PSYC54 Lecture on Jan. 18, 2005: Part I - PowerPoint PPT Presentation

1 / 37
About This Presentation
Title:

PSYC54 Lecture on Jan. 18, 2005: Part I

Description:

In the table, click Chapter X Notes,' X meaning the number of the chapter. ... (trials 1 and 2): fork, hammer, key, person, scissors, swan, toothbrush, umbrella ... – PowerPoint PPT presentation

Number of Views:25
Avg rating:3.0/5.0
Slides: 38
Provided by: CCen1
Category:
Tags: jan | lecture | part | psyc54

less

Transcript and Presenter's Notes

Title: PSYC54 Lecture on Jan. 18, 2005: Part I


1
PSYC54 Lecture on Jan. 18, 2005 Part I
Please note Other PSYC54 Lecture notes,
including those for Jan. 25 (Chapter III), can be
found on http//www.utsc.utoronto.ca/7Epsyc54/PS
YC54/TOC.html In the table, click Chapter X
Notes, X meaning the number of the chapter. The
chapter contents are on https//tspace.library.uto
ronto.ca/handle/1807/1021
  • Color and Motion in
  • Shape-from-shadow Pictures

Humera Iqbal, John Kennedy, Juan Bai, Tom Nolis
2
Tom Noliss Colored Shape-from-shadow Study
  • Apparent luminance may not be the same as
    physical luminance, somehow affecting our
    performance in shape-from-shadow perception.
  • This may be especially true if the shadowed
    region is in one color (e.g., green), and the
    line at the shadow border is in another (e.g.,
    red).
  • As another example, a blue line may be perceived
    as darker than a yellow shadow even though they
    have the same luminance scores in the software
    creating the image, and shape-from-shadow
    perception may be diminished.

3
Tactile PicturesJuan Bai John Kennedy
PSYC54 Lecture on Jan. 18, 2005 Part II
Rationale Surfaces and edges are tangible as
well as visible, hence outline drawings should be
possible for the blindfolded sighted people as
well as the blind, including the congenitally
blind. (Tactile pictures, or haptic pictures,
are also called raised-line drawings.)
4
DAngiulli, Kennedy, Heller (1998)
  • Participants
  • blind children aged 8-13
  • blindfolded sighted children, same age group
  • Materials
  • eight tactile pictures of common objects

5
DAngiulli et al.s (1998) Procedure
  • On trial 1
  • The children identified the 8 tactile pictures
    one by one.
  • On trial 2
  • They identified the same pictures, but in
    different orders.

6
DAngiulli et al.s (1998) Accuracy Results
  • Trial 1
    Trial 2
  • Blind children 46 56
  • Blindfolded
  • sighted children 9 28

7
Why Accuracy of at Most 56?
  • The perceiver may find information about
    figure-ground relations, orientation, occlusion,
    the observers vantage point, and three
    dimensionality in tactile pictures very difficult
    to appreciate (Eriksson, 1998).

8
DAngiulli et al.s (1998) Important Findings
  • The children often knew where to correct
    themselves on trial 2
  • The sighted children repeated 95 of the correct
    suggestions and only 3 of the incorrect ones.
  • The blind repeated 100 versus 24.
  • They did so without any feedback.

9
Kennedy and Bais (2002) Proposal
  • The children in DAngiulli et al. (1998) might
    have a criterion to judge how well their
    suggestions matched the shapes in the pictures,
    and to decide whether to stay with them.
  • Kennedy and Bai (2002) called this criterion
    fit.

10
What is Fit?
  • Fit is the degree of correspondence
  • between the suggested name and the
  • shapes
  • proportions, and
  • orders
  • of the parts in the picture.

11
Reasoning about Fit
  • If most lines are apt for the suggested name, a
    person may give a high fit, and is more likely to
    stay with this name.
  • .
  • Otherwise, the person may give a low fit, and is
    more likely to change the name.
  • On average, correct names for tactile pictures
    should have higher fit with the pictures than
    incorrect names.

12
Reasoning about Fit (Contd)
  • Relatively high fit
  • Ss. are most likely to stay with the name
  • on a later trial.
  • Relatively low fit
  • Ss. are most likely to change the name to
    another
  • candidate on a later trial.
  • Fit for the names that stay should be higher than
    fit for the names that are changed.

13
Kennedy and Bai (2002) Exp. 1 Stimuli
Demonstration (optic and haptic) star
Practice (haptic) tree
Old/repeated (trials 1 and 2) fork, hammer, key,
person, scissors, swan, toothbrush, umbrella
New (trial 2 only) car, sailboat
14
Exp. 1 Procedure
  • Twelve blindfolded sighted university students
    named 8 tactile pictures, each within 2 minutes.
  • They gave fit judgments for these suggestions,
    using a 7-point scale (1 means a very
    low fit, 7 a perfect fit).
  • After a break, they named the same pictures again
    and judged fit, but in different orders and mixed
    with 2 new pictures.

15
Exp. 1 Results on Accuracy
  • Accuracy of the 8 suggestions on trial 1 61
  • Accuracy of the 8 suggestions on trial 2 69
  • (71 for all 10, including old and new).

16
Larger Dimension MeansHigher Accuracy Rate
  • The largest dimension of the pictures in K and B
    (2002) was 22.5 cm (accuracy gt 60)
  • - 50 bigger than Lederman et al. (1990)
  • (accuracy up to 33).
  • - 30 bigger than DAngiulli et al. (1998)
  • (accuracy up to 56).

17
Exp. 1 Results on Accuracy and Fit
  • On trial 1,
  • mean fit for the correct names 5.6 (on the
    7-point scale).
  • mean fit for the incorrect names 3.6
  • (p lt 0.001).

18
Exp. 1 Results on Accuracy and Fit (Contd)
  • On trial 2, for the 8 old pictures,
  • mean fit for the correct names 5.8
  • mean fit for the incorrect names 3.8
  • (p lt 0.001).
  • On trial 2, for all 10 pictures,
  • mean fit for the correct names 5.8
  • mean fit for the incorrect names 3.7
  • (p lt 0.001).

19
Exp. 1 Results on Repetition and Fit
  • Names that stayed on trial 2 had a mean fit of
    5.3 on trial 1.
  • Names that were changed on trial 2 had a mean fit
    of 2.9 on trial 1
  • (p lt 0.02).

20
Exp. 1 Conclusions
  • Correct names had higher fit scores than
    incorrect names. So fit judgments can predict
    accuracy for identification of tactile pictures.
  • Names with lower fit judgments were more likely
    to be changed than names with higher fit
    judgments. So fit might be the criteria the
    participates used to decide whether to stay with
    previous responses.

21
Exp. 2 Rationale
  • High-fit pictures might be remembered as a single
    chunk, efficiently, since details did not have to
    be memorized separately.
  • Ill-fitting pictures might have extra parts that
    presented difficulty in recognition memory.
  • It is likely that the more parts of the display
    corresponded to the suggested referent, the more
    it is accessible in our memory task. So higher
    fit judgments should be coupled with better
    memory of tactile pictures.

22
Exp. 2 Stimuli
Demonstration (optic and haptic) apple
Practice (haptic) tree
Old/repeated (trials 1 and 2) cup, sailboat,
table, telephone, plus the 8 stimuli used in Exp.
1 (all inverted on trial 2).
New (trial 2 only) star, triangle, car (turned
90-degree clockwise), house (turned 90-degree
counterclockwise), coat hanger and pine tree
(inverted).
23
Exp. 2 Procedure
  • Twelve blindfolded Ss. ( subjects) saw tactile
    pictures twice, the 2nd time mixed with new,
    distraction pictures. On trial 2 they were asked
    whether they saw each tactile picture on trial 1.
  • To increase the difficulty of the task
  • - twelve pictures on trial 1, 6 more on trial
    2
  • - time allowed was 90s on trial 1, 45s on
    trial 2
  • - trial 2 introduced various orientations.

24
Exp. 2 Results and Conclusion
  • Pictures successfully remembered as old had a
    mean fit of 5.4 on trial 1.
  • Pictures failed to be recognized as old had a
    mean fit of 2.5 on trial 1.
  • (p lt 0.001).
  • Therefore, fit can predict performance on
    recognition memory for tactile pictures.

25
Exp. 3 Procedure and Findings
  • Another group of 12 blindfolded Ss. assessed fit
    of the suggestions offered by matched Exp. 1 Ss.
  • Results mean Pearson correlation coefficient of
    the 12 pairs of Ss. was 0.6 all rs were
    positive (p lt .0001, two-tailed binomial).
  • - Six of the rs reached significant level
    beyond p .05.

26
Exp. 3 Conclusions
  • Touch is consistent across participants.
  • Fit judgment is based on physical correspondence
    between lines and possible referents. It is an
    objective matter.

27
Exp. 4 Procedure
  • Still another group of blindfolded Ss. each
    touched 2 pictures and judged fit for their names
    offered by matched Exp. 1 Ss.
  • (These 2 pictures received the highest/lowest
    fit scores in Exp. 1.)
  • Then, they gave confidence rating on a 7-point
    scale for their fit judgments (1 low, 7 high).

28
Exp. 4 Findings
  • The difference between fit judgments for the 2
    pictures per participant was 3.0, twice that of
    the difference between the confidence judgments,
    p lt .007.
  • This is because both high and low fit judgments
    received high confidence scores, with mean
    confidence of 6.4 for high fit judgments (M
    6.6), and mean confidence of 4.9 for low fit
    judgments (M 3.6).

29
Exp. 4 Conclusions
  • Even low fit judgments can be coupled with
    relatively high confidence judgments, since poor
    fit is based on assessment of the correspondence
    between the physical features of the picture and
    the physical features of a suggested identity.
  • Fit judgments cannot be substituted by confidence
    judgments.

30
Exp. 5 Procedure and Findings
  • A group of Ss. visually judged fit for the names
    offered by matched Exp. 1 Ss.
  • Results fit judgments of the 2 groups
  • (visual and tactual group) were positively
    correlated.
  • Therefore, touch and vision extract
  • the same information from outlines.

31
Overall Conclusions
  • DAngiulli et al.s (1998) study indicated that
    Ss. have a accurate and consistent standard to
    judge their identification of tactile pictures.
    K and Bs (2002) experiments supported the idea
    that the basis for this standard is fit, which is
    the physical correspondence between the picture
    and its suggested name.
  • Fit judgments of tactile pictures are related
    usefully to identification accuracy, repetition,
    remembering, confidence, and visual judgments.

32
Blind People and Outline Drawings
Lines copying surface edges give us surface edge
impressions, including lines in tactile pictures.
Drawings identified by blind children (Kennedy,
1993)
33
Kennedy et al.s (1972) study on Tactile Pictures
Kennedy, Fox, and OGradys (1972) raised-line
drawings included four imprints and four
projections
34
Kennedy et al.s (1972) Study
  • The stimuli were 8 tactile pictures measured
    about 10 cm by 7 cm.
  • Thirty-four sighted Harvard students were asked
    to identify the pictures, firstly by touch (i.e.,
    blindfolded), then by vision.
  • Results haptically identified 2.4 of the 8, the
    easiest being the hand, the hardest being man
    with crossed arms and man with arm up (one
    identification for each).

35
Kennedy et al.s (1972) Study (Contd)
  • A group of 8 blind Harvard students (5
    congenitally blind) tried to identify the same 8
    pictures by touch.
  • On average the blind identified 1.25 pictures.
  • - easiest hand (three times), fork, cup.
  • Eight blind volunteers in Toronto, mostly teens,
    touched each picture for 2 minutes.
  • On average they identified 1.75 pictures.
  • - easiest hand, fork, face (three for
    each).

36
Implications from Kennedy et al. (1972)
  • Errors made by the blind do not appear to be
    random, but make visual sense.
  • - e.g., the table was called by the Toronto
    blind volunteers a house (twice) the fork was
    called a tree (twice), a brush, an ice-cream
    cone, etc.
  • There is some practical ability with pictures in
    the blind. On the other hand, the 10-20
    accuracy rate calls for improvement. Perhaps
    providing a suitable context will do?

37
References
DAngiulli, A., Kennedy, J. M., Heller, M. A.
(1998). Blind children recognizing tactile
pictures respond like sighted children given
guidance in exploration. Scandinavian Journal of
Psychology, 39, 187-190. Eriksson, Y. (1988).
Tactile Pictures Pictorial Representations for
the Blind 1784-1940. Gothenburg Gothenburg
University Press. Kennedy, J. M. (1993). Drawing
and the Blind. New Haven, CT Yale University
Press. Kennedy, J. M., Bai, J. (2002). Haptic
pictures Fit judgments predict identification,
recognition memory, and confidence. Perception,
31(8), 1013-1026. Kennedy, J. M., Fox, N.,
OGrady, K. (1972). Can haptic pictures help the
blind see? Harvard Graduate School of Education
Bulletin, 16, 22-23. Lederman, S. J., Klatzky, R.
L., Chataway, C., Summers, C (1990). Visual
mediation and the haptic recognition of
two-dimensional pictures of common objects.
Perception and Psychophysics, 47, 54-64.
Write a Comment
User Comments (0)
About PowerShow.com