Title: Li8 Structure of English
1Li8 Structure of English
- Evidence for morphological structure
2Morphology
- Maybe in order to understand mankind, we have to
look at the word itself. Mankind. Basically, its
made up of two separate words - mank and ind.
What do these words mean? Its a mystery, and
thats why so is mankind. - Jack Handey, Deep Thoughts
3Morphemes
- Morphology is the study of morphemes and their
behavior. - A morpheme is the minimal unit of meaning.
- dogs contains 2 morphemes
- 1 d?g quadruped of the genus canis
- 2 -z plural
- Note morphemes ? words
- A word is the smallest element that can occur in
isolation.
4Evidence for morphological decomposition and
storage
- New singulars
- New morphemes
- Psycholinguistic evidence
- Rule-governed behavior
- Priming studies
- Event Related Potentials
- Speech errors
- Disorders
5New singulars
6New morphemes
- -a/oholic
- -burger
- -gate
- Watergate, Fornigate
- -head
- pothead, metalhead, techhead, gearhead,
theoryhead, crackhead, hockey head - Mc-
- McMansion, mcjob, mcnews
It's true, I'm a Rageaholic...I just can't live
without Rageahol!
7McDonald's not lovin' McJob dictionary definition
- CHICAGO, Illinois (AP, 11-2003) -- McDonald's
says it deserves a break from the unflattering
way the latest Merriam-Webster's Collegiate
Dictionary depicts its job opportunities. Among
some 10,000 new additions to an updated version
released in June was the term "McJob," defined as
"low paying and dead-end work." In an open letter
to Merriam-Webster, McDonald's CEO Jim Cantalupo
said the term is "an inaccurate description of
restaurant employment" and "a slap in the face to
the 12 million men and women" who work in the
restaurant industry. The company e-mailed the
letter to media organizations Friday, and it also
was published in the Nov. 3 edition of an
industry trade publication. Cantalupo also wrote
that "more than 1,000 of the men and women who
own and operate McDonald's restaurants today got
their start by serving customers behind the
counter. McDonald's, the world's largest
restaurant chain, has more than 30,000
restaurants and more than 400,000 employees. Walt
Riker, a spokesman for McDonald's, said the Oak
Brook, Illinois-based fast-food giant also is
concerned that "McJob" closely resembles McJOBS,
the company's training program for mentally and
physically challenged people. - "McJOBS is trademarked and we've notified them
that legally that's an issue for us as well,"
Riker said.
McJob (m?kdZAb) n. A low-paying job that
requires little skill and provides little
opportunity for advancement. Source
Merriam-Webster Online
8Psycholinguistic evidence
9Over-regularization
- went ? goed ? went
- Happens c. late twos
- Suggests acquisition of morphological rules
10Wug tests
- Berko 1958
- 3 surface manifestations of English regular
plural /-z/ z, s, ?z - When/how do children learn these rules?
- Test paradigm
- Children are presented with a pretend creature
and told, "This is a wug." - Another wug is revealed, and the researcher says,
"Now there are two of them. There are two __." - Results
- Very young children are baffled by the question
and are unable to answer correctly, responding
with e.g. two wug." - Children in grade 1 were almost fully competent
with both s and z. - Both preschool and first-grade children dealt
poorly with ?z, giving the correct answer less
than half the time, possibly because it occurs in
the most restrictive context. - Major finding
- The first experimental proof that young children
have extracted generalizable morphological rules
from the language around them.
11Productive irregulars
- Al Jallad, Flint, and Richardson 2005
- Subjects come up with past tense for unfamiliar
V. - 1. Jim BLINGS everyday.
- Yesterday John __________________.
- 2. Alan CHINES everyday.
- Yesterday Alan __________________.
- Subjects in the experiment
- 26 children age 6-7 (oral test)
- 44 adults (written questionnaire)
- Sample Over-Irregularizations
- children
- pake ? pakeded
- bling ? blung, blang
- flink ? flunk
- frim ? frand
- adults
- mang ? mung
- shride ? shrude
- bling ? blank
Overall Rate of Irregular forms supplied
Individual Differences in Irregularization bars
show percentages of subjects making 0-20
irregulars, 20-40 irregulars etc.
12ERPs
- McKinnon et al. 2003.
- Observation
- Pronounceable non-words (flermuf) elicit
larger-amplitude N400 components than words
(muffler). - Prediction
- If readers treat non-words containing
non-productive morphemes (in-ceive) as unanalysed
wholes, then these non-words should elicit larger
N400s than matched words (receive). - Results
- Bound-stem non-words elicit a brain response
highly similar to that elicited by real words. - Conclusion
- Morphological decomposition and representation
extend to non-productive morphemes.
13BrothBrothelBrother
- Rastle et al. 2004
- Participants made visual lexical decisions to
stem targets preceded by masked primes sharing - (a) a semantically-transparent morphological
relationship with the target (e.g.,
cleaner-CLEAN) - (b) an apparent morphological relationship, but
no semantic relationship, with the target (e.g.,
corner-CORN) - (c) a non-morphological form relationship with
the target (e.g., brothel-BROTH). - Results showed significant and equivalent masked
priming effects in cases in which primes and
targets appeared to be morphologically related,
and priming in these conditions could be
distinguished from non-morphological form
priming. - These findings suggest a level of representation
at which apparently complex words are decomposed
based on their morpho-orthographic properties.
14Morphological speech errors
15Morphological errors
- Morpheme exchange
- slicely thinned
- Feature shift
- have to went for had to go
- Faulty access
- have teachen for have taught
- concludement for conclusion
- Experimentally controlled morphological errors
16Janssen and Humphreys 2002
- General logic
- If a string of phonemes with an identifiable
meaning can move independently during a speech
error, it is a production unit, with a separate,
independent representation at some cognitive
level. - Inflectional morphemes (e.g. -ed, -ing, -s) are
much more error prone than derivational morphemes
(e.g. -er, -ness, -able, -ion) (Garrett 1980,
Humphreys, 2002). - Can we also find experimental speech error
evidence of derivational decomposition? - Prediction
- If derivational morphemes are stored
independently from their stems, more
morphological speech errors should occur when -er
is a real morpheme than when it is only a
pseudo-morpheme.
17Method
- elicited morphological errors from speakers by
presenting a series of word quads, made up of two
pairs. Speakers read these words silently, after
which they disappeared. Speakers were
occasionally cued to respond aloud, as quickly as
possible, to the immediately preceding item.
Speakers had to then either repeat the previous
phrases exactly, or had to swap words between the
phrases. The critical pairs each consisted of an
initial ller word plus a critical word stem or
stemaffix.
18Results
- More affix errors occurred on the morphologically
complex forms than on the pseudo-morphs. - Conclusion derivational affixes are also stored
as separate morphemes.
19Morphological disorders
20Lexical disorder
- Badecker 2001
- CSS (65 years old) suffered a left
cerebro-vascular accident in 1990, resulting in
lexical impairments manifested in reading,
repetition, and oral and written naming tasks. - Apart from word-finding problems, CSSs spoken
output is fluent and he exhibits no apparent
comprehension impairment.
naming from definition Slender-bodied insect with
broad, often brightly-colored wings. butterfly
picture naming butterfly
monomorphemes 80 correct
doctor fly sun wheel pill
polymorphemes 50 correct!
21Semantic disorders
- Deep dyslexia semantic errors during reading
- Buchanan et al. 2003
- Deep dyslexic patient (JO, 48, L temporo-parietal
lobectomy) - Acquired reading disorder characterized by
production of - morphological errors (e.g., SLEEP read as
SLEEPING) - semantic errors (e.g. HEART read as BLOOD)
22Experiment
- Method
- Manipulate transparency in compound word naming
study - Results
- 94 stimuli
- 10 read correctly 9 of these were TT
- Transparent components read correctly more often
- Key point if there were no decomposition, all
compound types should show the same error rate - Response to PANCAKE
- cake, breakfast, man,. . .cake, man. . .cake,
mancake, man, man, p-, p-, cake, birthday,
breakfast, cake, p-a-n. . . cake, syrup... - Typical semantic error target cake ? output
birthday (shows decomposition) - NB 2nd and last attempts are semantic associate
for the whole compound (BREAKFAST, SYRUP) - Shows successful access of meaning of the
compound, even though she never manages to say
the whole thing - Conclusion
- JO can successfully parse compound into
constituents and access meaning of the whole the
errors occur after this point
23Deep dyslexia
- Rastle et al. 2005
- Observation
- Deep dyslexics often make morphological errors in
reading aloud - e.g. sexist read as sexy
- Two competing explanations
- representations are morphologically structured
- low imageability/frequency word modified to
visually similar word of higher
imageability/frequency) (Funnell 1987) - Prediction
- Theory 1 predicts more stem errors with
morphologically complex words (killer) than with
pseudosuffixed words (corner) and monomorphemic
words (cornea) when one equalizes for
imageability and frequency. - Theory 2 predicts no difference between these 3
categories. - Result
- Theory 1 (but not Theory 2) supported
24Regular vs. irregular
- Ullman et al. 1997, Ullman 2001 propose that
- the left inferior frontal cortex (along with the
basal ganglia) is involved in rule-based
computations - Irregular forms dealt with differently
- Prediction
- A lesion to this region should impair only
regular morphological processes, sparing the
ability to produce irregular morphology. - Test
- Four types of patients Parkinsons, Alzheimers,
anterior aphasics, and posterior aphasics - Patients were asked to perform a sentence
completion task requiring the production of a
verb in the past tense - E.g. Every day I dig a hole. Just like every
day, yesterday I ____ a hole - Results
- anterior aphasics and Parkinson patients
performed worse with regular than irregular verbs - the opposite for posterior aphasics and Alzheimer
patients - Conclusion
- Results consistent with theory that the left
frontal cortex and basal ganglia are involved in
rule-based language processing but not in the
processing of irregular morphology, and that
temporal lobe areas are implicated in the storage
of lexical forms. - But
25Regular vs. irregular
- Problem with Ullmans theory
- Agrammatic (Brocas) aphasics who perform worse
on irregular than regular morphological
transformations (producing irregular V forms) - patient RC (Shapiro Caramazza 2003)
- patient MR (Laiacona Caramazza 2004)
- Spanish-Catalan bilinguals (Caramazza et al.
2004) - Consistent with the classical view that left
inferior frontal cortex is involved in
morphosyntactic processing and not just in
rule-based transformations.
26Conclusions
- Humans decompose words into morphemes and store
them as such. - Both regular and irregular/restricted morphology
involve rule-governed (de)composition. - blang, inceive, Brocas aphasics
- Humans look for morphological structure even when
there isnt any. - cheeseburger, corner
27References
- Andrews, S. 1986. Morphological influences on
lexical access Lexical or nonlexical effects?
Journal of Memory and Language, 25, 726-740. - Andrews, S., B. Miller, K. Rayner. 2004. Eye
movements and morphological segmentation of
compound words There is a mouse in mousetrap.
European Journal of Cognitive Psychology
16285-311. - Badecker, William. 2001. Lexical composition and
the production of compounds evidence from errors
in naming. Language and Cognitive Processes 16.4. - Buchanan, Lori, Shannon McEwen, Chris Westbury,
and Gary Libben. 2003. Semantics and semantic
errors Implicit access to semantic information
from words and nonwords in deep dyslexia. Brain
and Language 846583. - Caramazza, Alfonso, Alessandro Laudanna, and
Cristina Romani. 1988. Lexical access and
inflectional morphology. Cognition 28. - Fiorentino, Robert. 2006. Masked priming of
compound constituents Implications for
morphological decomposition. Manuscript,
University of Maryland. - Janssen, Dirk and Karin Humphreys. 2002.
Morphological speech errors on agentive and
comparative affixes. Third International
Conference on the Mental Lexicon, Banff, Canada. - McKinnon, R., M. Allen, L. Osterhout. 2003.
Morphological decomposition involving
non-productive morphemes ERP Evidence.
Neuroreport 14883-886. - Marslen-Wilson et al. 1994. Psychol Rev 1013-33.
- Rastle, Kathleen, Matthew Davis, and Boris New.
2004. The Broth in my Brothers Brothel
Morpho-Orthographic Segmentation in Visual Word
Recognition. Psychonomic Bulletin Review
11.61090-1098. - Rastle, Kathleen, et al. 2005. New evidence for
morphological errors in deep dyslexia. Brain and
Language 97189-199. - Shapiro, K., Alfonso Caramazza. 2003. Looming a
loom Evidence for independent access to
grammatical and phonological properties in verb
retrieval. Journal of Neurolinguistics 1685-111.