communication promoted?
- Does this set-up truly represent synchronous
CMC as it is commonly conceptualized? - ?How much will the learners focus on meaningful
communication if it is clear that the feedback is
focusing exclusively on Korean particles? - Particles are crucial to expressing and
understanding meaning in Korean sentences thus,
the ostensibly grammar-oriented feedback should
facilitate communication - ?How can the word and particle banks be made
sufficiently rich for the participants
communicative purposes?
39The importance of piloting
- Picture-based tasks have been used successfully
in other experimental CMC research as a means of
guiding content and controlling amount/type of
feedback (e.g., Sachs Suh, 2007), but learners
were already proficient typists in the L2. - Will beginning learners be capable of interacting
smoothly in the current context? - What sorts of scaffolding will they actually
need? - What can we do to make the banks as easy to use
(and as facilitative of L2 development) as
possible?
40Future directions
- Pilot the tasks and competitive game component
with L2 learners - Get a clearer sense of what to expect in learner
input - Test how the word and particle banks are actually
used - Develop the system in modular fashion, ensuring
it will be extendible to other Korean language
activities - Activity model (indicating expected constructions
and words for the word bank) - Expert model (for linguistic analysis)
- Error diagnosis module
- Feedback module
- (cf. TAGARELA Amaral Meurers, 2006)
41Future directions (cont.)
- Develop activities to target more areas of
language - Make the tasks more complex, meaningful, and
relevant to real-life communicative situations - Use this set-up to test questions of SLA theory
and language pedagogy - Assess L2 development under different feedback
conditions (e.g., metalinguistic info vs.
recasts) with pre-test/post-test experimental
designs - Investigate optimization of feedback for
different areas of language, proficiency levels,
aptitude profiles, etc. - Integrate this system with the Korean language
curriculum at Georgetown
42Questions?Comments?
43References
- Amaral, L., Meurers, D. (2006). Where does
ICALL fit into foreign language teaching. CALICO
2006. University of Hawaii. - Amaral, L., Metcalf, V., Meurers, D. (2006).
Language awareness through re-use of NLP
technology. Pre-conference Workshop on NLP in
CALL Computational and Linguistic Challenges.
CALICO 2006. University of Hawaii. - Blake, R. (2000). Computer-mediated
communication A window on L2 Spanish
interlanguage. Language Learning Technology,
4(1), 120-136. - Chapelle, C. (2003). English language learning
and technology. Philadelphia John Benjamins. - Dickinson, M. Lee, C.M. (2008). Korean Particle
Error Detection via Probabilistic Parsing.
Workshop on Automatic Analysis of Learner
Language at CALICO-08. San Francisso. - Doughty, C.J., Long, M.H. (2003). Optimal
psycholinguistic environments for distance
foreign language learning. Language Learning
Technology, 7(3), 50-80. - Ellis, R. (2003). Task-based language learning
and teaching. Oxford Oxford University Press. - Heift, T., Schulze, M. (2007). Errors and
intelligence in computer-assisted language
learning Parsers and pedagogues. Routledge.
44References
- Kern, R.G. (1995). Restructuring classroom
interaction with networked computers Effects on
quantity and characteristics of language
production. The Modern Language Journal, 79,
457-473. - Ko, S., Kim, M., Kim, J., Seo, S., Chung, H.,
Han, S. (2004). An analysis of Korean learner
corpora and errors. Hanguk Publishing Co. - Lee, L. (2001). Online interaction Negotiation
of meaning and strategies used among learners of
Spanish. ReCALL Journal, 13(2), 232-244. - Lyster, R. (1998). Form in immersion classroom
discourse In or out of focus? Canadian Journal
of Applied Linguistics, 1, 53-82. - Mackey, A., Goo, J. (2007). Interaction
research in SLA A meta-analysis and research
synthesis. In A. Mackey (Ed.), Conversational
interaction in second language acquisition A
series of empirical studies (pp. 407-452).
Oxford Oxford University Press. - Metcalf, V., Boyd, A. (2006, December).
Head-lexicalized PCFGs for verb subcategorization
error diagnosis in ICALL. Workshop on Interfaces
of Intelligent Computer-Assisted Language
Learning. The Ohio State University. - Nagata, N. (1995). An effective application of
natural language processing in second language
instruction. CALICO Journal, 13(1), 47-67.
45References
- Nicholas, H., Lightbown, P.M., Spada, N.
(2001). Recasts as feedback to language learners.
Language Learning, 51, 719-758. - Payne, J.S., Whitney, P.J. (2002). Developing
L2 oral proficiency through synchronous CMC
Output, working memory, and interlanguage
development. CALICO Journal, 20(1), 7-32. - Pellettieri, J. (2000). Negotiation in
cyberspace The role of chatting in the
development of grammatical competence in the
virtual foreign language classroom. In M.
Warschauer R. Kern (eds.), Network-based
language teaching Concepts and practice (pp.
59-86). Cambridge CUP. - Petersen, K. (2006, December). Measuring L2
development in an ICALL context. Workshop on
Interfaces of Intelligent Computer-Assisted
Language Learning. The Ohio State University. - Russell, J., Spada, N. (2006). The
effectiveness of corrective feedback for the
acquisition of L2 grammar. In J.M. Norris L.
Ortega (Eds.), Synthesizing research on language
learning and teaching. Amsterdam John Benjamins. - Sachs, R., Suh, B-R. (2007). Textually enhanced
recasts, learner awareness, and L2 outcomes in
synchronous computer-mediated interaction. In A.
Mackey (Ed.), Conversational interaction in
second language acquisition A series of
empirical studies (pp. 197-227). Oxford Oxford
University Press. - Warschauer, M. (1996). Comparing face-to-face and
electronic discussion in the second language
classroom. CALICO Journal, 13(2), 7-26.