Title: Gender Typicality in Children
1Gender Typicality in Childrens Spoken
NarrativesAcoustic and Perceptual
AnalysesStudents Heather Bauer, Alysse Zittnan,
Adviser Benjamin MunsonDepartment of
Speech-Language-Hearing Sciences, College of
Liberal Arts
- Gender Differences in Adults Speech
- Spoken language is highly variable. Variability
affects all aspects of language sentence
structures, discourse strategies, word choice,
and pronunciation. - Adult men and women pronounce speech sounds
differently from one another. These differences
are evident even in short stretches of speech,
and allow a talkers sex to be ascertained at
greater-than-chance levels even when other
aspects of linguistic variation are controlled - The differences are not solely the consequence of
anatomic and physiologic differences between the
sexes. We know this because - 1. They are language- and culture-specific (Van
Bejooijen, 1995) - 2. There is only a weak correlation between
variation in pronunciation and variation in the
morphology of the speech-production mechanism - 3. They are not across the board (Munson et
al., 2006). They are more evident in some sound
classes than in others. Moreover, there are - Gender Differences in Childrens Speech
- Adults perceive boys and girls to sound different
from one another well in advance of the sex
differentiation in the speech-production
mechanism that occurs at puberty (Perry, Ohde,
Ashmead, 2001 inter alia). - Gender differences in childrens pronunciation
are not across the board. As with adults, boys
and girls speech differs more in some sounds and
sound classes than others. - Ergo, sex differences are to represent learned
behaviors that are the result of selective
attention to and emulation of specific models in
the ambient language. - In our laboratory, we have studied gender
typicality in childrens speech by comparing the
productions of 5-13 year old boys with GID (i.e.,
boys whose gender expression has been deemed not
to meet cultural expectations) to children whose
gender expression is deemed to meet cultural
expectations (henceforth Expected Gender
Development EGD). This provides us with an
opportunity to study gender in speech without the
confounding influence of biological sex.
- Results Acoustic Analyses
- When the average acoustic characteristics of
vowels were considered, the only difference to
approach statistical significance was one measure
of formant frequencies of vowels. There were no
differences for the consonants s or sh - Regression analyses predicting perceived gender
typicality from acoustic variables found that
adult listeners paid particular attention to the
way the vowel /æ/ was spoken. There was a weak
association between the acoustic characteristics
of s and sh. This is consistent with
previous work on adults, in which it has been
shown that adult attend to the characteristics of
these sounds when judging the gender typicality
of adults' voices. Together, 41 of variance in
the ratings was accounted for.
- Speech Samples
- The recorded spoken narratives used in this
experiment were elicited by asking each child to
tell a story about a picture (see title banner).
This was taken from a standardized test of
childrens narrative abilities. Narratives were
elicited as part of a larger data-collection
protocol that included measures of single-word
productions and sentence repetitions. - Data from 28 children was used. Each speaker was
categorized as either younger (average age 7
years) or older (average age 10 years) and as
either GID or EGD. The diagnoses of GID were
made by psychometrists with expertise in
childrens gender development. We used seven
speakers from each category for a total of 28
narratives, varying in length from about 30
seconds to 1 minute.
- Results Perception Task
- Two, two-factor within-subjects ANOVAs examined
the influence of gender typicality (GID vs. EGD)
and age group on perceived gender and perceived
age. - For perceived gender, there was a significant
main effect of gender group, F1,24 118.2, p lt
0.001, partial h2 0.83. This interacted
significantly with age, F1,24 4.6, p 0.042,
partial h2 0.16. Figure 1 shows that the
interaction occurred because the older GID boys
sounded less boy-like than the younger GID boys,
while the older EGD boys sounded more boy-like
than the younger EGD boys. - For perceived age, there was a significant main
effect of both gender group (F1,24 19.2, p lt
0.001, partial h2 0.44) and age group (F1,24
178.4, p lt 0.001, partial h2 0.88). These
interacted significantly, F1,24 24.0, p lt
0.001, partial h2 0.50. Figure 2 shows that
the interaction occurred because the older GID
boys were rated to sound older than the older EGD
boys, while there were no differences for the
younger group.
- Acoustic Measures
- The acoustic analysis was conducted with the
Praat signal-processing program. - Our analysis focused on acoustic correlates of
the articulation of vowels and consonants,
including vowels fundamental frequency (the
acoustic parameter that is perceived as pitch),
vowels formant frequencies (the acoustic
parameters that are perceived as vowel quality),
and two consonants known to relate to the
perception of gender, s and sh. These
parameters were chosen based on previous studies
that compared speech differences of adult men and
women (e.g., Munson et al., 2006). - Inter-rater reliability was assessed by having
both students analyze randomly selected passages
independently and compare their measures.
.
Figure 6. Perceived Gender based on Single
Words, Sentences, and Narratives
Figures 1 and 2. Spectrograms of the word sun
produced by a Boy with EGD (left) and
GID (right). The green circle is the s sound
and the red square is the first two formant
frequencies for the u sound.
- A comparison with previous acoustic and
perceptual analyses of these childrens single
word and sentence productions data showed that
the ratings of the gender typicality of the
narratives better differentiated between the two
groups than the ratings gender from words or
sentences - However, the correlation between acoustic
measures and the perceptual measures was weaker
for the narratives than it was for the single
words or the sentences. - Listeners might attended to suprasegmental
features (i.e., intonation, rate of speech) or
narrative content more than pronunciation
- Discussion
- As expected, children with GID were rated as
sounding less boy-like than their EGD peers.
This tendency was stronger in the groups of older
(10 year old) boys than in the younger (7 year
old) boys. In fact, the older boys with GID were
rated to sound less boy-like than the younger
boys. - When compared with previous perceptual data,
stronger differentiation between groups was found
for the narratives than for the single word
productions or the sentences. This suggests that
the less-structured task of narrative production
gives children more opportunities to use
permissible linguistic variation to convey gender - At the same time, the fact that there was a
smaller correlation between acoustic measures and
perceptual measures for narratives than for
single words and sentences suggests that some of
the parameters that adults use to perceive gender
in childrens speech are not related to
pronunciation. - Our ongoing research is examining the extent to
which gender-typicality judgments are related to
judgments of childrens overall language ability.
Our perceived age findings suggests a positive
relationship between how boy-like a child sounds
and how developmentally advanced they are.
- Perception Task
- Twenty-five adult participants were asked to
listen to each narrative and provide a rating of
gender on a 6-point scale, where 6 indicated
"definitely a boy", 1 indicated "definitely a
girl", and 2 through 5 indicated various degrees
of uncertainty (see Figure 3). They also
provided an estimate of the child's age in years.
The order of presentation was fully randomized.
The task took place in a sound-treated booth.
Responses were logged using the E-Prime software. - Listeners were unaware that they were listening
to children with GID and children with EGD. They
were also not told that all of the children were
boys.
- Purpose
- To examine whether the spoken narratives of
children with GID differ from those of children
with EGD perceptually (i.e., do adults rate the
children as sounding different?) and acoustically
(i.e., can we find evidence that children are
producing speech differently?) - Compare the acoustic and perceptual measures to
previously made acoustic and perceptual measures
of these childrens productions of single words
and sentences, to examine the influence of
linguistic complexity on the expression and
perception of gender in childrens speech - Significance
- Childrens gender expression is evidence of
selective attention during language acquisition.
Most theories of language posit that children
treat the input uniformly. The results of this
research have the potential to question this
assumption, and suggests that models of language
acquisition must posit a stronger role for
selective attention during acquisition.
Figure 3. Perceived Gender Typicality
Figure 4. Perceived Age
Acknowledgements The research on the acoustic and
perceptual characteristics of the speech of
children with Gender Identity Disorder was
completed in collaboration initiated by J.
Michael Bailey of the Northwestern University
Department of Psychology. The funding for the
data collection phase of this project was
provided by a grant to Janet Pierrehumbert
(Department of Linguistics, Northwestern
University) and J. Michael Bailey from the
Northwestern University Office of the Vice
President for Research. The data-collection
protocol was designed by Janet Pierrehumbert,
Benjamin Munson, and Karla K. McGregor
(Department of Speech-Language Pathology and
Audiology, University of Iowa). Data collection
was supervised by Ken Zucker (Clark Institute for
Mental Heath, Toronto, Ontario). The acoustic
analysis protocol was developed by Benjamin
Munson and Janet Pierrehumbert. The perceptual
analysis protocol was developed by Benjamin
Munson. All of the acoustic and perceptual
analyses described herein were conducted at the
University of Minnesota. The authors of this
poster take sole responsibility for the
interpretations that are presented herein, and
acknowledges that the different collaborators on
this project might interpret these same findings
differently. References Munson, B., Jefferson,
S.V. McDonald, E.C. (2006). The influence of
perceived sexual orientation on fricative
perception. Journal of the Acoustical Society of
America, 119, 2427-2437 Perry, T.L., Ohde, R.,
Ashmead, D. (2001). The acoustic bases for
gender identification from children's voices.
Journal of the Acoustical Society of America,
109, 2988-2998. Van Bezooijen, R. (1995).
Sociocultural aspects of pitch differences
between Japanese and Dutch women. Language and
Speech, 38, 253-265.