GroupLevel Measurement - PowerPoint PPT Presentation

1 / 51
About This Presentation
Title:

GroupLevel Measurement

Description:

Burgeoning of multilevel theory and research in last 25 years ... 1 psychologist, 1 sociologist, 1 anthropologist, etc. Team D. Members differ in their rank ... – PowerPoint PPT presentation

Number of Views:47
Avg rating:3.0/5.0
Slides: 52
Provided by: davidmoo
Category:

less

Transcript and Presenter's Notes

Title: GroupLevel Measurement


1
Group-Level Measurement
  • Katherine Klein
  • University of Pennsylvania
  • kleink_at_wharton.upenn.edu
  • CARMA Presentation
  • February 2007

2
Why Group-Level Measurement?
  • Burgeoning of multilevel theory and research in
    last 25 years
  • Great progress in conceptualizing and measuring
    group-level constructs
  • Especially shared constructs
  • Continuing challenges and opportunities
  • Especially regarding configural constructs

3
A Few Terms and Assumptions
  • Ill refer to groups but much or all of what I
    say will apply as well to organizations,
    departments, stores, etc.
  • Ill focus on the creation and use of original
    survey measures to assess group constructs.
  • Ill address statistical issues in passing only.
  • But see past CARMA presenters including James
    LeBreton, Gilad Chen, Paul Bliese, Dan Brass,
    Steve Borgatti, and others

4
Roadmap
  • Fundamentals Theory First
  • Construct Types Global, Shared, and Configural
    Constructs
  • Practicalities and Technicalities
  • Survey Wording
  • Sampling
  • Qualitative Groundwork
  • Single-source Bias
  • Justifying Aggregation
  • Opportunities and Challenges
  • The Configuration of Diversity
  • Social Network Analysis

5
Fundamentals Theory First
  • Constructs are our building blocks in developing
    and in testing theory.
  • High quality measures are construct valid.
  • The development of construct valid measures thus
    begins with careful construct definition.
  • Group-level constructs describe the group as a
    whole and are of three types (Kozlowski Klein,
    2000)
  • Global, shared, or configural.

6
Global Constructs
  • Relatively objective, easily observable,
    descriptive group characteristics.
  • Originate and are manifest at the group level.
  • Examples
  • Group function, size, or location.
  • No meaningful within-group variability.
  • Measurement is generally straightforward.

7
Shared Constructs
  • Group characteristics that are common to group
    members
  • Originate in group members attitudes,
    perceptions, cognitions, or behaviors
  • Which converge as a function of attraction,
    selection, socialization, leadership, shared
    experience, and interaction.
  • Within-group variability predicted to be low.
  • Examples
  • Group climate, norms, leader style.
  • Measurement challenges are well understood.

8
Configural Group-Level Constructs
  • Group characteristics that describe the array,
    pattern, dispersion, or variability within a
    group.
  • Originate in group member characteristics (e.g.,
    demographics, behaviors, personality, attitudes)
  • But no assumption or prediction of convergence.
  • Examples
  • Rates, diversity, fault-lines, social networks,
    team mental models, team star or weakest member.
  • Measurement challenges are less well understood.

9
A Related Framework Chans (1988) Composition
Typology
  • Shared Constructs
  • Direct consensus models (e.g., group norms)
  • Referent shift models (e.g., team efficacy)
  • Configural Constructs
  • Dispersion model (e.g., climate strength)
  • Additive models (e.g., mean group member IQ)
  • Multilevel, Homologous Models
  • Process model (e.g., efficacy-performance
    relationship)

10
Construct Definition ComplexitiesAn Example
Shared Leadership
  • Shared leadership
  • A dynamic, interactive influence process among
    individuals in work groups in which the objective
    is to lead one another to the achievement of
    group goals It involves peer, or lateral,
    influence and at other times involves upward or
    downward hierarchical influence
  • Conger Pearce, 2003, p. 286
  • Is this a shared construct, or a configural
    construct, or ?

11
Construct Definition ComplexitiesAn Example
Shared Leadership
  • Well, how would you measure it?
  • Shared team leadership as a shared construct
  • Team members share in the leadership of this
    team.
  • Many team members provide guidance and direction
    for other team members.
  • Shared team leadership as a configural construct
    (network density)
  • To what extent do you consider _____ an informal
    leader of the team?

12
Construct Definition ComplexitiesAn Example
Shared Leadership
  • Calling it a referent shift construct is not
    the answer.
  • Referent shift is a measurement strategy, not a
    construct type
  • Shifting the referent in an unthinking manner can
    be quite problematic
  • The members of my team
  • Express confidence that we will achieve our
    goals
  • Will recommend that I am compensated more if I
    perform well
  • Are friendly and approachable
  • Rule with an iron hand

13
A Quick Recap
  • Theory first Define and explain the nature of
    your group-level constructs.
  • Is it a clearly objective description of the
    group?
  • If yes, a global construct.
  • Do you expect within-group agreement?
  • If yes, a shared construct.
  • Does it describe the group in terms of the
    pattern or array of group members on a common
    attribute?
  • If yes, a configural construct.

14
Now What?
  • Having defined your constructs, the goal is to
    create measures that
  • Are construct valid
  • Show homogeneity within (shared constructs)
  • Show variability between (all group-level
    constructs)
  • Practicalities and technicalities
  • Survey wording
  • Sampling
  • Qualitative groundwork
  • Minimizing single-source bias
  • Testing for aggregation

15
Survey WordingGlobal Constructs
  • Draw attention to objective descriptions of each
    group.
  • Gather data from experts and observers (SMEs) who
    can provide valid information about the groups in
    question.
  • No need to gather data from individual
    respondents within groups
  • Use language that fits your sample.

16
Survey Wording Shared Constructs
  • Draw attention to shared group characteristics
  • Use a group referent rather than individual
    referent to enhance
  • Within group agreement
  • Between group variability
  • Predictive validity
  • Gather data from individual respondents so
    within-group agreement can be assessed.
  • Actual consensus methods (discussion prior to
    group survey completion) work well but are
    labor-intensive.

17
Survey WordingConfigural Constructs
  • Draw attention to individual group member
    characteristics by using an individual referent.
  • Gather data from experts and observers (SMEs) who
    can provide valid information regarding
    individual group members, or gather data from
    individual respondents within groups.
  • The challenge is perhaps less in the survey
    wording than in operationalizing the array or
    pattern of interest.

18
Sampling
  • Substantial between-group variability is
    essential. Seek samples in which groups vary
    considerably on the constructs of interest
  • Whether they are global, shared, or configural.
  • Statistical power reflects both
  • Group sample size (n of groups)
  • Within-group sample size
  • When group size is large (number of respondents
    per group), measures of shared constructs are
    more reliable.
  • More research needed on power in multilevel
    analyses.

19
Qualitative Groundwork
  • The survey wording and sampling guidelines seem
    fairly obvious and easy, but
  • Check your assumptions in the field prior to
    survey data collection.
  • Are you measuring the right groups?
  • Example Grocery stores or departments?
  • Is there meaningful between-group variability?
  • Example Fast food chain
  • Are you measuring the right variables, and not
    too many of them?
  • Beware the blob.

20
Single-Source Bias
  • Group-level correlations between measures of
    shared group constructs may be disturbingly high.
  • Examples
  • Transformational and transactional leadership
  • Task, emotional, and procedural conflict
  • Aggregation does not average away response
    biases.
  • Rather, group members may share response biases
  • Halo, logical consistency, social desirability
  • Response bias may be particularly influential
    when respondents must make subtle distinctions
    among constructs.

21
Single-Source BiasBeating the Blob
  • Survey measures
  • Choose and measure truly distinct constructs
  • Use different survey response formats
  • Survey design
  • Keep survey items measuring distinct constructs
    separate.
  • Help respondents recognize the distinction
    between leadership types, or conflict types, for
    example.

22
Single-Source BiasBeating the Blob
  • Survey analysis
  • Randomly split the within-group sample of
    respondents during data analysis.
  • All receive the same survey, but half provide IV
    and the other half provide the DV for analyses
  • Survey administration
  • Randomly split the within-group sample of
    respondents during data administration.
  • Respondents receive distinctive surveys. Half
    receive the IV survey and the other half receive
    the DV survey.

23
A Quick Recap
  • Having
  • Defined our constructs
  • Written our survey items
  • Conducted qualitative groundwork
  • Sampled appropriately
  • Taken steps to reduce single source bias
  • Were almost ready for hypothesis testing
  • But first We need to justify aggregation

24
Justifying Aggregation
  • Why is this essential?
  • In the case of shared constructs, our very
    construct definitions rest on assumptions
    regarding within- and between-group variability.
  • If our assumptions are wrong, our construct
    theories, our measures, and/or our sample are
    flawed and so are our conclusions.
  • So, test both
  • Within group agreement
  • The construct is supposed to be shared, but is it
    really?
  • Between group variability (reliability)
  • Groups are expected to differ significantly, but
    do they really?

25
Justifying Aggregation rwg(j)
  • Developed by James. Demaree, Wolf (1984)
  • Assesses agreement in one group at a time.
  • Compares actual to expected variance.
  • Answers the question
  • How much do members of each group agree in their
    responses to this item (or this scale)?
  • Highly negatively correlated with the within
    group standard deviation
  • Valid values range from 0 to 1
  • Rule of thumb rwg(j) of .70 or higher is
    acceptable

26
Justifying Aggregation rwg
  • Common to report average or median rwg(j) for
    each group for each variable
  • If rwg(j) is below .70 for one or more groups,
    check
  • Does the group have low rwg(j) values on several
    variables?
  • Do many groups have low rwg(j) values on this
    variable?
  • Remember rwg(j) indicates within-group
    agreement, not between-group variability.
  • Beware When variance in a group exceeds
    expected variance, out of range rwg(j) result.

27
Justifying Aggregation h2
  • Assesses between-group variance relative to total
    variance, across the entire sample.
  • Based on a one-way ANOVA
  • Answers the question
  • To what extent is variability in the measure
    predictable from group membership?
  • The F-test provides a test of significance
  • The larger the sample of individuals, the more
    likely eta2 is to be significant.
  • Beware h2 may be inflated when group sizes are
    small (under 25 individuals per group)
  • But, this is an easy way to begin tests of
    aggregation

28
Justifying Aggregation ICC(1)
  • Assesses between-group variance relative to total
    variance
  • Based on a one-way ANOVA
  • Answers the question
  • To what extent is variability in the measure
    predictable from group membership?
  • The F-test provides a test of significance
  • Based on h2 but controls for the number of
    predictors relative to the total sample size, so
    ICC(1) is not biased by group size.

29
Justifying Aggregation ICC(2)
  • Assesses the reliability of the group means
    (i.e., between-group variance) in a sample, based
    on ICC (1) and group size.
  • Answers the question
  • How reliable are between-group differences on the
    measure?
  • Reflects ICC(1) and within-group sample size
  • Example If ICC(1) .20 and
  • Mean group size is 5, expected ICC(2) .56
  • Mean group size is 20, expected ICC(2) .71

30
Justifying Aggregation An Example
31
A Quick Recap
  • The hope is that we have successfully
  • Defined our constructs.
  • Written our survey items.
  • Conducted qualitative groundwork.
  • Collected data from a large sample of groups.
  • Taken steps to reduce single source bias.
  • Justified aggregation.
  • And moved on to test our hypotheses.
  • So, what remains?

32
Opportunities and ChallengesThe Configuration
of Diversity
  • Configural constructs describe the array,
    pattern, dispersion, or variability within a
    group.
  • The easy example is diversity
  • Demographic diversity
  • Climate strength
  • But even the easy example isnt so easy What is
    the definition of diversity? And how should it
    be measured?

33
The Configuration of Diversity
  • A starting definition of diversity
  • The distribution of differences among the members
    of a group with respect to an attribute, X, such
    as age, ethnicity, conscientiousness, positive
    affect or pay.
  • Okay, but whats maximum diversity?
  • Which team has maximum age diversity?
  • 20, 20, 20, 70, 70, 70
  • 20, 30, 40, 50, 60, 70
  • 20, 20, 20, 20, 20, 70
  • 20, 70, 70, 70, 70, 70

34
The Configuration of Diversity
  • Diversity isnt one thing.
  • Its three things Separation, Variety, or
    Disparity
  • The three types differ in
  • Meaning or substance
  • Pattern or shape
  • Likely consequences
  • Appropriate operationalization
  • Blurring across these distinctions leads to fuzzy
    theory, misguided operationalizations, and
    potentially invalid research conclusions

35
The Configuration of DiversityExample Three
Research Teams
  • Team S
  • Members differ in their view of qualitative
    research.
  • Half of the team members respect it, half dont.
  • Team V
  • Members differ in their discipline.
  • 1 psychologist, 1 sociologist, 1 anthropologist,
    etc.
  • Team D
  • Members differ in their rank
  • 1 senior professor, others are incoming graduate
    students.

36
Diversity as Separation
  • Differences in group members position, attitude,
    or opinion along a continuum
  • Min Every member has the same opinion
  • Max Two polarized extreme factions
  • Theory Similarity-attraction
  • Operationalization Standard deviation

37
Diversity as Variety
  • Differences in kind or category
  • Min Every member is the same type
  • Max Each group member is a different type
  • Theory Requisite variety, cognitive resource
    heterogeneity
  • Operationalization Blaus index of categorical
    differences

38
Diversity as Disparity
  • Differences in concentration or proportion of
    valued assets or resources
  • Min Every member has an equal portion of the
    resource
  • Max One member is rich and all others are
    impoverished
  • Note Disparity is asymmetric
  • Theory Inequality, relative deprivation,
    tournament compensation
  • Operationalization Coefficient of variation
    (SD/Mean)

39
The Configuration of DiversityA Recap
  • Theory first
  • Separation is about position, attitude, or
    opinion
  • At maximum Polarized factions
  • Variety is about knowledge or information.
  • At maximum One of a kind
  • Disparity is about resources or power.
  • At maximum One towers over others
  • Operationalize accordingly
  • The coefficient of variation is not a default or
    catch-all

40
Opportunities and Challenges Social Network
Analysis
  • Multilevel analysis and social network analysis
    have developed along separate paths.
  • Rich opportunities for cross-fertilization.
  • Social network analysis provides a means to
    conceptualize and operationalize configural
    constructs.
  • Illuminating the pattern or array of
    interpersonal ties within a group

41
Opportunities and Challenges Social Network
Analysis
  • Many of our shared constructs appear to rest on
    tacit, often fuzzy, assumptions about
    interpersonal ties with groups.
  • Examples Cohesion, communication, coordination,
    knowledge sharing, shared leadership, conflict
  • But we know little about the configuration of
    interpersonal ties the structures that
    underlie our shared constructs and measures.

42
An Example Social Network Analysis and Shared
Team Conflict
  • When teams report high task or emotional
    conflict, what is the structure of interpersonal
    ties within the team?
  • As a starting point
  • How dense are positive (advice) ties?
  • How dense are negative (difficulty) ties?

43
An Example Social Network Analysis and Shared
Team Conflict
  • Task and emotional conflict The blob
  • r .83
  • Advice density and negative tie density More
    weakly correlated
  • r -.36
  • Task conflict (mean task and emotional conflict),
    advice density, and negative tie density
  • Team Conflict and Advice Density r -.47
  • Team Conflict and Difficulty Density r .40

44
Negative Tiesin a Low Conflict Team
45
Negative Ties in a High Conflict Team
46
Advice Ties in a High Conflict Team
47
Advice Ties in a Low Conflict Team
48
Social Network AnalysisA Recap
  • Social network analysis illuminates the
    configuration of interpersonal ties in groups.
  • What network structures underlie our shared
    constructs and measures?
  • Do network measures provide incremental validity?
  • Not just density, but centralization, cliques,
    and more.
  • What explains between-group differences in
    network structures?

49
In Conclusion
  • Theory first. Define your constructs.
  • Are they global, shared, or configural?
  • Measure constructs and collect data with care
  • Match item wording to the construct
  • Conduct qualitative groundwork
  • Sample appropriately
  • Take steps to reduce single source bias
  • Test for aggregation
  • Studying configural constructs remains a
    challenge and an opportunity
  • Conceptualizing and measuring diversity
  • Integrating social network analysis within our
    arsenal

50
Some Helpful References
  • Bliese, P. D. (2000). Within-group agreement,
    non-independence, and reliability Implications
    for data aggregation and analysis. In K. J. Klein
    S. W. J. Kozlowski (Eds.), Multilevel theory,
    research and methods in organizations (pp.
    349-381). San Francisco Jossey-Bass.
  • Borgatti, S. P. (2003). The network paradigm in
    organizational research A review and typology.
    Journal of Management, 29, 991-1013.
  • Chan, D. (1998). Functional relations among
    constructs in the same content domain at
    different levels of analysis A typology of
    composition models. Journal of Applied
    Psychology, 83, 234-246.
  • Harrison, D. A. Klein, K. J. (2007). Whats
    the difference? Diversity as separation,
    variety, or disparity in organizations. Academy
    of Management Review.
  • Harrison, D. A. McLaughlin, M. E. (1996).
    Structural properties and psychometric qualities
    of organizational self-reports Field tests of
    connections predicted by cognitive theory.
    Journal of Management, 22, 313-338.
  • James, Demaree, Wolf, G. (1984). Estimating
    within-group interrater reliability with and
    without response bias. Journal of Applied
    Psychology, 69, 85-98.

51
Some Helpful References
  • Klein, K. J., Conn, A. B., Smith, B., Sorra, J.
    S. (2001). Is everyone in agreement? An
    exploration of within-group agreement in employee
    perceptions of the work environment. Journal of
    Applied Psychology, 86, 3-16.
  • Klein, K. J., Conn, A. B. Sorra, J. S. (2001).
    Implementing computerized technology An
    organizational analysis. Journal of Applied
    Psychology, 86, 3-16.
  • Kozlowski, S. W. J. Klein, K. J. (2000). A
    multilevel approach to theory and research in
    organizations. In Klein, K. J. Kozlowski, S. W.
    J. (Eds.), Multilevel theory, research, and
    methods in organizations (pp. 3-90). San
    Francisco Jossey-Bass.
  • Morgeson, F. P. Hofmann, D. A. (1999). The
    structure and function of collective constructs
    Implications for multilevel research and theory
    development. Academy of Management Review, 24,
    249-265.
  • Ostroff, C., Kinicki, A. J., Clark, M. A.
    (2002). Substantive and operational issues of
    response bias across levels of analysis An
    example of climate-satisfactoin relationships.
    Journal of Applied Psychology, 87, 355-368.
Write a Comment
User Comments (0)
About PowerShow.com