Title: Language and stuttering
1Language and stuttering
2Overview
- Language factors in models of stuttering
- Linguistic factors that affect the frequency and
loci of stutter events - Language factors in the development of stuttering
- Language factors that affect fluency treatment in
children - Language disorders and disfluency
3- Ways to think about Fluency and Language
???
4Extant models of stuttering
- Most of the available models of stuttering assume
a mature linguistic and motor system (i.e., The
Covert Repair Hypothesis Postma Kolk, 1993
the Neuropsycholinguistic Theory Perkins, Kent
Curlee, 1991). - The major developmental theory (Capacities
Demands Adams, 1990 Starkweather Gottwald,
1990) proposes limits in the childs ability to
integrate linguistic and motor functions, but
lacks evidence of the specific limitations that
predispose to stuttering.
5A Multifactorial Model of Stuttering in adults
(Smith, 1999)
Cognitive proc.
Language
Cognitive
Language proc.
Language
Motor
Cognitive
NOT
Pre-motor plan
Patterned output emerges from the interaction of
multiple systems
Motor output is the result of various stages
6Linguistic effects on motor function Use of the
spatial-temporal index (STI) to assess
interactions among systems
Is there evidence of an underlying template of
movement, despite trial to trial differences in
duration and amplitude? Smith Weber-Fox, ASHA
1999
7 The STI decreases with age. Adults are
highly reliable pattern generators from trial to
trial. Thus we can use the STI as an index of the
depth of the attractor. Adults have a very stable
attractor for this utterance. Children are less
stable than adults even at age 12.
8 Childrens (five-year olds) speech motor
output is negatively affected by increasing the
length and complexity of the utterance. The STI
increases when the phrase buy bobby a puppy is
embedded in a complex sentence.
Maner, K., Smith, A., Grayson, L. (2000).
Influences of length and syntactic complexity on
speech motor performance of children and adults.
Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing
Research. See also J. Kleinow A. Smith (2000)
same issue.
9 The speech motor output of adults was not
adversely affected by embedding the phrase in a
longer, complex sentence.
Maner, K., Smith, A., Grayson, L. (2000).
Influences of length and syntactic complexity on
speech motor performance of children and adults.
Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing
Research.
10But, adults who stutter show motor stability
effects when linguistic complexity is increased
(Kleinow Smith, 2000 -- example of AWS.)
11From adults to children Problems in modeling
stuttering onset
- Although stuttering is widely viewed as a
disorder of speech motor control, abnormalities
in speech motor function are not seen in early
stages of the disorder, and become apparent only
later (Kelly, Smith Goffman, 1995). - Stuttering is not present at the onset of
language acquisition, but has its onset after
children develop rudimentary syntax (Bernstein
Ratner, 1997).
12Additional concerns for models of stuttering onset
- Psycholinguistic models of stuttering and normal
disfluency predict moments of fluency breakdown,
but NOT THE TYPES witnessed in early stuttering,
which are characterized by struggle and awareness
of speech difficulty. - Stuttering is thus distinct from other childhood
communicative disorders, in which childrens
abilities are impaired from onset, and children
show little awareness and reactivity to their
impaired output.
13Some data Linguistic factors at stuttering onset
- Language screening scores predict chronicity
(Yairi, et al., 1996) - Onset during rapid period of language growth
(lexical spurt, morphological acquisitions) - Stuttering gravitates toward grammatically
incorrect productions, utterances with higher
TTR, MLU.
14- Other language findings at stuttering onset
- Stuttered utterances have higher MLU, TTR and
show developmental formulation errors (Bernstein
Ratner, 1998) - CWS have lowered expressive lexicon, and shorter
utterances (Bernstein Ratner Silverman, 1998)
in general, evidence of sub-clinical linguistic
weaknesses is emerging (Conture colleagues) - CWS show atypically heightened self-monitoring of
speech, as measured by response to DAF (Razzak
Bernstein Ratner, 1999)
15Linguistic properties of stuttered events across
the lifespan
- Phonetics
- Syntax
- Lexicon
- Metalinguistics
- Trade-offs among domains and fluency
16- Phonetic factors in stuttering
- Phonetic factors
- impossible to separate from word frequency
factors - may differ across languages in bilingual
stuttering, suggesting higher-order determinants
(Bernstein Ratner Benitez, 1985) - Phonological factors
- word-initial loci consistent with phonological
distinction between onset and rhyme in encoding
of word forms - Stress
- recent evidence suggests that relationship of
lexical stress to stuttering is conflated with
word frequency patterns in English - Possible interactions with syntax at onset (i.e.,
Wh-words), with subsequent motor learning effects
17- Other considerations - phonology
- At onset, no evidence of phonological disorder
(Yairi, et al, 1996), though persistent cases may
show phonological delay (Louko). - Stuttering does not appear to gravitate toward
late-acquired ("difficult") or errored phonology
(Throneburg, et al., 1995), possibly because
other factors are more important - No evidence of motor abnormalities in young
children (Kelly, Smith Goffman), suggesting
non-motor based origin for early stutter moments
18How syntax affects fluency
19- Does Length or Syntax determine the frequency of
stuttering?
- Syntactic complexity more highly correlated with
utterances stuttered (r .954) than is length (r
.701) (Bernstein Ratner Sih, 1987) - MLU a better predictor of stuttering than
syllable length of utterance (Brundage
Bernstein Ratner (1989) - DSS less well correlated with stuttering than
utterance length, but both are strongly
associated with frequency of stuttering (Logan
Conture, 1995)
20- A sample syntactic hierarchy
Sentence type Sample Sentence and order 1.
Simple active declarative The boy found the
lady. 2. Negative The boy didn't find the
lady. 3. Question Did the boy find the
lady? 4. Passive The boy was found by the
lady. 5. Dative The boy found the lady a
chair. 6. Prepositional phrase The boy found
the lady with the red hair. 7.
Coordinate w. forward The boy is finding a
lady or a man. reduction 8. Right-embedded
relative clause We had thought that the boy
found the lady. 9. Left-embedded
relative That the boy found the lady was a
lie. 10. Center-embedded
relative The chair that the boy found the lady
was broken.
21- Stuttering loci in children
- Stuttering most likely to be utterance- and
clause-initial - Stuttering loci primarily at syntactic
constituent boundaries (i.e., before NP, VP, PP,
etc.) (Bernstein, 1981 Bloodstein Gantwerk,
1967 Bloodstein Grossman, 1981 Wall, et al.,
1981) - Syntactic loci remain constant across languages
in bilingual adults, while phonetic loci vary
(Bernstein Ratner Benitez, 1985 Newman, 1990) - Syntactic effects on fluency appear to decline in
adolescence (Silverman Bernstein Ratner, 1997)
22Lexical access and fluency
23Formulation difficulty and stuttering
- There are strong interactions between fluency and
grammaticality (stuttering children) in
spontaneous speech (Bernstein Ratner, 2000)
24Implications for stuttering therapy
- If language demand affects fluency in children,
what does this imply for diagnosis and
therapeutic planning?
25Self-monitoring and early stuttering - DAF
responses Children who stutter - responses are
slightly heightened
Difference between conditions significant at t
-2.32 p .05
26DAF responses Children who do not stutter
(Razzak Ratner, ASHA, 1999)
Difference between conditions non-significant
27A developmental model of early stuttering
(Bernstein Ratner, 2000 and in prep.)
Hyperfunctional self-monitoring
Linguistic fragility
Awareness struggle
Disfluency
Learning/development of secondaries
28Language disorder and fluency
- A growing literature suggests that children with
SLI differ from typically developing children in
fluency of verbal output (Hall, et al., 1993
Hall,1996 Boscolo, Ratner Rescorla, 2002
Miller et al., ASHA,2001). Because of growing
concern that a standard set of linguistic
features may not suffice to identify SLI
cross-linguistically (Leonard, 1998), the
potential of extra-linguistic factors in
diagnosis of SLI is of practical research and
clinical value.
29Data from a longitudinal study of SLI children
- The following data derive from 34 pairs of
children followed longitudinally by Rescorla
(1989) and her colleagues (e.g., Rescorla
Schwartz, 1990 Rescorla Ratner, 1996
Rescorla Fechnay, 1996) from ages 2 to 13. - In each group, half of the children had been
identified as late talkers/SLI-E, while the
others were identified as typically-developing. - For these analyses, there were 14 pairs of
children at age 3, 12 pairs of children at age 5,
and 22 TD/30 LT children at age 9.
30Method
- At ages 3 and 5, children were engaged in
15-minute play sessions with their mothers at
age 9, they produced a narrative in response to
Frog, where are you? - Data were transcribed in CHAT, with fluency codes
to indicate place and nature of fluency
breakdown. - The following types of disfluencies were
considered normal filled and unfilled pauses,
phrase revisions and repetitions - The following types of disfluencies were
considered stutterlike disfluencies (Yairi et
al., 1996) blocks, broken words, part and whole
word repetitions, prolongations of consonants.
31Disfluency rates - age 3
Note distribution of Stutter-like Dysfluencies
(SLDs) statistically different between groups by
Kolmogorov-Smirnov Test For Different
Distributions
32Results Age 5
No statistical differences between groups.
33Results age 9
- As reported in Boscolo, et al. (2002), by age
nine, frequency of SLDs was significantly
different (p lt.02) for children who had started
as late talkers. These children had nearly twice
the rate of SLDs per 100 words as their typically
developing peers.
34Summary group fluency effects
- Collapsed across the age range, SLDs were
significantly more likely to be found in the
speech output of children with a history of late
expressive language development (p lt.019,
Mann-Whitney-Wilcoxon), whereas overall
disfluency rates and normal disfluency types were
only slightly more frequent in the speech of
these children than their typically-developing
peers. SLD rates were statistically elevated in
LTs at ages 3 and 9.
35- There was also a large range of disfluency rates
unique to the late-talking groups, suggesting the
potential for individuals in these groups to be
misdiagnosed as having a clinical fluency
problem. - At ages 3 and 5, a total of four different
late-talking children had SLD scores in excess of
the typical 3 diagnostic cut-off for stuttering
(Yairi, et al., 1996), while at age 9, one
additional child approached this level of SLD
frequency in spoken output.
36Discussion
- Our findings confirm that children with SLI-E
demonstrate substantially more disfluency in
spontaneous conversation, including some types of
disfluency more typical of stuttering, rather
than typically fluent, children, although no
child in the study demonstrated clinical
stuttering, as defined by the presence of
struggle, accessory behaviors or self-image as a
child who stutters. - Similar data have been reported by Hall (1996)
and Hall, Yamashita Aram (1993). - What are the ramifications for clinical practice?
37- Specifically in regard to the distinct patterns
of fluency breakdown observed in our sample of
children with SLI-E, results concur with those of
Miller et al. (2001) that fluency profiles may be
useful in compiling profiles of SLI, when
combined with other assessment measures. These
data add to ongoing work that suggests a high
degree of overlap in the linguistic, fluency and
phonological abilities of very young children
having primary language and fluency disorders
(Ratner, in press).