Title: Sketch of Melanau Morphology
1Sketch of Melanau Morphology
2Preliminaries Morphology and the Comparative
Method
- The CM is designed to explain regular
correspondences (correspondence sets) and at the
same time to organize the exceptions (also called
irregularities). - The three main causes of irregularity are
borrowing, analogy, and archaic residue. - There are others, including free variation and
partial lexical diffusion of a sound change.
3Three causes of irregularity
- Borrowing introduces words into the language that
have not developed organically from the
proto-language. - Analogy can alter or erase the effects of sound
changes, inducing phonological irregularity in
service to morphological regularity. - Free variation signals a potential sound change
in progress. At first, an innovating variant may
appear as an irregularity. In time, it may
become the only variant, which will then be
regular.
4Analogy
-
- Todays topic is analogy as the cause of certain
irregularities in the phonological comparisons. - The topic is so named because in large part,
grammatical change operates on the Principle of
Analogy. (Crowley, Chapter 7, p. 150)
5Example of how analogy works
- Analogy is a logical operation that yields
grammatical information even when lexical
information is lacking. - This is a wug. Over here are two _______.
- Tommorrow I will wug you.
- Ted is _________ you at this moment.
- Yesterday Beatrice ________ you at 900 a.m.
- Has she _______ Phillip yet?
6Child Language Acquisition
- Jean Berko Gleason (1958) found that six-year old
children tended overwhelmingly to give regular
noun and verb patterns when presented with
Analogy tests. - They rarely offered irregular patterns such as
wug/wug/wug on the pattern of cut/cut/cut. - http//www.bu.edu/psych/faculty/gleason/
7Historically, analogy begins in the mouths of
babes and can cause the loss of irregular
allomorphs.
- I falled down and hurted myself.
8Old English had a number of declensions each with
its own regular plural suffix.
- Old English Modern English
- hand-a hand-s
- gear year-s
- eag-an eye-s
- stan-as stone-s
- Arlotto (1972132)
9Back to Melanau
10Affix types
- Proto-Malayo-Polynesian had prefixes, infixes,
and suffixes. - Melanau dialects appear to have prefixes and
infixes, but no suffixes. (Perhaps Kanowit has
vestiges of suffixes, called archaic residues.) - It seems that suffixes were lost on the path from
PMP to PM.
11Prefixes
- Prefixes in Melanau resemble those reflected in
hundreds of languages of the Philippines,
Malaysia and Indonesia. - Melanau prefixes have the shape C- or C?- or
C?N-, where Cbilabial or nasal, and N undergoes
systematic alternation (m, n, n, ?) as
determined by the initial consonant of the stem.
12Prefixal C(?)-
- p(?)- nominals are semantically unpredictable.
- p(?)- verbs are causative.
- p?N- nouns and adjectives are instrumental
meaning for. Allomorphs include p?m-, p?n,
p?n- and p??-. - b(?)- verbs are intransitive meanings range from
having to doing. - s(?)- adjectives and nouns denote wholeness
- t(?)- verbs and adjectives denote stative
13Prefixal nasals with verbs
- m(?)- transitive verb, active voice.
Allomorphs include an infix -u-. - n(?)- transitive verb, passive voice.
Allomorphs include an infix -i-. - (m?)N- intransitive verb, activity or motion.
Allomorphs include m?m-, m?n, m?n-, m??-, m?-
and m-, n- n-, ?-.
14m?- m- -u-
15Back Formation /p/ gt p and /b/ gt b
16Blusts Infixing Rules for Mukah Melanau
- The active-transitive verb morpheme manifests as
infix -u-, and the passive manifests as i-, when
the stem-initial consonant is a voiceless
obstruent and the adjacent penult vowel is schwa,
e.g. t?bas tubas tibas clear-cut. - The active infix -u- derives from PMP -um-, and
the passive infix -i- derives from PMP -in-.
17Melanau passive infix -i-
18Nasal Substitution
- NS applies to p?N- and m?N- alike.
19Nasal Substitution
- NS rule prefixal N- assimilates to the point of
articulation of a voiceless obstruent, and then
the latter disappears. - Belawi
- p?N- sapaw gt p?n-sapaw gt p?n-apaw
- Kanowit
- p?N-sabo? gt N-sabo? gt n-sabo? gt n-abo?
20Compounding and Reduplication
21A Note on Austronesian Root Theory
-
- Many researchers (Gonda 1969, Blust 1988, Wolff
19xz) have attempted to recover a number of
monosyllabic roots that appear as recurring
partials in many words across the language
family. Such monosyllables cannot be
reconstructed for Proto-Malayo-Polynesian or
Proto-Austronesian, because virtually all of the
reconstructable vocabulary is disyllabic. - Therefore these roots remain largely
unaccounted for.
22Possible examples
23This is just a taste of Melanau morphology.
- Summary Morphological analogy, as a force of
grammatical change, can alter or erase the
effects of phonological change. - Like borrowing, analogy is used by historical
phonologists to explain apparent irregularities.
24What to do about a suspected analogical formation?
- If a word-part is affected by analogy, it is set
aside for the moment. Only the remainder
(usually the stem) is grist for the phonological
mill. -
-
25(No Transcript)
26Nature of Internal Reconstruction
- Notice that formally speaking, internal
reconstruction differs little from (synchronic)
morphophonemics, whose goal is to posit an
underlying form. In historical phonology, the
same kind of result is interpreted as the older
form, from which the attested data is derived via
grammatical or phonological change.
27Ablaut problem
28(No Transcript)
29(No Transcript)
30This material is intended to illustrate the
principles presented in Chapter 6 (and a tiny bit
of Chapter 7) of our textbook.