Title: The E-Collaboration Paradox: Evidence from Two Empirical Studies
1The E-Collaboration ParadoxEvidence from Two
Empirical Studies
2The e-collaboration paradox
- Media naturalness proposition
- The less face-to-face-like a medium is, the more
obstacles exist for effective communication. - Compensatory adaptation proposition
- Often individuals avoid using unnatural media,
but, if they decide to use them, those
individuals compensate for the communication
obstacles posed by the media.
3Study 1
- An Experimental Study of 20 Process Redesign
Dyads - Analysis Method Used Comparison of Means (ANOVA
Mann-Whitney U Tests)
4Participants
- The research study involved subjects with
substantial hands-on experience in process
redesign in the defense sector, recruited from
management and engineering ranks of a large
defense contractor. - Their ages ranged from 23 to 60, with a mean age
of 35. Fifty-nine percent of the subjects were
males.
5Treatment conditions
- The subjects were randomly assigned to dyads and
to communication media conditions. - Each dyad completed two similar process
redesign-related tasks using different
communication media for each task. - Half of the dyads (i.e., 10 dyads) completed one
of the tasks face-to-face while the other half
completed the same task electronically. - After this, all dyads moved on to the next task,
using different media than they had used in the
previous task that is, the dyads previously
interacting face-to-face now interacted
electronically and vice-versa.
6Electronic communication medium
- A set of Web-based threaded online discussion
boards created the electronic communication media
employed in the experiment. - The online discussion boards were developed using
Microsoft Frontpage 2000 and Active Server Pages. - They were used in a quasi-synchronous manner
i.e., they were refreshed at short time
intervals, creating an online chat-like
environment where the discussions were threaded. - One Web-based online discussion board was created
for each dyad. All online discussion boards were
identical.
7Variables and measures
- Cognitive effort
- Measured based on NASAs task load index (a.k.a.
NASA-TLX) developed by Hart and colleagues (Hart
and Staveland, 1988). - Communication ambiguity
- Measured based on an instrument previously
developed and validated by Kock (2001), answered
on a 1 to 7 Likert-type scale. - Message preparation
- Measured based on an instrument previously
developed and validated by Kock (2001), answered
on a 1 to 7 Likert-type scale.
8Variables and measures
9Variables and measures
- Fluency
- Measured by counting the number of words
exchanged by the members of the dyads and
dividing it by the number of minutes each dyad
took to complete the task (Kock, 1998). - Task outcome quality
- Measured by comparing the process sketches
generated by the dyad members with correct
models (Kock and Murphy, 2001). Two different
coders generated similarity scores used to
assess task outcome quality independently.
10Results
11Results
12Results
- The results suggest that the use of an
e-collaboration tool, when compared with the
face-to-face medium, increased perceived
cognitive effort by about 41, perceived
communication ambiguity by about 80, and
perceived message preparation by about 47, while
at the same time reducing fluency by
approximately 77. - The study also suggests that the use of the
e-collaboration tool had no significant impact on
the quality of the outcomes generated by the
dyads.
13Conclusion
- The study supports the e-collaboration paradox
notion - Media naturalness proposition
- The less face-to-face-like a medium is, the more
obstacles exist for effective communication
obsv. cognitive effort, communication ambiguity. - Compensatory adaptation proposition
- Often individuals avoid using unnatural media,
but, if they decide to use them, those
individuals compensate for the communication
obstacles posed by the media obsv. message
preparation, fluency, task outcomes.
14Study 2
- An Survey Study of 290 New Product Development
Teams - Analysis Method Used Structural Equation
Modeling (PLS)
15Participants
- Contact persons in a variety of technology-based
companies in the Northeastern USA were selected
to participate in the study. - To be included in this study, each company must
have developed a product that had been launched
into the marketplace and commercialized for at
least six months. - Data from 290 new product development projects in
66 companies were obtained.
16Research instrument
- A questionnaire developed based on previous
research on NPD teams (Kessler and Chakrabarti,
1999 Lynn et al 2000) was used. - All constructs in the study were measured using
multiple-item scales, which in turn were
Likert-type scales (0 Strongly Disagree to 10
Strongly Agree).
17Constructs and measures
18Constructs and measures
19Structural model
H1 (neutral)
H3 (positive)
H4 (positive)
H6 (positive)
H5 (positive)
H2 (neutral)
20Results
H1 (ß-.009)
R2.226
H3 (ß.351)
H4 (ß.460)
R2.123
H6 (ß.488)
H5 (ß.090)
Notes T values were calculated through the
bootstrapping method Significant at p lt .001
in a one-tailed test
H2 (ß.102)
R2.315
21Conclusion
- The study supports the e-collaboration paradox
notion - Media naturalness proposition
- The use of electronic communication (as opposed
to the face-to-face medium) induces procedural
structuring, which suggests an attempt (through
procedural structuring) to compensate for media
obstacles. - Compensatory adaptation proposition
- Procedural structuring has a stronger effect on
task outcome variables than electronic
communication use, as individuals compensate (or
perhaps overcompensate) for the communication
obstacles posed by the electronic communication
media.
22Implications
- Development of e-collaboration tools in
situations where compensatory adaptation is - Desirable and unhindered
- Desirable but hindered (e.g., cognitive fatigue
situations) - Undesirable (e.g., entertainment-related B2C
situations)
23Key references
Final slide
- Kock, N. (2004), The Psychobiological Model
Toward a New Theory of Computer-mediated
Communication Based on Darwinian Evolution,
Organization Science, V.15, No.3, pp. 327-348. - Kock, N. (2001), Compensatory Adaptation to a
Lean Medium An Action Research Investigation of
Electronic Communication in Process Improvement
Groups, IEEE Transactions on Professional
Communication, V.44, No.4, pp. 267-285. - Kock, N. (1998), Can Communication Medium
Limitations Foster Better Group Outcomes? An
Action Research Study, Information Management,
V.34, No.5, pp. 295-305.
Available from http//www.tamiu.edu/nedkock/
24Early development of symbolic communication
artifacts, e.g., cave paintings, about 30,000
years ago.
Emergence of modern humans, i.e., Homo sapiens
sapiens, about 100,000 years ago.
FtF communication through complex speech.
Emergence of early Homo sapiens grades, e.g.,
Homo sapiens neanderthalensis.
FtF communication through rudimentary speech.
Emergence of genus Homo, e.g., Homo habilis and
erectus.
FtF communication through facial expressions and
simple sounds.
Emergence of the genus Australopithecine, e.g.,
Australopithecus afarensis and africanus.
25E-communication behavior theories
26E-communication behavior theories (contd. 1)
27E-communication behavior theories (contd. 2)
28E-communication behavior theories (contd. 3)