Title: Lecture Nine Early and Late Childhood Stuttering
1Lecture Nine - Early and Late Childhood Stuttering
- Orienting Questions How does stuttering change
over childhood? How does stuttering affect the
wider spectrum of behaviour?
2Lecture Outline
- How stuttering helps to understand why fluent
speech breaks down - Describe research with children who stutter
- Sensory and motor factors
- Social factors
- How research can inform speech therapy
- Use of external devices to improve fluency
- Using social psychology methodology
3Prevalence and incidence
- Stuttering will affect approximately 5 of the
population at some time. - Disorder affects children disproportionately -
the age of onset is usually between the ages of 3
and 7 years. - Boys more affected than girls. Ratio 31 at onset
- Social factors may be important in this area.
4Two recent studies that (together) deal with
stuttering from onset to teenage
- The Illinois study (Yairi, Ambrose and
co-workers) children from around onset of
stuttering (two years or so of age) to about
eight years. - The UCL study from age eight to teenage
5The Illinois study
- Diagnostic factors Three groups
- Primary
- Secondary
- Other
6Primary Factors
- Family history
- Persistent stutterers come from families who have
members who persisted - Transmission model - single-major locus
explanation (Ambrose et al, 1993). - Contrasts with Kidd et al. - sex-modified
polygenic mode - Gender
- Boys at higher risk (67 boys)
7Primary Factors
- Stuttering like disfluencies (SLD) Stutter-like
disfluencies because typical, but not exclusive
to people who stutter - SLD
- part-word repetitions
- single-syllable word repetitions
- disrhythmic phonation
- Other
- interjections
- multiple syllable word and phrase repetitions
- revisions or abandoned utterances
- Dissension about SLD
- Wingate (2001) SLD are not stuttering events
- our own two-class scheme puts some SLD and OD in
each class - advocate present separate counts so other people
can do their way
8Primary Factors
- Duration of stuttering history
- The longer stuttering goes on, the higher is the
risk it will persist (esp. girls) - Disfluency length Extent and duration
- More extensive stuttering events (bu, bu, but)
are sign of higher risk - Sound prolongations and blocks
- Apart from early months, prolongation is a factor
that differentiates children who will eventually
persist from those who recover. - Also as shrink, sign recovery in progress.
9Secondary Factors
- Stuttering severity
- Head and neck movements
- Phonological skills
- Expressive language skills
10Other Factors
- Concomitant disorders
- e.g. ADHD
- Awareness and affective reactions
- evidence that some young children are aware and
that this upsets them
11The UCL study from age eight to teenage
- Illinois work - important information but
limitations - Recovery not complete at age 8 (is by teenage)
- No control group, we have limited data on control
subjects - Our work shows important changes in disfluency
and social characteristics happen after age eight
that relate to diagnosis, assessment and possibly
treatment.
12The UCL study from age eight to teenage
- Follow-up study of children who stutter between
the ages 8 and 18 years. - First seen when attending clinic for initial
assessment at around 3 - 8 years - Cohort constant in terms of treatment received
- Recorded at age 8 or younger so verified that
stuttering - Followed up to age at which recovery complete
13The UCL study from age eight to teenage
- Total number of children who stutter with English
as a first language involved in the study - 372. - Not all children have performed the full range of
tests. Some of reasons - still participating (not yet reached age
appropriate for some of the tests). - Geographical
- Lost contact
14The UCL study from age eight to teenage Core
Group
- Important subgroup of the children we have seen
over the years - Consists of 24 children all of whom have been
classified as persistent (13) and recovered (11)
using the following criteria - Three recordings - 8-10 years, 1012 years and 12
years - SSI
- Parental and child rating using adaptation of
Kully and Boberg questionnaire - Researchers rating
15The UCL study from age eight to teenage
Secondary Group
- Consists of 84 children who stutter for whom we
have a minimum of one audio recording, together
with history of therapy, initial contact details,
date of birth, gender and at least one of the
research projects. - Sixty-six (79) of secondary group have been
classified as recovered/persistent - Children in this group can be transferred to the
Core Group when at appropriate age / assessment
completed etc
16We have the following information/data on the
Secondary Group
- Motor (tapping) 23/84 (27)
- Motor (lip tracking) 14/84 (17)
- Temperament 29/84 (35)
- Handedness 67/84 (80)
- Grammar 24/84 (29)
- Vocabulary 17/84 (19)
- Lexical retrieval 20/84 (24)
- Priming 5/84 (6)
- Intelligence 10/84 12)
- Academic 37/84 (44)
- Five children (6) are no longer participating
- Therapy history 84/84 (100)
- Demographic 40/84 (48)
- Family History 39/84 (46)
- Anxiety 27/84 (32)
- Health 44/84 (52)
- Scans 9/84 (11)
- Age of Onset 55/84 (65)
- Hearing Threshold 40/84 (48)
- Hearing problems 40/84 (48)
- Masking threshold 40/84 (48)
17Other participants
- There are a further 164 children for whom we have
a minimum of one audio recording, but incomplete
data with regard to therapy, contact details etc.
- Studies of recovered/persistent use subjects from
core and secondary groups - Studies that havent looked at recovered/persisten
t per se (e.g. some of the hearing tests and
social studies) used selection of all the
speakers
18Sensory and Motor Factors
Do children who persist have a hearing / timing
problem?
Reason that usually motivates investigation of
hearing is because altering hearing improves
speech control (more about this later)
19Survey data (n 61)
- Parents reported a high incidence of hearing
problems in the General Health Questionnaire
conducted as part of demographic assessment. - Further investigation established a high
incidence of otitis media with effusion (OME) - 21.3 overall (13/61)
- with 16.7 persistent (5/30), and 25.8 recovered
(8/31) - There was also a high likelihood of OME being
treated by ventilation tubes - 38.5 overall (5/13)
- 20 persistent (1/5)
- 50 recovered (4/8).
- Possible that stuttering recovered because
hearing problem resolved (NB would be peripheral)
20Data from Hearing Studies
- Used masking technique (probe sound masked by
white noise) - results indicate that auditory sensitivity for
sounds in noise continues to develop through to
teenage - a different pattern of auditory development
exists for the participants who stutter compared
with participants who do not stutter. - Backward masking performance at teenage
distinguishes speakers who persist in their
stutter from those who recover.
21Hearing - conclusions
Data that have been collected on a sample (n
61, 30 persistent, 31 recovered) of these
participants show that for recovered speakers at
teenage, there are no hearing problem either at
central or peripheral levels. In speakers who
persist in their stutter, there is a hearing
problem that affects central hearing at teenage
that is specific to speakers who persist in
their stutter, which could be a residual problem
hanging over from OME in early childhood.
22Data from Timing Studies
- Used various tests from the Dow Moruzzi battery
designed to assess performance of the cerebellum - As with hearing, cerebellar performance continued
to develop through to teenage - a different pattern of cerebellar development
exists for the participants who stutter compared
with participants who do not stutter. - No studies yet to test fro differences between
persistent and recovered people who stutter
23(No Transcript)
24Conclusions
- Evidence to implicate auditory and speech timing
factors in the onset and development of the
disorder. - Moderating timing mechanisms using altered
auditory feedback improves fluency.
25The role of biological, environmental and social
factors in the breakdown of fluent speech
26Speech is a social phenomenon. It is therefore
important to know what role social factors, and
their changing role over development, play in
stuttering. Stuttering has been shown to be
topic- (Mallard Westbrook, 1986) and
situation-specific (Yaruss, 1997). The disorder
is governed in part by affective factors that
are socially moderated.
27Pre-school and early years
- Intelligence
- Attitudes toward speaking
- Temperament
- Family Interaction
28Intelligence - EMPIRICAL
- CWS score significantly lower on intelligence
tests than do fluent controls. Evident in both
verbal and non-verbal intelligence tests. - the intelligence and social class of CWS who are
receiving treatment is found to be above average
29Intelligence - PERCEPTION
- Fluent children report CWS as less intelligent
than controls - Adults perceive adults who stutter to be have
average or above average intelligence.
30Attitude - EMPIRICAL
- Attitudes have been shown (in particular
circumstances) to be causally-related to
behaviour - Stuttering usually starts between three and five
years. - Attitudes about many things can change between
ages 3 and 5
31Communication Attitudes and Stuttering
- Why do we need to measure the communication
attitudes of young children who stutter? - How can the communication attitudes of young
children who stutter be assessed - How can information on attitudes be used to
improve prediction and treatment?
32Communication Attitudes and Stuttering
- Stuttering usually starts between three and five
years (Dalton Hardcastle, 1977) - An instrument that offers an indication of
attitude in young children would be useful for
early diagnosis and intervention (Yairi
Ambrose, 1992 Onslow, 1994). - Increase of intervention at a young age
(Lidcombe) etc. Need to be able to assess
attitude change to measure efficacy of treatment.
33Communication Attitudes and Stuttering - chicken
or egg?
- Conflicting views as to whether the negative
communication attitudes of cws reported in the
literature are cause or effect - the onset of stuttering is a result of the belief
that speech is difficult (Bloodstein, 1987
Brutten Dunham, 1989) - the negative beliefs that people who stutter have
about speech is a product of, rather than a cause
of their dysfluency (Guitar, 1976 Peters
Guitar, 1991).
34Computer assessment
- Animation and sound
- Standardised presentation of stimuli
- Response collection and statistical analysis are
automated
35Participants
- 143 monolingual English children
- 68 boys 35 girls, mean age 5.2 years (sd .59)
- 22 children with English as an alternative
language - 8 boys 14 girls, mean age 5.32 years (sd .58)
- 16 monolingual English children who stutter
- 12 boys 4 girls, mean age 5.83 years (sd .82)
36A one way ANOVA indicated a significant effect of
language group Post hoc tests indicated that
there was a significant difference between the
mean scores of the monolingual English children
and children who stutter (p.001) and between
EAL children and children who stutter (p.023).
37Exploratory factor analysis
- Factor 1
- classroom-based situations
- Factor 2
- play and home-based situations
- Factor 3
- talking with known older children and adults
(relatives and friends) - Factor 4
- talking with other children and adults
38- Significant effect of language group across the
four factors - Post hoc tests show persistent group to be
significantly more anxious than - their monolingual peers on 3 out of the 4
factors.
39Conclusions - young children and communication
attitudes
- Identification of stuttering
- Negative attitudes apparent close to onset
- Are those children with more negative attitudes
toward communication more at risk for persistent
stuttering? - COMCAS allows measurement of specific areas of
communication attitudes.
40Temperamental characteristics and fluency
breakdown
- Research evidence that temperament contributes to
cognitive development (Miceli et al, 1998 Singer
Fagan, 1992) - Also contributes to language acquisition (Dixon
Shore, 1997 Dixon Smith, 2000) - Relationship with speech and language disorders
- delayed language development (Caulfield et al,
1989 Paul et al, 1990,1997) - stuttering (Anderson et al, 2003 Embrechts et al
2000 Lewis Goldberg, 1997
41Temperament - EMPIRICAL
- Temperament is distinguished from personality
traits by being much more physiologically based. - Temperament is a contributing factor to both the
development and persistence of stuttering. - CWS exhibit a more vulnerable or sensitive
temperament, which could possibly be a
contributing factor in the development,
maintenance or chances of recovery of stuttering
42Temperament and Stuttering
- 10 CWS versus 10 controls
- CWS
- 7 boys, 3 girls age range 3 years 7 months - 7
years 2 months, mean 5years 10 months (sd 1.36
years) - Controls
- 7 boys, 3 girls age range 3 years 8 months - 7
years 2 months, mean 5 years 4 months (sd 1.16
years)
43Temperament and Stuttering
- The Behavioral Style Questionnaire (BSQ)
- Parental report
- Measures nine components of temperament
Activity, Predictability, Approach, Adaptability,
Intensity, Mood, Persistence, Distractibility,
Threshold
44Conclusions
- We found temperament differences others have not
- Anderson found a temperament difference that
Embrechts did not - One variable that went in different directions
between Anderson and Embrechts - One consistent finding CWS not adaptable
45Family interaction - does parental speech rate
and/or linguistic style affect fluency?
- Speech Rate
- when parents reduce speech rate their 3 yo
children also reduce speech rate (Guitar
Marchinkowski, 2001) - no difference in the speech rate of parents of
children stutter compared to controls (Kloth et
al, 1995, 1998)
46Family interaction - does parental speech rate
and/or linguistic style affect fluency?
- Linguistic style
- parents of pre-school children at stuttering
onset use less complex language than controls
(Kloth et al, 1995) - children will begin to stutter after
intervention to enhance expressive language
(Hall, 1997 Meyers et al, 1990) - relationship between conversation turn-taking
style and breakdown of fluency (Winslow Guitar,
1994)
47Adolescence
- Bullying
- Self-esteem
- Personality
- Anxiety
48Bullying - EMPIRICAL
- Two main questions
- 1. Are CWS more likely to be bullied than
non-stuttering peers? - 2. Does bullying exacerbate the problem of
stuttering? - There only appears to be evidence for the former.
49Factors that increase the risk of a child being
bullied
- few friends
- low-status friends
- rejection by peers
- these factors often apply to children with
special educational needs.
50Why are CWS likely to be bullied?
- often reluctant or unable to participate verbally
in school activities (or social groups in
general). - this may lead to them being seen as shy or
withdrawn - would lead to the perception that they have few
friends
51Bullying - EMPIRICALRatings by fluent adults of
children who stutter.
- CWS were not readily accepted as members of their
classroom - stuttering students who were more able to adjust
to interpersonal situations responded better to
therapy
52Bullying - EMPIRICALRetrospective self-ratings
by adults who stutter.
- 11 of adults who stutter report being bullied at
school (compared to 30 in general population) - More recently 74 of 276 adults who stutter
report being bullied at school - 6 of these stated that the bullying had a
long-term negative effect on their stuttering. - Problems with long-term retrospective ratings
53Social Exclusion and Bullying
- Single classroom group in each of 16 schools
across England - One child who stutters in each class
- Sociometric interview procedure
- Allocation to social groups
- Nomination to behavioural categories
54Social Exclusion and Bullying
- Social Groups
- CWS twice as likely to be rejected than controls
(43.518.86) - CWS four times less likely to be popular than
controls (6.2525.84) - Using chi-square analysis both differences are
significant - Trend for CWS to adopt the predominant social
profile of the group
55Social Exclusion and Bullying
- Behavioural Categories
- Control children twice as likely to be nominated
as leaders than CWS (12.92 compared to 6.25) - More CWS nominated as bully victim (37.25) and
seeks help (25) than controls (10.6 and 13.18) - Using chi-square analysis significant difference
in nominations to bully victim category
56Social Exclusion and Bullying
- Persistent/Recovered
- No significant differences between groups in
either social or behavioural - No significant relationship between stuttering
severity and social preference scores
57Social Exclusion and Bullying
- Conclusions
- CWS more likely to be bullied and rejected than
their peers and are less likely to be popular - CWS tend to adopt the predominate social profile
of the group. - No relationship between severity and social status
58Bullying - EMPIRICAL
- Problems with sociometric interview
- Triangulation of methodologies
- Other methods of assessing social profile
- diaries
- phone logging
59Self-esteem - BACKGROUND
- Self-perception can be affected by speech
disorders and can result in low self-esteem - Many therapeutic techniques often include goals
to improve an individual's concept of self-worth - Actual role of self-concept is in stuttering is
disputed - once individuals have developed a concept of self
as a PWS spontaneous recovery becomes less
likely. - PWS respond more positively to therapy if their
speech disorder is integrated as part of their
self-concept.
60Self-esteem - EMPIRICALSelf -ratings
- Children
- using a variety of rating instruments, most
studies report no difference between CWS and
normative data. - Adults
- relationship between low self-esteem and high
rates of stuttering - a trend toward low self-esteem
- feelings most commonly expressed by PWS are
anxiety, helplessness, victimization, and low
self-esteem.
61Personality - EMPIRICAL
- Investigations using a number of different
assessments show no differences between
school-age CWS and controls (CWNS) in personality
factors related to neuroticism or anxiety. - Personality alone does not appear to be a
predictor of the developmental pathway of
stuttering (persistency or recovery). Personality
traits are useful in combination with other
factors
62Personality - PERCEPTION
- CWS are generally stereotyped as more nervous,
shy, withdrawn, tense and anxious - Unlike intelligence these negative perceptions
are continued into adulthood. - Held by a wide range of groups, from members of
the general public, including college professors
and teachers and even clinicians
63How people who stutter are perceived
- By parents of children who stutter
- A high proportion of parents described their
children variously as - nervous, highly strung,
anxious, fearful, clingy or tense. (Starkweather,
1997) - By Clinicians
- 75 described children who stutter as nervous
and fearful and 64 as shy and insecure (Wood
Williams, 1971)
64How people who stutter are perceived
- By academics and students
- increased social distance
- rated as lower academic ability
- highly anxious (McKinnon, Hess Landry, 1986)
- Rated more negatively on 20 personality traits
- Academics rated people who stutter more
negatively than did the students - (Dorsey Guenther, 2000)
65Does research support the concept that people who
stutter are more anxious than non-stutterers
- Yes
- self report (Gabel et al 2002)
- saliva cortisol levels (Blood et al 1994)
- No
- self report (Peters Hulstijn, 1984, Blood et
al, 1994) - state/trait anxiety inventories (Miller Watson,
1992) - Skin conduction response (Dietrich and
Rohman,2001)
66Anxiety
- We examined for differences between the recovered
and persistent groups (and between each of these
groups and fluent controls) in trait and state
anxiety at teenage. - Trait anxiety is a personality characteristic
(how anxious a person is). - State anxiety is a transitory state that varies
and fluctuates over time and situation.
67- Three groups persistent (n18), recovered
(n17), control (n19) - 12 19 years
- Groups age and gender matched
- Recovery / persistence established as previously
indicated - STAIC (Speilberger et al, 1970)
- Looked at feelings in 4 different speaking
situations (states) plus trait anxiety
68- Significant effect of language group across the
four state conditions - Post hoc tests show persistent group to be
significantly more anxious than - controls in the shop, phone and class conditions
- No significant differences between groups in the
trait condition.
69Conclusions
- No significant differences between groups in
underlying trait anxiety - Persistent stutterers more anxious in
communication situations than recovered and
control participants - Is this an effect of continued stuttering?
- Or have children recovered from stuttering
because anxiety has been reduced (therapy)? - Latter is unlikely as therapy that participants
experienced (MPC, City) does not focus on anxiety
reduction - It would seem that anxiety is not a causal factor
in stuttering but measurements of anxiety could
be useful way of tracking recovery
70Adults
71Occupations - PWS self-report
- 70 of the participants reported that they
believed they could have had a better job if they
did not stutter - 56 reported deciding on their career because it
required less speaking - 35 of the participants responded that they
believed that their career progression had been
affected because they stuttered. - 32 felt discriminated against in the employment
process
72Occupations - Employers perceptions
- PWS viewed more positively following successful
speech therapy - positive change in career
- more success at interviews
- promotion and upgrades
73and finally (1)
- CWS receiving treatment score above norms on
intelligence tests - this may be an effect of
social status. - People who stutter are generally perceived as
more anxious, nervous, shy and insecure. Also
rated as less academically able. - Little evidence that parental speaking styles
affect childrens fluency. - Retrospective reports indicate that bullying
and/or rejection at school can affect long term
fluency.
74and finally (2)
- Sociometric studies show a relationship between
language groups and bullying and/or rejection. - No evidence that social isolation causes fluency
breakdown or leads to persistent stuttering. - The self-esteem of PWS becomes more negative as
they become older
75and finally (3)
- Negative communication attitudes are present in
children who stutter close to onset of the
disorder. - Children who stutter present with a different
temperamental profile compared to children who do
not stutter. - PWS are widely perceived to be anxious and
nervous although empirical evidence is
contradictory. - Employment opportunities for PWS are restricted
but improve if fluency improves
76- Social and emotional factors are critical in the
onset and maintenance of stuttering - Most models explaining the onset and development
of the disorder include a social component - There are methods and issues in social psychology
that can be used to investigate this area
77Furnham, A., Davis, S. (2004). Involvement of
social factors in stuttering A review and
assessment of current methodology. Stammering
Research, 1, 112-122. Howell, P., Davis, S.
Williams, R. (submitted). Late childhood
stuttering. Journal of Speech, Language and
Hearing Research. Wingate, M. (1988). The
structure of stuttering A psycholinguistic
study. New York Springer-Verlag. Wingate, M. E.
(2002). Foundations of stuttering. San Diego
Academic Press. Yairi, E., Ambrose, N. G.
(2005). Early childhood stuttering. Austin TX
Pro Ed. Speech Group website http//www.speech.p
sychol.ucl.ac.uk/