Title: Define intelligence in one sentence
1Define intelligence in one sentence What are
the top 5 skills to define intelligence?
2Outline
- Implicit theories
- Testing and scoring intelligence
- Intelligence scales
- Factor analysis of intelligence
- Biological bases of intelligence
- Alternative approaches
- Cultural contexts
- Multiple intelligences
3Experts definitions
- In 1921, when the editors of the Journal of
Educational Psychology asked 14 famous
psychologists What is intelligence. - The responses varied but they all generally
implied that intelligence involves 1) the
capacity to learn from experience and 2) the
ability to adapt to the surrounding environment. - Sixty-five years later (Sremberg Detterman,
1986), 24 cognitive psychologists with expertise
in intelligence research were asked the same
question. They, too, highlighted the importance
of learning from experience and adapting to the
environment. - They also broadened the definition to emphasize
the importance of metacognition, that is people's
understanding and control of their own thinking
processes. - Contemporary experts also more heavily emphasized
the role of culture, pointing out that what is
considered intelligent in one culture may be
considered stupid in another culture (Serpell,
2000).
4Experts definitions
Therefore, intelligence seems to be the capacity
to learn from experience, using metacognitive
processes to enhance learning, and the ability to
adapt to the surrounding environment, which may
require different adaptations within different
social and cultural contexts.
5Implicit theories
- All of us have our own implicit (unstated) ideas
about what it means to be smart that is, we have
our own implicit theories of intelligence. - We use our implicit theories in many social
situations, such as when we meet people or when
we describe people we know as being very smart or
not so smart. - Within our implicit theories, we also recognize
that intelligence has different meanings in
different contexts. A smart salesperson may show
a different kind of intelligence than a smart
neurosurgeon or a smart accountant, each of whom
may show a different kind of intelligence than a
smart choreographer, composer, athlete, or
sculptor.
6Implicit theories
- Implicit theories of intelligence may differ
from one culture to another. - For example, there is evidence that Chinese
people in Taiwan include interpersonal and
intrapersonal (self-understanding) skills as part
of their conception of intelligence (Yang
Sternberg, 1999). Rural Kenyan conceptions of
intelligence encompass moral as well as cognitive
skills (Grigorenko, et al., 2001). - Western cultures can attribute intelligence even
in the absence of moral rules. Machiavelli docet.
7Testing and scoring intelligence
- Explicit definitions of intelligence also
frequently take on an assessment-oriented focus. - In fact, some psychologists, such as Edwin
Boring (1923), define intelligence as whatever it
is that the tests measure. - This definition, however, is circular because
according to it, the nature of intelligence is
what is tested but what is tested must
necessarily be determined by the nature of
intelligence. - Moreover, what different tests of intelligence
test is not always the same thing, so it is not
feasible to define intelligence by what tests
test as though they all measured the same thing. - The attempt to understand intelligence by
measuring various aspects of intelligence has a
long history.
8Testing and scoring intelligence
- Go back to your list of top 5 skills.
- When making these assessments, were
psychophysical or judgment abilities more
important to you? - Contemporary measurements of intelligence
usually can be traced to one of two very
different historical traditions. - One tradition concentrated on lower-level,
psychophysical abilities (i.e., sensory acuity,
physical strength, and motor coordination) the
other focused on higher-level, judgmental
abilities (which we traditionally describe as
related to thinking).
9Testing and scoring intelligence
- Francis Galton (1822-1911) believed that
intelligence is a function of psychophysical
abilities, and for several years Galton
maintained a well-equipped laboratory where
visitors could be tested on a variety of
psychophysical tests such as weight
discrimination (the ability to notice small
differences in the weights of objects), pitch
sensitivity (the ability ro hear small
differences benveen musical notes) and physical
strength (Galton, 1883). - However, many attempt to unify the various
dimensions of psychophysically based intelligence
failed. For example, the psychophysical tests did
not predict college grades. Thus, the
psychophysical approach to assessing intelligence
soon faded almost into oblivion, although it
would reappear many years later.
10Testing and scoring intelligence
- An alternative to the psychophysical approach
was developed by Alfred Binet and Theodore Simon,
who also attempted to assess intelligence as they
were asked to devise a procedure for
distinguishing normal from mentally retarded
learners (Binet Simon, 1916). - Thus, Binet and his collaborator set out to
measure intelligence as a function of the ability
to learn within an academic setting. In Binet's
view, judgment, rather than psychophysical
skills, is the key to intelligence. - For Binet, intelligent thought (mental judgment)
comprises three distinct elements direction,
adaptation, and criticism.
11Testing and scoring intelligence
- Direction involves knowing what has to be done
and how to do it. - Adaptation refers to customizing a strategy for
performing a task and then monitoring that
strategy while implementing it. - Criticism is the ability to critique your own
thoughts and actions. - Initially, when Binet and Simon developed their
intelligence test, they were interested in some
means of comparing the intelligence of a given
child with that of other children of the same
chronological (physical) age, the so called
mental age.
12Testing and scoring intelligence
- William Stern (1912) suggested instead to
evaluate intelligence by using an intelligence
quotient (IQ) a ratio of mental age (MA) divided
by chronological age (CA), multiplied by 100.
- This ratio can he expressed mathematically as IQ
(MA/CA) x 100. When mental age exceeds
chronological age, the IQ score is above 100,
viceversa the IQ score is below 100. - For various reasons, ratio IQs, too, proved
inadequate. - The main is that increases in mental age slow
down at about age 16 years. What does a mental
age of 60 mean?!
13Intelligence scales
- Nowadays, the competitive Wechsler scales, are
probably even more widely used. - There are three levels of the Wechsler
intelligence scales, including the third edition
of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale
(WAIS-III), the third edition of the Wechsler
Intelligence Scale for Children (WISC-III), and
the Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of
Intelligence (WPPSI). - The verbal score is based on tests such as
vocabulary and verbal similarities. - The performance score is based on tests such as
picture completion, which requires identification
of a missing part in a picture of an object, and
picture arrangement, which requires rearrangement
of a scrambled set of cartoon-like pictures into
an order that tells a coherent story.
14Factor analysis of intelligence
- Psychologists interested in the structure of
intelligence have relied on factor analysis as
the indispensable tool for their research. - Factor analysis is a statistical method for
separating a construct -intelligence in this
case- into a number of independent factors (i.e.,
abilities) that are assumed to form the basis of
individual differences in test performance. - Charles Spearman usually is credited with
inventing factor analysis (Spearman, 1927). - Using this tool, he concluded that intelligence
can be understood in terms of both a single
general factor that pervades performance on all
tests of mental ability and a set of specific
factors, each of which is involved in performance
on only a single type of mental-ability test. - In Spearman's view, the general factor, which he
labeled g factor, provides the key to
understanding intelligence. Spearman believed "g"
to be the result of "mental energy". - Many contemporary psychologists still believe
Spearman's theory to be essentially correct
(e.g., Jensen, 1998 see essays in Sternberg
Grigorenko, 2002).
15Factor analysis of intelligence
- In contrast to Spearman, Louis Thurstone (1938)
concluded that the core of intelligence resides
in seven factors, which he referred to as primary
mental abilities. According to Thurstone, the
primary mental abilities are as follows - Verbal comprehension measured by vocabulary
tests. - Verbal fluency measured by time-limited tests
requiring the test-taker to think of as many
words as possible that begin with a given letter. - Inductive reasoning measured by tests such as
analogies and number-series completion tasks. - Spatial visualization measured by tests
requiring mental rotation of pictures of objects. - Number measured by computation and simple
mathematical problem-solving tests. - Memory measured by picture and word-recall
rests. - Perceptual speed measured by tests that require
the test-taker to recognize small differences in
pictures or to cross out the as in strings of
varied letters
16Factor analysis of intelligence
- At the opposite extreme from Spearman's single
g-factor model is J. P. Guilford's (1967, 1982,
1988) structure-of-intellect (SOI) model, which
includes up to 150 factors of the mind in one
version of the theory. - According to Guilford, intelligence can be
understood in terms of a cube that represents the
intersection of three dimensions operations,
contents, and products. - Operations are simply mental processes, such as
memory and evaluation (making judgments. - Contents are the kinds of terms that appear in a
problem, such as semantic and visual. - Products are the kinds of responses required,
such as units (single words, numbers, or
pictures), classes (hierarchies), and
implications. - Although considering as many as 100 factors can
be considered an exaggeration, Guilford's most
valuable contribution was to suggest that we
consider various kinds of mental operations,
contents, and products in our views and our
assessments of intelligence.
17Factor analysis of intelligence
- A more parsimonious way of handling a number of
factors of the mind is through a hierarchical
model of intelligence such as the one developed
by Raymond Cattell (1971). - This model proposes that general intelligence
comprises two major subfactors fluid abilities
(speed and accuracy of abstract reasoning,
especially for novel problems) and crystallized
abilities (accumulated knowledge and
vocabulary). - Subsumed within these two major subfactors are
other, more specific factors. - A similar view was proposed by Philip E. Vemon
(1971), who made a general division between
practical-mechanical (procedural) and
verbal-educational (declarative) abilities.
18Factor analysis of intelligence summary of models
19Biological bases of intelligence
- Although the brain is clearly the organ
responsible for intelligence, early studies
(e.g., those by Lashley) seeking biological
indices of intelligence and other aspects of
mental processes were a resounding failure,
despite great efforts. - More recent biological studies are largely
correlational, showing statistical associations
between biological and psychometric or other
measures of intelligence. These studies do not
establish causal relations! - One line of research looks at the relationship
of brain size to intelligence (see Jerison, 2000
Vemon et al., 2000).
20Biological bases of intelligence
- The evidence suggests that, for humans, there is
a modest but significant statistical relationship
between brain size and intelligence - and the
direction of the relationship is unknown. - Moreover, it probably is more important how
efficiently the brain is used than what its size
is. For example, on average, men have larger
brains than women, but women have better
connections through the corpus callosum. So it is
not clear which sex would be, on average, at an
advantage - and probably neither would be. - It is important to note that the relationship
between brain size and intelligence does not hold
across species (Jerison, 2000). Rather, what is
held seems to be a relationship between
intelligence and brain size, relative to the
rough general size of the organism.
21Biological bases of intelligence
- Some of the current studies offer some appealing
possibilities. For example, complex patterns of
electrical activity in the brain, which are
prompted by specific stimuli, appear to correlate
with scores on IQ tests (Barrett Eysenck,
1992). - Several studies, not fully confirmed, suggested
that speed of conduction of neural impulses may
correlate with intelligence, as measured by IQ
tests. - Surprisingly, neural-conduction velocity appears
to be a more powerful predictor of IQ scores for
men than for women, so sex differences may
account for some of the differences in the data
(Wickett Vemon, 1994), but even in this case
results are not definitive.
22Biological bases of intelligence
- An alternative approach to studying the brain
suggests that neural efficiency may be related to
intelligence such an approach is based on
studies of how the brain metabolizes glucose
during mental activities. - Haier and his colleagues (1992) have cited
several other researchers who support their own
findings that higher intelligence correlates with
reduced levels of glucose metabolism during
problem-solving tasks -that is, smarter brains
consume less sugar than do less smart brains
doing the same task. - Moreover, cerebral efficiency increases as a
result of learning on a relatively complex task
involving visuospatial manipulations (i.e., the
computer game Terns) - As a result of practice, more intelligent
participants show not only lower cerebral glucose
metabolism overall but also more specifically
localised metabolism of glucose. - Thus, more intelligent participants may have
learned how to use their brains more efficiently
to focus their thought processes on a given task.
23Biological bases of intelligence
- Some neuropsychological research (e.g.,
Dempster, 1991) suggests that performance on
intelligence tests may not indicate a crucial
aspect of intelligence the ability to set goals,
to plan how to meet them, and to execute those
plans. - Specifically, people with lesions on the frontal
lobe of the brain frequently perform quite well
on standardized IQ tests, which require responses
to questions within a highly structured
situation, but which do not require much in the
way of goal setting or planning. - If intelligence involves the ability to learn
from experience and to adapt to the surrounding
environment, the ability to set goals and to
design and implement plans is fundamental. - An essential aspect of goal setting and planning
is the ability to attend appropriately to
relevant stimuli and to ignore or discount
irrelevant stimuli according to the settings they
occur in. - Hence, many researchers and theorists urge us to
take a more contextual view of intelligence. This
leads straight to the final part of the lesson
24Alternative approaches to intelligence
- According to contextualists, intelligence cannot
be understood outside its real-world context. - The context of intelligence may be viewed at any
level of analysis, focusing narrowly, as on the
home and family environment, or extending
broadly, as on entire cultures. - For example, even cross-community differences
have been correlated with differences in
performance on intelligence tests. Differences
were found in rural vs. urban communities, low
vs. high proportions of teenagers to adults
within communities, and low vs high socioeconomic
status of communities (see Coon, Carey, Fulker,
1992). - Theorists who endorse the contextual model study
just how intelligence relates to the external
world in which the model is being applied and
evaluated. - But what prompted psychologists to believe that
culture might play a role in how we define and
assess intelligence?
25Cultural context and intelligence
- In a famous study of 1971, the investigators
asked adult members of the Kpelle tribe in Africa
to sort terms representing concepts. - In Western culture more intelligent people
typically will sort hierarchically. Less
intelligent people will typically sort
functionally. They may sort "fish" with "eat,"
for example, or they may sort "clothes" with
"wear". The Kpelle sorted functionallyeven after
investigators unsuccessfully tried to get the
Kpelle spontaneously to sort hierarchically.
Finally, in desperation, one of the experimenters
asked a Kpelle to sort as foolish person would
sort, and the Kpelle quickly and easily sorted
hierarchically.
26Cultural context and intelligence
- A study by Sarason and Doris (1979) provides a
more familiar example. - The researchers tracked the IQs of an immigrant
population Italian Americans. - Less than a century ago, first-generation
Italian-American children showed a median IQ of
87 (low average range 76-100), even when
nonverbal measures were used and when so-called
mainstream American attitudes were considered. - Some social commentate and intelligence
researchers of the day pointed to heredity and
other non environmental factors as the basis for
the low IQs. Stephen Ceci (1991) notes that the
subsequent generations of Italian-American
students who take IQ tests today show slightly
above-average IQs. Even the most fervent
hereditarians would be unlikely to attribute such
remarkable gains in one or two generations to
heredity. - Cultural assimilation, including integrated
education, seems a much more plausible
explanation.