Outputoutput correspondence - PowerPoint PPT Presentation

1 / 65
About This Presentation
Title:

Outputoutput correspondence

Description:

Output-output correspondence was introduced by McCarthy & Prince (1995) to ... I-nom remain a-nom donkey. 8/23/09. 49. Case agreement 2. Case Agreement of adverbials ... – PowerPoint PPT presentation

Number of Views:69
Avg rating:3.0/5.0
Slides: 66
Provided by: caroli6
Category:

less

Transcript and Presenter's Notes

Title: Outputoutput correspondence


1
Output-output correspondence
  • Phonology-morphology interface
  • Level-related affixation
  • Reduplication
  • Hypochoristics
  • Gradient attraction
  • Syntax-morphology interface
  • Case
  • Passive morphology
  • ECM constructions
  • Coordinate structures

2
Output-output correspondence
  • Output-output correspondence was introduced by
    McCarthy Prince (1995) to account for
    morphologically-based phonological effects.
  • Instead of taking an input as a reference, a
    morphological operation applies to a ready
    output, a form which has already been through
    phonology.
  • Faithfulness to input is ranked differently than
    correspondence between two outputs.

3
Output-output correspondence
  • Definition of Correspondence (McCarthy Prince
    1995262)
  • Given two strings S1 and S2, correspondence is
    a relation R between the elements of S1 and those
    of S2. When a R b, the elements a of S1 and b
    of S2 are called correspondents of each other.

4
  • The notion of correspondence is vague. The
    correspon-dence relation takes its substance from
    a series of constraints implementing the kind of
    relation needed in each case
  • MAX (no deletion), DEP (no epenthesis)
  • Additional constraints are
  • IDENT(F), LINEARITY (saying something about the
    ordering of the elements) CONTIGUITY (saying
    something about the adjacency of elements),
    ANCHOR-Edge (about the edges of the corresponding
    elements), HEAD-MATCH (if one of the
    correspondent has a head, its correspondent has
    the same head), etc.

5
Input-Output faithfulness and Output-Output
correspondence
  • Input
  • (full model)
  • F C
  • Output1 Output2
  • Prediction of the Correspondence Theory
  • IO-Faith gtgt Prosodic Constraints gtgt OO-Faith/Corr
  • or
  • OO-Faith/Corr gtgt Prosodic Constraints gtgt IO-Faith
  • Relations betw. Input and Output2 are assumed to
    be rare.

6
Correspondence
7
1. Reduplication
  • Reduplication is a morphological operation (often
    plural, iterative, habituative, intensifier )
    consisting in copying (reduplicating part or
    whole of a stem). According to McCarthy Prince,
    only authentic prosodic constituents
    (syllables, feet, prosodic words) can be
    reduplicants.
  • Ilokano Reduplicant Template Heavy syllable
    (McCarthy Prince 1995)
  • s
  • / \
  • m m

8
Reduplication in Ilokano
  • Reduplicant consists of a closed syllable
  • tra.ba.ho trab - tra.ba.ho work
  • Red Stem
  • Reduplicant consists of a syllable with a long
    vowel
  • róot ro - róot litter
  • Red Stem

9
Reduplication
  • Lardil Reduplicant Templates (McCarthy Prince
    1995) Binary feet
  • F F
  • / \
  • s s s
  • / \
  • m m

10
Reduplication in Lardil
  • Reduplicant consists of two syllables
  • kele-th kele kele-kele to cut
  • pareli-th pareli parel-pareli to gather
  • Reduplicant consists of a heavy syllable
  • la-th latha laa-la to guide
  • aali-th aali aal-aali to be
    thirsty

11
Why does reduplication needs OO-correspondence?
  • In some languages, the segmental make-up
    (so-called melody, a misnomer) of the reduplicant
    copies the segmental make-up of the full form,
    rather than taking its raw material from the
    input. Two cases
  • - overapplication a phonological process has
    seemed to apply, though its context of
    application is not visible at the surface
    (non-surface apparent)
  • - underapplication a phonological process does
    not apply, though its context of application is
    present at the surface (non-surface true)

12
Overapplication
  • In Javanese, there is a process of h-deletion
    taking place intervocally
  • Javanese h-deletion
  • Root Rootmy RootDem.
  • anh anh-ku an-e strange
  • arah arah-ku ara-e direction

13
Overapplication
  • In reduplication this process takes also place in
    environements other than intervocalic. The
    phonological result of h-deletion is copied from
    the base to the reduplicant.
  • Reduplication Overapplication of h-deletion
  • bedah bedah-bedah beda-beda-e broken
  • dajøh dajøh-dajøh dajø-dajø-e guest

14
Underapplication
  • In Akan, there is a process of palatalization.
    Coronals are affricated before a front vowel, and
    /h/ is realized as a palatal fricative.
  • Palatalization in Akan
  • t? k divide
  • d?e de receive
  • çi hi border

15
Underapplication
  • In reduplication, though the vowel of the
    reduplicant is always i, no palatalization
    takes place. The consonant of the base is
    faithfully copied.
  • Reduplication
  • ki-ka t?i-ka bite
  • hi-haw çi-haw trouble

16
2. Different levels of affixation
  • It has been observed that affixes appear in a
    certain order, and that they behave as classes of
    affixes w.r.t. this property.
  • In English, besides other morphological
    operations like compounding and inflection, two
    levels of derivational affixation have been
    described.
  • - Level I affixes which influence the phonology
    of the stem -ic, -ation, -al
  • - Level II affixes which do not -less, -ness,
    -y, -ing

17
  • - Level II affixes are peripheral to Level I
    affixes.
  • (but see Fabb 1988 who showed that more
    restrictions are at play than just ordering)
  • To account for this, Kiparsky, Mohanan and others
    developed a model of Lexical Phonology, in which
    morphology and phonology are interleaved
  • Some morphology applies (level I affixation),
    then phonology. Phonology consists of a set of
    ordered rules.
  • After completion of phonology, some more
    morphology applies (level II affixation), then
    the whole phonology applies again.

18
  • Level II phonology has no access to morphological
    information provided at earlier levels (and
    vice-versa) we thus have a cyclic model of the
    morphology-phonology interactions (but see
    Mohanan who allows loops in Malayalam).
  • When all levels have been completed (there may be
    more than two), the so-called post-lexical
    phonology applies, which is the sentence-level
    phonology. This phonology is automatic, applies
    in all contexts, and doesnt care about levels.
    Final Devoicing in German is an example of this
    type.

19
Why does affixation needs OO-correspondence?
  • OT has problems with the results of Lexical
    Phonology.
  • It can replace the set of ordered rules inside of
    each level, but the levels themselves are more
    difficult to account for.
  • Some examples

20
Why does affixation needs OO-correspondence?
  • Level ordering of affixes (Benua 1995) New
    York-Philadelphia dialects (æ-tensing E is
    tense)
  • Unaffixed Class 1 Affix Class 2 Affix
  • class klEs classic klæ.sik classy klE.si
  • mass mEs massive mæ.sv massable mæ.s-
  • pass pEs passive pæ.sv passing pæ.s

21
Why does affixation needs OO-correspondence?
  • A standard kind of OT cannot account for the
    different vowel in the stem of these words, due
    to the different kind of affixation.
  • The alternation between the two kinds of vowels
    is due to syllabification Benua has the
    following constraint
  • æ-tensing (æCs)
  • This constraint cannot be ranked as to deliver
    all forms properly.

22
  • Benua (1995) proposes to account for level II
    affixes with correspondence to related outputs,
    in the examples above class, pass, and so on.
  • Level I affixes take the input as input, and
    level II affixes take the output of class and
    pass as inputs.
  • The faithfulness to the output, when relevant, is
    assumed to be greater than the faithfulness to
    the input. This explains why level II affixes do
    not trigger much phonological changes in the
    stem.

23

24
  • A second example of Benua
  • condemn/ condemnation / condemning
  • -ation is a class 1 suffix and takes the input as
    base
  • -ing is a class 2 suffix and takes the output as
    base

25

26

27

28
3. Hypochoristics
  • A third kind of morphological process for which
    OO-correspondence has been assumed is
    hypochoristic formation.
  • A first example comes from the i-formation in
    German which consist of a syllabic trochee, the
    unmarked (but not the minimal foot) of German
  • Prosodic Constraint on German i-formations
  • i-formations F s's

29
  • Katharína gt Káthi Tóm gtTómmi
  • Bénjamin gt Bénni Úlrich gt Úlli
  • Klínsmann gt Klínsi Hirn gt Hirni
  • Andréas gt Ándi Gabriéle gt Gábi
  • Mánfred gt Mánni Wáltraud gt Wálli
  • Wílhelm gt Wílli Cornélia gt Cónni
  • Wést/Ostdeutscher gt Wéssi / Óssi

30
  • Many languages build hypochoristics in a similar
    way.
  • Prosodic Constraint in French
  • Hypocoristics F s or ss'
  • True hypochoristics
  • Véronique Véro
  • Dominique Domi, Dom, Do
  • Bénédicte Béné
  • Elisabeth Zabeth, Babé, Babette, Beth

31
  • French also has
  • Reduplications (Echo-words) s s'
  • /\
  • (C)V
  • père gt pépère, ours gt nounours, main gt
    main-main
  • The input to these reduplications is a
    monosyllabic word.
  • But the syllabification is not part of the input
    it is an added structure pointing to the fact
    that these reduplications are faithful to an
    output rather than to an input.

32
  • IO-Faithfulness gtgt Prosodic Constraints gtgt
    BT-Faithfulness
  • The emergence of the unmarked (TETU) is a
    landmark of this pattern. The prosodic
    constraints in the middle are responsible for the
    unmarked pattern of the language bisyllabic
    foot, trochaic pattern, open syllables

33
  • If the relation between input and output is
    active, the unmarked form has no chance to
    emerge, since all kinds of inputs are there, and
    faithfulness is high.
  • But the forms entering the relation OO have a
    chance to emerge as unmarked, since the prosodic
    constraints are higher.
  • Trochaic feet (iambic in the case of French, open
    syllables and the like) emerge.

34
  • Conclusion and open problems
  • 1. Since correspondence is a vague notion, all
    kinds of forms should be able to enter into a
    correspondence relation. How can we delimit the
    desirable correspondence relations from the
    undesirable ones?
  • 2. OO-constraints lead to an explosion of the
    constraints.
  • 3. OO-correspondence needs an existing output in
    order to be workable. In some cases, surface
    forms seem to be faithful to a form which is
    never realized as an output. In those cases, we
    have opacity.

35
  • Conclusion and open problems
  • 4. Lexical Phonology, as well as all models using
    ordered rules have no problems with opacity. The
    existence of intermediate forms, neither inputs
    nor outputs, is a natural consequence of rule
    ordering.
  • 5. OT has big problems with those. Since no
    derivation enters phonology, no intermediate step
    should ever be needed.
  • 6. We will see later on that alternative
    solutions have been offered to the opacity
    problem.

36
Gradient attraction
  • If output output correspondence is needed anyway,
    why not treat all kinds of morphological
    relationships as output-output correpondences?
  • This is the step taken by Burzio (to appear) in
    his Gradient Attraction theory.
  • Burzio claims that similar (output)
    representations attract each other and that they
    do so gradiently. The more similar they are, the
    greater the attraction.

37
  • Modified OT (Burzio, to appear)
  • other representations
  • Input gt Grammar gt Output
  • The other representations are forms which are
    related in terms of morphemic parenthood or of
    analogy.

38
  • Gradient attraction
  • Allomorphs consist to a large extent of the same
    segmental material and have (partly) the same
    semantic representation.
  • But they also contrast with each other in order
    to keep their distinctness (Flemmings dispersion
    theory)
  • Gradient attraction is different from
    output-output correspondence. One of the reasons
    os that allophonic variations of complex words
    can be triggered not only by the stem but also by
    the affix(es).

39
  • Examples
  • Stress position 1 titánic is attracted not only
    by títan but also by barbáric and dynámic
  • Stress position 2 módernist is influenced by
    módern and not by the one of modérnity, because
    -ist adjoins only to adjectival bases.

40
  • Segmental alternation allophony of french gros,
    grosse and gros fat with liaison.
  • According to Burzio, the third form is attracted
    by both other forms, takes its vowel quality from
    the feminine form and its consonant from the
    masculine (both facts are unfortunately wrong!
    The vowel quality is always the same, and the
    liaison consonant is voiced.)
  • Steriade cites a much better example also from
    French an adjective like ancien old has three
    allomorphs ãsj, ãsjn and ãsjn. The
    liaison case takes ist vowel quality from the
    masculine and ist vowel from the feminine.

41
OO-Correspondence in Syntax?
  • With syntax, there seems to be little compelling
    evidence for the need for output-output
    correspondence.
  • Possible evidence for OO-correspondence in the
    syntax comes from at least two domains
  • the syntax-morphology interaction
  • coordinate structures

42
Syntax-Morphology Interaction
  • Alternations that affect grammatical functions
    tend to minimize differences among the various
    construction types
  • In a representational model, this suggests an
    influence of OO-correspondence.
  • One case in point is the rule for Case marking in
    the German passive

43
Case rules for the active clause
  • Nom NPs bear nominative case
  • Acc NPs that are not the highest argument
    bear accusative case
  • Dat NPs that are neither the highest nor the
    lowest argument bear dative case
  • Uniqueness, etc.

44
Case rules for the active clause
  • Er kommt nom
  • he comes
  • er sieht ihn nom acc
  • he sees him
  • er gibt ihr es nom dat acc
  • he gives it to her

45
Case rules in the passive
  • What we find
  • Es wird ihr gegeben
  • it-nom is her-dat given
  • What we should get
  • sie wird es gegeben
  • she-nom is it given

46
Case rules in the passive
  • A possible account
  • Maximize faithfulness between the active and the
    corresponding passive!
  • (00-correspondence)
  • The Alternative Rule Ordering
  • 1. Case potential is determined
  • e.g. by a lexical rule
  • 2. Absorption of the accusative
  • e.g. late in the syntax

47
Case rules in other constructions
  • Similar ideas can be applied to
  • Complex predicates (retaining the Case of the
    preposition)
  • jemanden anwinken
  • someone.acc at-wave
  • jemandem zuwinken
  • someone.dat to-wave
  • but ... is this OO?

48
Case agreement 1
  • ECM-constructions and Case Agreement interact in
    a fashion that may also be understood in terms of
    OO-correspondence
  • Case Agreement of some predicate nominals
  • Ich bin ein Esel
  • I-nom am a-nom donkey
  • ich bleibe ein Esel
  • I-nom remain a-nom donkey

49
Case agreement 2
  • Case Agreement of adverbials
  • er grüsst die Männer einen nach dem anderen
  • he greets the-acc men one-acc after the other
  • die Männer grüssen ihn einer nach dem anderen
  • the men greet him one-nom after the other

50
Case agreement 3
  • Predicates and some adverbs may take over the
    Case of the noun phrase they are linked to in
    terms of semantics ...
  • For ECM-constructions, we expect Case agreeing
    expressions to always take over the Case of the
    NP they are linked to.

51
Case agreement in ECM-contexts
  • But there seem to be two dialects
  • 1. Ich lasse ihn einen Helden sein
  • I let him-acc an-acc hero be
  • 2. Ich lasse ihn ein Held sein
  • I let him-acc an-nom hero-nom be
  • 1. Agreement maintained
  • 2. Nominative maintained

52
Case agreement in ECM-contexts
  • 1. Ich lasse die Männer einen nach dem anderen
    ankommen
  • I have the men one-acc after the other arrive
  • 2. Ich lasse die Männer einer nach dem anderen
    ankommen
  • I have the men one-nom after the other arrive

53
Case agreement in ECM-contexts
  • Solution 1
  • OO-Correspondence between the finite clause and
    the infinitive
  • Solution 2
  • Case determination before nom gt acc change in the
    subject position of the infinitive

54
Summary
  • The Case effects described so far may either be
    interpreted as being due to
  • OO-correspondence
  • lexical determination of Case, followed by a
    syntax-triggered change
  • more complex Case rules

55
Parallelism in coordinate structures
  • It may thus make more sense to look at a
    construction type that bears some vague
    resemblance to reduplication --- conjunctions.
  • In principle, the two parts of a coordination
    construction are fairly independent of each other
    ...
  • .... this changes when they are affected by a
    reduction operation.

56
Parallelism in coordinate structures
  • Scope is a very interesting example for this.
  • Independent
  • I introduced one of the boys to every teacher
  • is scope-ambiguous
  • ONE gt EVERY
  • EVERY gt ONE

57
Parallelism in coordinate structures
  • I introduced one of the boys to every teacher,
    and Bill did, too
  • involving a reducing coordination, is
  • two-ways ambiguous, NOT four ways, as one might
    expect!

58
Parallelism in coordinate structures
  • More examples
  • an American runner seems to have won a gold
    medal, and a Russian athlete does, too
  • the two indefinite NPs agree w.r.t. specificity
  • one guard was seen in front of every building,
    and a policeman was, too.

59
Parallelism in coordinate structures
  • In an ellipsis/coordination reduction
    construction, the scope relations among the
    elements in clause A must be identical to the
    ones in clause B.
  • In the Y-model of grammar, in which
  • phonology and semantics do not communicate, this
    is difficult to account for.

60
Parallelism in coordinate structures
  • Across-the-board rule application was invented in
    order to account for such facts.
  • Who did you meet t and invite t
  • The parallelism facts fit neatly into
    OO-correspondence, however.

61
A special form of OO-correspondence
  • Perhaps, quite a different concept of
    OO-correspondence is called for in syntax ...
  • Many syntactic approaches assume more than one
    level of representation ...
  • Surface structure
  • Logical Form
  • Argument Structure

62
A special form of OO-correspondence
  • It has been observed that UG tries to minimize
    differences between these levels.
  • This economy of derivation may reflect
    OO-correspondence between different levels.

63
A special form of OO-correspondence
  • From a single input, two, three or more of such
    representations are generated.
  • Minimal Link (superiority) effects may reflect
    the attempt to minimize structural differences
    between lor.s (Müller, Williams)

64
MLC 1
  • Who do you expect to say what
  • what do you expect who to say
  • More relations of the pre-movement/declarative
    structure are preserved in the former example
  • koj kogo mili who what saw

65
MLC 2
  • In the clitic (Wackernagel) position
  • weil er es ihr gibt
  • because he it her gives
  • pronoun order has been claimed to be identical to
    base order ...
Write a Comment
User Comments (0)
About PowerShow.com