Title: Elizabeth Crais
1USING THE BEST AVAILABLE EVIDENCE TO IDENTIFY
INFANTS AND TODDLERS WITH (OR AT RISK FOR)
COMMUNICATION DEFICITS
Elizabeth Crais The University of
North Carolina at Chapel Hill TelAbility/WATCH
Project January 11, 2008
2Where We Are Now?
- Clear evidence that early intervention works
- Growing evidence across a variety of disabilities
supports maxim earlier ID is better - 10-12 of school-age population receiving special
education services - 12-16 of children have developmental or
behavioral disabilities (Comm. on Child with Dis,
2001) - 7-24 of 2-3 year olds have social-emotional or
behavioral deficits (Briggs-Gowan et al, 2001) - How early are we identifying these children?
3Percentage of Children Identified For EI By Age
Range
- 1.6 Birth to 2 years
- 4.9 3-5 years
- 11 6-18 years (U.S. Department of Education,
2005) - Even among those identified early 0 - 3
- 14 0-1 years
- 32 1-2 years
- 54 2-3 years
- (Part C Update, 2004)
- Most commonly identified early concerns are
related to motor or language delays
4Are We Using the Best Predictors?
- One of best predictors of childs future language
is childs current communication performance
(Brady, Marquis, Fleming, McLean, 2004 Facon,
Facon-Bollengier, Gruber, 2002). - Language skills are a strong predictor of
cognitive skills (Brady et al, 2004).
5Current Evidence Based Predictors
- Vocal Behaviors
- Vocabulary Comprehension
- Prelinguistic communication
- Gesture Use
- Symbolic Play
6Vocal Behaviors Predictive of Later Language
- More vocalizations
- More consonant-vowel syllables
- Rate of vocalization
- Rate of vocalizations with consonants
- Rate of vocalizations in interaction with others
(). (McCathern, Yoder, Warren, 1999). - Ability to use sounds is strongest predictor of
language skills one year later (First Words)
7Prelinguistic Communication
- Rate of prelinguistic communication
- Rate of symbol use (especially gestures)
- Rate of different symbol use
- Significantly correlated with later language
outcomes 12 months later - Amount of prelinguistic communication predictive
of later symbolic communication (Calandrella
Wilcox, 2000). - Frequency of nonverbal communication in
preschoolers with ASD predicts language 1 year
later (Sigman Ruskin, 1999)
8 Motivation for Our Research in Autism
- Diagnosis of children with autism typically
occurs between 2 - 3 years of age - Diagnosis is rare before two years of age
- Yet the literature shows enhanced outcomes with
early identification and intervention - Thus, our ultimate goal is to identify children
with autism even earlier - But what are the barriers to earlier
identification?
9 Barriers To Early ID Include
- Difficulty identifying behaviors that could be
markers for group differences - Need to look for both presence of atypical
behaviors and absence of typical behaviors - Limited knowledge of developmental course of
behaviors that may be common in young children
(e.g., repetitive movements, mouthing) - Therefore, relatively late identification makes
it difficult to know the course of early
development in these children
10Retrospective Video Analysis
- Ecologically valid methodological tool for
earlier identification of children at very early
periods in development (prior to diagnosis) - Several retrospective video studies of autism
suggested young children with autism can be
distinguished from typically developing children
(Adrien et al., 1993 Baranek, 1999 Osterling
and Dawson, 1994)
11Retrospective Video Analysis
- UNC researchers have used footage that families
provided of their child under two years of age
(well before diagnosis). - Footage representative of a wide range of family
play situations. - Videotapes edited for randomly selected
cross-section of situations and events. - Studies include typically developing infants,
infants later diagnosed with autism, and infants
diagnosed with developmental delays at 9-12 and
15-18 months of age. - (Baranek, 1999 Watson, Crais, Baranek, Roy,
Dykstra, 2004 Lanter, Colgan, McComish, Watson,
Baranek, Crais, submitted).
12 Possible Behaviors of Interest
- Increasing interest and research in early
prelinguistic behaviors in children developing
typically and with autism - Areas of promise targeted today are gesture use,
play development, and relationship between play
behaviors and gesture use - Helpful to first be familiar with typical
development in these areas
13 Development of Gestures Why Are They Important?
- Early means to communicate
- One of the first signs of intentionality
- Can be used in profiling skills
- Can be helpful in identifying delays
- Important to intervention planning
14Gesture Development and Use
- Amount of gesture use can help distinguish
between late talkers and children with true
language deficits (Thal Tobias, 1992 1994). - Early onset of pointing predictive of advanced
language skills (Butterworth Morisette, 1996). - Use of distal (e.g., show, give, point) gestures
predictive of higher rate of communication
(McLean, McLean, Brady, Etter, 1991).
15 What Are Gestures?
- Gestures are actions produced with the intent to
communicate and are typically expressed using the
fingers, hands, and arms, but can also include
facial features (e.g., lip smacking for "eating")
and body motions (e.g., bouncing for horsie") - (Iverson Thal, 1998)
16 Age Ranges of Emergence of Common Gestures
- Reaching 6-9 months
- Giving 8-11 months
- Showing 8-13 months
- Pointing 9-14 months
- (Bates et al., 1975 Carpenter, Nagell,
Tomasello, 1998 Crais, Douglas, Campbell,
2004 Masur, 1983)
17 Functions of Gestures
- Social interaction initiating or sustaining a
social game or routine, providing comfort,
teasing, showing off - Behavior regulation regulate behavior of others
to obtain an object, get them to carry out
action, or stop someone from doing something - Joint attention direct others attention in
order to comment on an object or event, provide
information on an object or event, or acknowledge
shared attention to an object or event
18Age of Emergence of Functional Categories
- Protests 6-8 months
- Requests for actions 6-10 months
- Requests for objects 6-10 months
- Comments 8-11 months
- Answering 13-16 months
- (Carpenter, Mastergeorge, Coggins, 1983 Crais
et al, 2004)
19Array of Gestures Seen in 9-12 Month Old Children
(Crais et al, 2004)
- Behavior Regulation
- Protest (use body, push away object with hand/s)
- Request Objects (reach for object, pull on
adults hand with object, point to obtain object) - Request Actions (reach to be picked up, do the
action) - Social Interaction
- Seek Attention (body movement, grab hand, bang
object) - Social Games (participate by imitating, initiate
games) - Representational Gestures (bye bye, imitation
clapping, show functions of objects) - Joint Attention
- Comment (show object, give object)
20Array of Additional Gestures Seen in 15-18 Month
Old Children
- Behavior Regulation
- Protests (shake head no)
- Request Objects (reach while opening closing
hand) - Request Actions (point, take hand of adult, give
object) - Joint Attention
- Comment (point to object, point to object by
request) - Social Interaction
- Seek Attention (show off)
- Representational Gestures (hug objects, smack
lips, clap for excitement/accomplishment), - (Crais et al, 2004)
21 Results Comparing Group Means for Gesture
Functions at Time 1 (9-12 m)
totgest_1
ASDltTYP DD lt TYP
ASD lt TYP
(ASD n24, DD n14, TYP n22)
22Gesture Use In Infants Toddlers
- Results indicate differences in gesture use
between 9-12 month old infants later diagnosed
with autism or developmental disabilities and
children with typical development. - Total number of gestures (differences between
children with ASD TYP, DD TYP) - Group differences on behavior regulation joint
attention gestures, but not social interaction
(9-12 15-18 months)
23Gestures Seen in Sample
24Levels of Play Development
Level 0 No object play Exploratory the way
infant examines the environment in order to gain
information from objects or toys (e.g., mouthing,
banging, shaking, poking) Level I
(indiscriminate actions), Level 2 (simple
manipulations of single objects) Relational two
or more objects used in combination with one
another, without regard to attributes or
functions of objects (e.g., objects pushed,
stacked, nested, piled) Level 3 (taking objects
apart), Level 4 (general combinations)
25Levels of Play Development
Functional influenced by social or cultural
properties of objects (e.g., pretend actions,
spoon to dolls mouth) Level 5 (directed toward
object), Level 6 (toward self), Level 7 (toward
doll), Level 8 (toward other person) Symbolic
items, attributes not actually present, or
substitution of objects Level 9 (object
substitution), Level 10 (agent play), Level 11
(imaginary play) (Baranek, Barnett, Adams,
Wolcott, Watson, Crais, 2005 Belsky Most,
1981 Casby, 1991, Knox, 1997 Libby, Powell,
Messer, Jordan, 1998 Lifter, Sulzer-Azaroff,
Anderson, Cowdery, 1993)
26 Age Ranges of Play Levels
- Exploratory
- Level 1 (indiscriminate actions) 2 - 10
months - Level 2 (simple manipulations) 2 - 10 months
- Relational
- Level 3 (Takes objects apart) 10 - 18 months
- Level 4 (General combinations) 10 - 18 months
- Functional
- Level 5 (object directed) 12 18 months
27 Age Ranges of Play Levels
- Functional
- Level 6 (self directed) 12 18 months
- Level 7 (doll directed) 12 - 18 months
- Level 8 (other directed) 12 - 18 months
- Symbolic
- Level 9 (object substitution) 18 30 months
- Level 10 (agent play) 18 30 months
- Level 11 (imaginary play) 18 30 months
28 Concurrent Language/Play Associations
- 13 - 20 months
- First words appear along with more consistent
communicative gestures and single play schemes
(e.g., child feeds self with spoon) - 20 - 24 months
- Word combinations appear along with single play
schemes combined (e.g., child feeds self with
spoon, then drinks from cup). - (Kennedy, Sheridan, Radlinski, Beeghly, 1991)
29 Predictive Language/Play Associations
- Early skill with communicative gestures predicts
later language levels (Mundy Gomes, 1998 Thal,
Bates, Goodman, Jahn-Samilo, 1997) - Early functional object play has been associated
with later language ability (Lyytinen et al.,
1999 Ungerer Sigman, 1984) - Level of symbolic play exhibited is predictive of
later language skills (Lyytinen, Laakso,
Poikkeus, Rita, 1999 Lyytinen, Poikkeus, Laakso,
Eklund, Lyytinen, 2001).
30Longitudinal Relations between Play and Gesture
Behaviors in Infants with Autism (Watson, Crais,
Baranek, Roy, Dykstra, 2004)
- Examined predictive relations within and across
the domains of play and gesture from 9-12 to
15-18 months
31Subjects(Watson, Crais, Baranek, Roy, Dykstra,
2004)
- 27 children in three groups
- Autism spectrum disorder (n 15) DSM-IV
criteria, verified by CARS scores, and for 10 of
15 by ADI-R - DD (n 4) nonspecific, mixed diagnoses
- Typical (n 8)
32T1 to T2 Gesture Use (Watson et al. 2004)
- Figure 1 Mean Frequency of Gestures for Total
Sample (p.08)
33T1 to T2 Gesture Use (Watson et al., 2004)
- Figure 2 Mean Frequency of Gestures for ASD
Sample
34T1 to T2 Higher Level Play (Relational Play)
(Watson et al., 2004)
- Figure 3 Mean Second in Higher Level Play for
Total Sample
35T1 to T2 Higher Level Play (Relational Play)
(Watson et al., 2004)
- Figure 4 Mean Seconds in Higher Level Play for
ASD Sample
36Clinical Implications
- Preliminary guidelines for clinicians seeking
developmental models for both the range of
expected ages of emergence of targeted gestures
and play behaviors and their hierarchy in
typically developing children. - Clinicians can assess the depth and breadth of
gestures and play behaviors used by children
demonstrating communication delays - Hierarchy of gesture and play development can be
used in determining potential developmental
targets for intervention.
37Red Flags Approach Social Development
- Be concerned if the child is not By
- Responding to familiar adults with a social
smile 3 months - Talking back by vocalizing to familiar adults
who talk to child 8 months - Attentive to social games played by familiar
adults (e.g., Peek-a-Boo) 8 months - Participating in social games (e.g., hands up for
Pat-a-Cake) 12 months - Showing and/or giving objects to familiar
adults 15 months - Pointing to objects to indicate interest in
them 18 months - Seeking adult interaction to play with toys/look
at books 18 months - Pointing either spontaneously or by request to
pictures in books 24 months -
- General Social Concerns inattentiveness to
people, lack of eye contact or shared mutual gaze
with familiar adults by 12 months preferring to
play alone at 18 months or older social play is
limited to chase or tickle games at 24
months.
38Intentionality
- Be concerned if the child is not By
- Showing any type of intentional behavior (e.g.
requests, protests) 10 months - Communicating for a variety of reasons (e.g.,
protesting, requesting, - seeking social interaction, commenting) 18
months - Using a variety of means (e.g., gestures,
vocalizations, eye gaze) 18 months -
- General Intentionality Concerns children
who primarily regulate others behavior (e.g.,
putting someones hand on toy to operate it,
leading adult to door to open it), but do not
display more social forms of communicating (e.g.,
giving book to another to read, pointing to
objects of interest, drawing attention to self
for social reasons).
39Play Skills
- Be concerned if the child is not By
- Mouthing, banging, shaking, and/or manipulating
objects 8 months - Throwing, dropping toys especially for others to
get 12 months - Participating in social games (e.g., looks for
Mom during Peek-a-Boo). 12 months - Giving or showing toys to adults 15 months
- Pushing, pulling, turning on, putting in, and
taking out objects 18 months - Showing knowledge of how to use toys functionally
(e.g., push toy car) 18 months - Stacking cups or rings on a toy stand (not
necessarily correct) 24 months - Performing some pretend play behaviors (e.g.,
drinking from empty cup) 24 months - Combining play acts (e.g., rocks baby and puts
down for nap) 30 months - Playing with familiar children some of the time
when in close proximity 36 months - Taking turns in constructive or pretend play with
familiar children 48 months - General Play Concerns children who
primarily perform play behaviors representative
of younger children have limited play behaviors
(e.g., play with only one type of toy, play very
briefly with toys) seem averse to playing with
others (including caregivers) play alone for
longer periods of time than would be expected at
their developmental age have stereotypic play
behaviors (e.g., repeatedly open and close
drawers, line up their toys and become upset if
others change the order) or display unusual play
behaviors (e.g., rub plastic stacking rings over
their hands or face, lick and smell toys).
40Comprehension Skills
- Be concerned if the child is not By
- Looking at objects looked at by others 12
months - Acting on objects that are noticed 12 months
- Imitating ongoing actions 12 months
- Responding to own name 15 months
- Attending to an object mentioned 15 months
- Doing what is usually done in a situation (e.g.,
child puts on coat - when others do) 18 months
- Using conventional behaviors (e.g., combing hair
with comb) 18 months - Acting on objects as the agent (e.g., child
brushes own teeth - when asked to Brush the babys teeth) 24
months - Responding to many object names 24 months
- Retrieving a familiar object out of sight 24
months
41Sound Production
- Be concerned if the child is not By
- Producing cooing and gooing sounds 6 months
- Babbling in repeated sequences of sounds (e.g.,
baba, gaga) 11 months - Producing 3 different consonant sounds (e.g., b,
p, m, n, d, k, t, g, w) 18 months - Imitating any non-speech sounds (e.g., truck
sound, animal sounds) 18 months - Using sound sequences that sound like
talking 24 months - Producing (VC) syllables (e.g., up), (CVC)
syllables with a single - consonant (e.g., cake), and some vocalizations or
words - with two or more different consonants (e.g., pat,
tummy) 24 months - Producing 6 different consonants 24 months
- Producing initial consonants in most words (says
at for pat, ot for boat) 36 months - Producing 10 different consonants 36 months
- Producing any final sounds (bo for boat, e
for eat) 36 months
42Word Productions and Word Combinations
- Be concerned if the child is not By
- Producing any words or word approximations 18
months - Producing 50 words or word approximations 28
months - Producing 100 words 30 months
- Combining words 28 months
-
- E. Crais. (2001). Identifying communication and
related developmental disabilities in young
children. In J. Roush (Ed.), Screening for
hearing loss and otitis media in children. San
Diego, CA Singular
43Questions?