Title: Predicting and Explaining Individual Performance in Complex Tasks
1Predicting and Explaining Individual Performance
in Complex Tasks
- Marsha Lovett, Lynne Reder, Christian Lebiere,
John Rehling, Baris Demiral
This project is sponsored by the Department of
the Navy, Office of Naval Research
2Multi-Tasking
- A single person can perform multiple tasks.
- A single model should be able to capture
performance on those multiple tasks. - A single person brings to bear the same
fundamental processing capacities to perform all
those tasks. - A single model should be able to predict that
persons performance across tasks from his/her
capacities.
3- A way to keep the multiple-constraint advantage
offered by unified theories of cognition while
making their development tractable is to do
Individual Data Modeling. That is, to gather a
large number of empirical/experimental
observations on a single subject (or a few
subjects analysed individually) using a variety
of tasks that exercise multiple abilities (e.g.,
perception memory, problem solving), and then to
use these data to develop a detailed
computational model of the subject that is able
to learn while performing the tasks.
Gobet Ritter, 2000
4- ZERO
- PARAMETER
- PREDICTIONS!
5Basic Goals of Project
- Combine best features of cognitive modeling
- Study performance in a dynamic, multi-tasking
situation (albeit less complex than real world) - Explain not only aggregate behavior but variation
(using individual difference variables) - Predict (not fit/postdict) complex performance
- Use cognitive architecture and fixed parameters
- Employ off-the-shelf models whenever possible
- Plug in individual difference params for each
person
6How to predict task performance
- Estimate each individuals processing parameters
- Measure individuals performance on standard
tasks - Using models of these tasks, estimate
participants corresponding architectural
parameters (e.g., working memory capacity,
perceptual/motor speed) - Build/refine model of target task
- Select global parameters for model of target task
(e.g., from previously collected data) - Plug into model of target task each individuals
parameters to predict his/her target task
performance
7Example Memory Task Performance
- Fit task A to estimate individuals parameters
8Zero-Parameter Predictions
- Plug those parameters into model of task B
(Lovett, Daily, Reder, 2000)
9Challenges of Complex Tasks
- Modeling the target task is harder
- More than one individual difference variable
likely impacting target task - Possibility of knowledge/strategy differences
10What about knowledge differences?
- Develop tasks that reduce their relevance
- Train participants on specific procedures
- Measure skill/knowledge differences in another
task and incorporate them in model - Use model to predict variation in relative use of
strategies by way of estimates of individuals
processing capacities
11Individual Differences in ACT-R
- Most ACT-R models dont account for impact of
individual differences on performance, but the
potential is there - There are many parameters with particular
interpretations related to individual difference
variables - Most ACT-R modelers set parameters to universal
or global values, i.e., defaults or values that
fit aggregate data
12ACT-R Individual Differences
P1, P2, P3,
M1, M2, M3,
W1, W2, W3,
13Overview of Talk
- Review tasks we are studying
- Illustrate methodology
- Highlight key results
- Visual search vs. memory strategies trade off in
final performance gt complex task modeling offers
best constraint with fine-grained analysis
14Modified Digit Span (MODS)
15Modified Digit Span (MODS)
16P/M Tasks
- In our earlier studies, initial training phase of
target task was used to collect data on
individuals perceptual/motor speed. - e.g., Time to find object A7 and click on it
- In later studies, separate task used to measure
perceptual and motor speed.
17How to predict task performance
- Estimate each individuals processing parameters
- Measure individuals performance on MODS,
PercMotor - Using models of these tasks, estimate
participants corresponding architectural
parameters (e.g., working memory capacity,
perceptual/motor speed) - Build/refine model of target task
- Select global parameters for model of target task
(e.g., from previously collected data) - Plug into model of target task each individuals
parameters to predict his/her target task
performance
18W affects Performance
- W is the ACT-R parameter for source activation,
which impacts the degree to which activation of
goal-related facts rises above the sea of other
facts activations - Higher W gt goal-related facts relatively more
activated gt faster and more accurately retrieved
gt better MODS performance
19Estimating W
- Model of MODS task is fit to individuals MODS
performance by varying W - Best fitting value of W is taken as estimate
20Estimating PM
- For simplicity, we estimated a combined PM
parameter directly from each individuals
perceptual/motor task performance. - This PM parameter was then used to scale the
timing of the target tasks perceptual-motor
productions.
21Joint Distribution of W and P/M
W and P/M are tapping distinct characteristics
22ACT-R Individual Differences
P1, P2, P3,
M1, M2, M3,
W1, W2, W3,
23Specifics of our Approach
- Estimate each individuals processing parameters
- Measure individuals performance on modified
digit span, spatial span, perceptual/motor speed - Using models of these tasks, estimate
participants W, P, M - Build/refine model of air traffic control
taskAMBR - Select global parameters for AMBR model
- Plug in individuals parameters to predict
performance across different AMBR scenarios
24AMBR Air Traffic Control Task
- Complex and dynamic task
- Spatial and verbal aspects
- Multi-tasking
- Testbed for cognitive modeling architectures
25AMBR TaskACaircraft, ATCair traffice controller
- As ATC, you communicate with AC and other ATC to
handle all AC in your airspace - Six commands with different triggers
- First ACCEPT, then WELCOME incoming AC (these two
separated by short interval) - First TRANSFER, then order a CONTACT message from
outgoing AC (these two separated by short
interval) - Decide to OK or REJECT requests for speed
increase - When a command is not handled before AC reaches
zone boundary, this is a HOLD (error)
26Issuing an AMBR Command
- Text message or radar cues particular action
- Click on Command Button
- Click on Aircraft (in radar screen)
- Click on Air Traffic Controller (if necy)
- Click on SEND Button
27(No Transcript)
28(No Transcript)
29General Methods
- Empirical Methods
- Day 1 Collect MODS and P/M data and train on
AMBR plus AMBR practice - Day 2 Review AMBR instructions, battery of AMBR
scenarios - Modeling Methods
- Use MODS PM data to estimate W and PM for each
subject - Plug individual W and PM values into AMBR model
- Compare individuals AMBR performance with model
predictions
30Experiments 1 2
- AMBR Scenario Design
- Experiment 1 alternating 5 easy, 5 hard
- Experiment 2 9 scenarios of varying difficulty
- AMBR Dependent Measures
- Total time to handle each command
- Number of hold errors
31Off-the-shelf ACT-R Model of AMBR
- Scan for something to do Radar, Left, Right,
Bottom text windows - When an action cue is noticed, determine if it
has been handled or not scan/remember - If the cue has not been handled, click command,
AC, ATC, SEND - Resume scanning
32Model Captures Range of Performance
33Model Predictions
- Prediction of whether a subject commits an error
in a scenario, based on scenario details and
individuals W P/M
34Indl Diffs Impact on Hold Errors
- Hold errors only weakly dependent on W, more
strongly on P/M and scenario difficulty
Hold Errors
Parameter Value
35Scenario Difficulty
Scenario
36Mean Errors by Scenario
Scenario
37Be Careful What (DM) you Model
- Error data too coarse to constrain model
- Even total RT/command data insufficient
- Model predicts that scanning strategy plays a
large role in performance. - This is consistent with participant reports who
may be doing any combination of visual search or
memory retrieval
38Observable Behaviors
- Subject
- T 0.0 Cue Accept T6?
- T 3.6 ACCEPT button
- T 5.9 AC T6
- T 6.7 ATC EAST
- T 7.7 SEND button
- Model
- T 0.0 Cue Accept T6?
- T 3.7 ACCEPT button
- T 5.7 AC T6
- T 7.0 ATC EAST
- T 8.2 SEND button
Stochastic variation on the single-action level
is part of subject and model behavior
39The Details Are Inside
- Model I/O
- T 0.0 Cue Accept T6?
- T 3.7 ACCEPT button
- T 5.7 AC T6
- T 7.0 ATC EAST
- T 8.2 SEND button
- Model Trace
- T 1.5 Notice cue
- T 2.5 Subgoal task
- T 3.7 Mouse click
- T 3.8 Start AC search
- T 4.9 Find AC
- T 5.7 Mouse click
- T 7.0 Mouse click
- T 8.2 Mouse click
40Conclusion thus far
- Visual search vs. memory strategies trade off in
final performance gt even when modeling a complex
task, coarse dependent measures (accuracy, total
RT) hide important details - Previous AMBR model fit group data well
- Only by seeking extra constraint of modeling
individual participants were important gaps in
model fidelity revealed
41Modifications for Experiment 3
- Use more fine-grained measures Action RT
Clicks - Modify the ATC task to increase memory demand
- More interesting for our purposes
- More realistic
- Lengthen scenario length so same planes are in
play - Hide AC names until click, then only after delay
- Use model to bracket appropriate difficulty level
42Raw Characteristics of Data
- Experiment 3
- Action RT 12.1 sec, Holds 3.3 / subject
- Action RT correlates with W (r -0.314) and Pm
(r 0.485) - Holds correlates with W (r -0.444) and Pm (r
0.508)
43Model Modifications
- Search not only can give the answer sought (a
specific ACs location) but an additional
rehearsal of that information - In slack times, possible strategy of studying
radar screen to rehearse AC names (called
exploratory clicks)
44Model Predicts Hold Errors
- Predicts errors per subject, r 0.81
- Hold errors depend more on W (compared to
previous version of task) but still mostly
dependent on PM and scenario difficulty - Move to modeling more fine-grained aspects of
data
45Model Predicts Number of Clicks
46(No Transcript)
47W, P/M affect RT click by click
Hi-Hi Model Subject
- Set W-P/M parameters in model corresponding to
participants (e.g., hi-hi lo-lo) - Run model to produce RT predictions click by
click (for 2 commands Accept and Contact)
Lo-Lo Model Subject
48W, P/M affect RT click by click
- Set W-P/M parameters in model corresponding to
participants - Run model to produce RT predictions click by
click (for 2 commands Accept and Contact)
49Conclusion thus far
- Modeling more fine-grained measures required task
and model modifications, but this produced
individual participant predictions that were very
promising. - Clicking on correct AC the first time ranges from
69 to 96 - Akin to remember vs. scan strategies
- Higher number -gt more (accurate) remembering
- This detailed aspect of performance relates to W
50Theoretical InterludeSpatial vs. Verbal WM
- Our working assumption (parsimoniously) posits a
single source activation parameter, W - W modulates the degree to which goal-relevant
facts are activated above the sea of unrelated
facts - regardless of spatial/verbal representation
- This perspective still allows for spatial/verbal
distinctions in performance but explains them as
a function of differences in spatial/verbal
skills etc.
51Opportunity to Test in Current Work
- AMBR task has spatial and verbal aspects
- Included verbal and spatial working memory tasks
in battery, starting with Experiment 3 - Which span task produces W estimates that best
predict individuals AMBR performance? - Spatial Span task from Miyake and Shah (1996)
R
R
R
normal
normal
reversed
52Opportunity to Test in Current Work
- Result
- Experiments 3 4 Spatial Span-based W predicts
AMBR performance better than MODS-based W - Possible explanations
- Spatial format more relevant for this task?
- Spatial Span shows more variability -gt more
sensitive? - Spatial Span variability taps other sources of
variation? - Are there separate Ws for verbal and spatial WM?
53Opportunity to Test in Current Work
- Result
- Experiments 3 4 Spatial Span-based W predicts
AMBR performance better than MODS-based W - Possible explanations
- Spatial format more relevant for this task?
- Spatial Span shows more variability -gt more
sensitive? - Spatial Span variability taps other sources of
variation? - Are there separate Ws for verbal and spatial WM?
54Spatial Span taps speed as well
- Another study, spawned by this issue, shows
relationship between individuals mental rotation
speed and Spatial Span - Pattern of correlations with PM
- MODS r.25 Spatial Span r.65
- Pattern of correlations with AMBR components
MemMouse
Mouse
Mouse
55Theoretical Interlude Conclusion
- Studying verbal vs. spatial memory resources in
context of AMBR task moves theoretical debate to
more realistic arena - This complements work with laboratory tasks and
allows greater potential for generalization of
results
56Strategic Variation Emerges
- Experiment 4 also revealed several sources of
strategic variation, explored further in
Experiment 5 - Waiting for AC name ranges from 42 to 100
- May reflect lack of confidence in memory, utility
of checking ones memory - Somewhat negatively correlated with W
- Initiating welcome and contact commands in
anticipation of text cue (ranges from 0 to 100) - Making exploratory clicks on ACs during slack
time (ranges from never to gt 5 per scenario)
57Experiment 5 Details
- Scenarios designed to have low (6 ACs) vs. high
memory load (total 12 ACs) - Speed requests most common command
- Most interesting for model predictions
- Least susceptible to snowball effects
- Dependent measures include RTs for individual
clicks and strategy use as a function of scenario
difficulty and command
58Modeling Specific AMBR Components
Hard Scenarios
Accuracy of first AC click
Easy Scenarios
Accuracy of first AC click
59Modeling Specific AMBR Components
Hard Scenarios
RT to Correct AC click
Easy Scenarios
RT to Correct AC click
60Model Predictions Match Data
- Main effects of scenario difficulty amplified for
low W individuals - Main effects of command type (more/less
memory-demanding) amplified for low W - Wait-for-AC-name strategy varied as a function of
command type - Exploratory clicks strategy varied as a function
of scenario difficulty
61Summary of Conclusions
- Complex tasks are not a modeling panacaea! Only
by seeking extra constraint of modeling
individual participants were important gaps in
models fidelity revealed. - Studying verbal vs. spatial memory resources in
context of AMBR task moves theoretical debate to
more realistic arena. - Variability in performance -- from different use
of strategies and/or from differences in
processing capacities -- is there for the
looking. Studying performance on average offers
incomplete understanding.
62(No Transcript)
63Features of Our Approach
- Our approach aims to jointly provide
- Predictions that are accurate and detailed
- At the individual participant level
- Generated in real time (or faster)
- Based on an interpretable model with variation in
meaningful individual difference parameters - That generalize to variants of the target task
64Joint Distribution of W and P/M
W and P/M are tapping distinct characteristics