Li8 Structure of English - PowerPoint PPT Presentation

1 / 27
About This Presentation
Title:

Li8 Structure of English

Description:

From www.powerpointbackgrounds.com Messages do not appear on the full product. – PowerPoint PPT presentation

Number of Views:69
Avg rating:3.0/5.0
Slides: 28
Provided by: peopleDs7
Category:
Tags: english | li8 | structure

less

Transcript and Presenter's Notes

Title: Li8 Structure of English


1
Li8 Structure of English
  • Evidence for morphological structure

2
Morphology
  • Maybe in order to understand mankind, we have to
    look at the word itself. Mankind. Basically, its
    made up of two separate words - mank and ind.
    What do these words mean? Its a mystery, and
    thats why so is mankind.
  • Jack Handey, Deep Thoughts

3
Morphemes
  • Morphology is the study of morphemes and their
    behavior.
  • A morpheme is the minimal unit of meaning.
  • dogs contains 2 morphemes
  • 1 d?g quadruped of the genus canis
  • 2 -z plural
  • Note morphemes ? words
  • A word is the smallest element that can occur in
    isolation.

4
Evidence for morphological decomposition and
storage
  • New singulars
  • New morphemes
  • Psycholinguistic evidence
  • Rule-governed behavior
  • Priming studies
  • Event Related Potentials
  • Speech errors
  • Disorders

5
New singulars
  • indice, homo sapien

6
New morphemes
  • -a/oholic
  • -burger
  • -gate
  • Watergate, Fornigate
  • -head
  • pothead, metalhead, techhead, gearhead,
    theoryhead, crackhead, hockey head
  • Mc-
  • McMansion, mcjob, mcnews

It's true, I'm a Rageaholic...I just can't live
without Rageahol!
7
McDonald's not lovin' McJob dictionary definition
  • CHICAGO, Illinois (AP, 11-2003) -- McDonald's
    says it deserves a break from the unflattering
    way the latest Merriam-Webster's Collegiate
    Dictionary depicts its job opportunities. Among
    some 10,000 new additions to an updated version
    released in June was the term "McJob," defined as
    "low paying and dead-end work." In an open letter
    to Merriam-Webster, McDonald's CEO Jim Cantalupo
    said the term is "an inaccurate description of
    restaurant employment" and "a slap in the face to
    the 12 million men and women" who work in the
    restaurant industry. The company e-mailed the
    letter to media organizations Friday, and it also
    was published in the Nov. 3 edition of an
    industry trade publication. Cantalupo also wrote
    that "more than 1,000 of the men and women who
    own and operate McDonald's restaurants today got
    their start by serving customers behind the
    counter. McDonald's, the world's largest
    restaurant chain, has more than 30,000
    restaurants and more than 400,000 employees. Walt
    Riker, a spokesman for McDonald's, said the Oak
    Brook, Illinois-based fast-food giant also is
    concerned that "McJob" closely resembles McJOBS,
    the company's training program for mentally and
    physically challenged people.
  • "McJOBS is trademarked and we've notified them
    that legally that's an issue for us as well,"
    Riker said.

McJob (m?kdZAb) n. A low-paying job that
requires little skill and provides little
opportunity for advancement. Source
Merriam-Webster Online
8
Psycholinguistic evidence
9
Over-regularization
  • went ? goed ? went
  • Happens c. late twos
  • Suggests acquisition of morphological rules

10
Wug tests
  • Berko 1958
  • 3 surface manifestations of English regular
    plural /-z/ z, s, ?z
  • When/how do children learn these rules?
  • Test paradigm
  • Children are presented with a pretend creature
    and told, "This is a wug."
  • Another wug is revealed, and the researcher says,
    "Now there are two of them. There are two __."
  • Results
  • Very young children are baffled by the question
    and are unable to answer correctly, responding
    with e.g. two wug."
  • Children in grade 1 were almost fully competent
    with both s and z.
  • Both preschool and first-grade children dealt
    poorly with ?z, giving the correct answer less
    than half the time, possibly because it occurs in
    the most restrictive context.
  • Major finding
  • The first experimental proof that young children
    have extracted generalizable morphological rules
    from the language around them.

11
Productive irregulars
  • Al Jallad, Flint, and Richardson 2005
  • Subjects come up with past tense for unfamiliar
    V.
  • 1. Jim BLINGS everyday.
  • Yesterday John __________________.
  • 2. Alan CHINES everyday.
  • Yesterday Alan __________________.
  • Subjects in the experiment
  • 26 children age 6-7 (oral test)
  • 44 adults (written questionnaire)
  • Sample Over-Irregularizations
  • children
  • pake ? pakeded
  • bling ? blung, blang
  • flink ? flunk
  • frim ? frand
  • adults
  • mang ? mung
  • shride ? shrude
  • bling ? blank

Overall Rate of Irregular forms supplied
Individual Differences in Irregularization bars
show percentages of subjects making 0-20
irregulars, 20-40 irregulars etc.
12
ERPs
  • McKinnon et al. 2003.
  • Observation
  • Pronounceable non-words (flermuf) elicit
    larger-amplitude N400 components than words
    (muffler).
  • Prediction
  • If readers treat non-words containing
    non-productive morphemes (in-ceive) as unanalysed
    wholes, then these non-words should elicit larger
    N400s than matched words (receive).
  • Results
  • Bound-stem non-words elicit a brain response
    highly similar to that elicited by real words.
  • Conclusion
  • Morphological decomposition and representation
    extend to non-productive morphemes.

13
BrothBrothelBrother
  • Rastle et al. 2004
  • Participants made visual lexical decisions to
    stem targets preceded by masked primes sharing
  • (a) a semantically-transparent morphological
    relationship with the target (e.g.,
    cleaner-CLEAN)
  • (b) an apparent morphological relationship, but
    no semantic relationship, with the target (e.g.,
    corner-CORN)
  • (c) a non-morphological form relationship with
    the target (e.g., brothel-BROTH).
  • Results showed significant and equivalent masked
    priming effects in cases in which primes and
    targets appeared to be morphologically related,
    and priming in these conditions could be
    distinguished from non-morphological form
    priming.
  • These findings suggest a level of representation
    at which apparently complex words are decomposed
    based on their morpho-orthographic properties.

14
Morphological speech errors
15
Morphological errors
  • Morpheme exchange
  • slicely thinned
  • Feature shift
  • have to went for had to go
  • Faulty access
  • have teachen for have taught
  • concludement for conclusion
  • Experimentally controlled morphological errors

16
Janssen and Humphreys 2002
  • General logic
  • If a string of phonemes with an identifiable
    meaning can move independently during a speech
    error, it is a production unit, with a separate,
    independent representation at some cognitive
    level.
  • Inflectional morphemes (e.g. -ed, -ing, -s) are
    much more error prone than derivational morphemes
    (e.g. -er, -ness, -able, -ion) (Garrett 1980,
    Humphreys, 2002).
  • Can we also find experimental speech error
    evidence of derivational decomposition?
  • Prediction
  • If derivational morphemes are stored
    independently from their stems, more
    morphological speech errors should occur when -er
    is a real morpheme than when it is only a
    pseudo-morpheme.

17
Method
  • elicited morphological errors from speakers by
    presenting a series of word quads, made up of two
    pairs. Speakers read these words silently, after
    which they disappeared. Speakers were
    occasionally cued to respond aloud, as quickly as
    possible, to the immediately preceding item.
    Speakers had to then either repeat the previous
    phrases exactly, or had to swap words between the
    phrases. The critical pairs each consisted of an
    initial ller word plus a critical word stem or
    stemaffix.

18
Results
  • More affix errors occurred on the morphologically
    complex forms than on the pseudo-morphs.
  • Conclusion derivational affixes are also stored
    as separate morphemes.

19
Morphological disorders
20
Lexical disorder
  • Badecker 2001
  • CSS (65 years old) suffered a left
    cerebro-vascular accident in 1990, resulting in
    lexical impairments manifested in reading,
    repetition, and oral and written naming tasks.
  • Apart from word-finding problems, CSSs spoken
    output is fluent and he exhibits no apparent
    comprehension impairment.

naming from definition Slender-bodied insect with
broad, often brightly-colored wings. butterfly
picture naming butterfly
monomorphemes 80 correct
doctor fly sun wheel pill
polymorphemes 50 correct!
21
Semantic disorders
  • Deep dyslexia semantic errors during reading
  • Buchanan et al. 2003
  • Deep dyslexic patient (JO, 48, L temporo-parietal
    lobectomy)
  • Acquired reading disorder characterized by
    production of
  • morphological errors (e.g., SLEEP read as
    SLEEPING)
  • semantic errors (e.g. HEART read as BLOOD)

22
Experiment
  • Method
  • Manipulate transparency in compound word naming
    study
  • Results
  • 94 stimuli
  • 10 read correctly 9 of these were TT
  • Transparent components read correctly more often
  • Key point if there were no decomposition, all
    compound types should show the same error rate
  • Response to PANCAKE
  • cake, breakfast, man,. . .cake, man. . .cake,
    mancake, man, man, p-, p-, cake, birthday,
    breakfast, cake, p-a-n. . . cake, syrup...
  • Typical semantic error target cake ? output
    birthday (shows decomposition)
  • NB 2nd and last attempts are semantic associate
    for the whole compound (BREAKFAST, SYRUP)
  • Shows successful access of meaning of the
    compound, even though she never manages to say
    the whole thing
  • Conclusion
  • JO can successfully parse compound into
    constituents and access meaning of the whole the
    errors occur after this point

23
Deep dyslexia
  • Rastle et al. 2005
  • Observation
  • Deep dyslexics often make morphological errors in
    reading aloud
  • e.g. sexist read as sexy
  • Two competing explanations
  • representations are morphologically structured
  • low imageability/frequency word modified to
    visually similar word of higher
    imageability/frequency) (Funnell 1987)
  • Prediction
  • Theory 1 predicts more stem errors with
    morphologically complex words (killer) than with
    pseudosuffixed words (corner) and monomorphemic
    words (cornea) when one equalizes for
    imageability and frequency.
  • Theory 2 predicts no difference between these 3
    categories.
  • Result
  • Theory 1 (but not Theory 2) supported

24
Regular vs. irregular
  • Ullman et al. 1997, Ullman 2001 propose that
  • the left inferior frontal cortex (along with the
    basal ganglia) is involved in rule-based
    computations
  • Irregular forms dealt with differently
  • Prediction
  • A lesion to this region should impair only
    regular morphological processes, sparing the
    ability to produce irregular morphology.
  • Test
  • Four types of patients Parkinsons, Alzheimers,
    anterior aphasics, and posterior aphasics
  • Patients were asked to perform a sentence
    completion task requiring the production of a
    verb in the past tense
  • E.g. Every day I dig a hole. Just like every
    day, yesterday I ____ a hole
  • Results
  • anterior aphasics and Parkinson patients
    performed worse with regular than irregular verbs
  • the opposite for posterior aphasics and Alzheimer
    patients
  • Conclusion
  • Results consistent with theory that the left
    frontal cortex and basal ganglia are involved in
    rule-based language processing but not in the
    processing of irregular morphology, and that
    temporal lobe areas are implicated in the storage
    of lexical forms.
  • But

25
Regular vs. irregular
  • Problem with Ullmans theory
  • Agrammatic (Brocas) aphasics who perform worse
    on irregular than regular morphological
    transformations (producing irregular V forms)
  • patient RC (Shapiro Caramazza 2003)
  • patient MR (Laiacona Caramazza 2004)
  • Spanish-Catalan bilinguals (Caramazza et al.
    2004)
  • Consistent with the classical view that left
    inferior frontal cortex is involved in
    morphosyntactic processing and not just in
    rule-based transformations.

26
Conclusions
  • Humans decompose words into morphemes and store
    them as such.
  • Both regular and irregular/restricted morphology
    involve rule-governed (de)composition.
  • blang, inceive, Brocas aphasics
  • Humans look for morphological structure even when
    there isnt any.
  • cheeseburger, corner

27
References
  • Andrews, S. 1986. Morphological influences on
    lexical access Lexical or nonlexical effects?
    Journal of Memory and Language, 25, 726-740.
  • Andrews, S., B. Miller, K. Rayner. 2004. Eye
    movements and morphological segmentation of
    compound words There is a mouse in mousetrap.
    European Journal of Cognitive Psychology
    16285-311.
  • Badecker, William. 2001. Lexical composition and
    the production of compounds evidence from errors
    in naming. Language and Cognitive Processes 16.4.
  • Buchanan, Lori, Shannon McEwen, Chris Westbury,
    and Gary Libben. 2003. Semantics and semantic
    errors Implicit access to semantic information
    from words and nonwords in deep dyslexia. Brain
    and Language 846583.
  • Caramazza, Alfonso, Alessandro Laudanna, and
    Cristina Romani. 1988. Lexical access and
    inflectional morphology. Cognition 28.
  • Fiorentino, Robert. 2006. Masked priming of
    compound constituents Implications for
    morphological decomposition. Manuscript,
    University of Maryland.
  • Janssen, Dirk and Karin Humphreys. 2002.
    Morphological speech errors on agentive and
    comparative affixes. Third International
    Conference on the Mental Lexicon, Banff, Canada.
  • McKinnon, R., M. Allen, L. Osterhout. 2003. 
    Morphological decomposition involving
    non-productive morphemes ERP Evidence.
    Neuroreport 14883-886.
  • Marslen-Wilson et al. 1994. Psychol Rev 1013-33.
  • Rastle, Kathleen, Matthew Davis, and Boris New.
    2004. The Broth in my Brothers Brothel
    Morpho-Orthographic Segmentation in Visual Word
    Recognition. Psychonomic Bulletin Review
    11.61090-1098.
  • Rastle, Kathleen, et al. 2005. New evidence for
    morphological errors in deep dyslexia. Brain and
    Language 97189-199.
  • Shapiro, K., Alfonso Caramazza. 2003. Looming a
    loom Evidence for independent access to
    grammatical and phonological properties in verb
    retrieval. Journal of Neurolinguistics 1685-111.
Write a Comment
User Comments (0)
About PowerShow.com