Title: Bilingual Processing of English Aspectual Properties
1Bilingual Processing of English Aspectual
Properties
- S. Seegmiller, D. Townsend, M. Call, S. Mancini,
N. Ilia - Montclair State University
2Introduction Telicity and Aspect
- Comprehension requires understanding of the
aspectual properties of events - Three morphological, syntactic, and/or semantic
properties seem to play a role in denoting events
as ongoing, recurring, and bounded
3Introduction Telicity and Aspect
- Three morphological, syntactic, and/or semantic
properties seem to play a role in denoting events
as ongoing, recurring, and bounded - Morphological aspect (also known as Viewpoint
aspect)
4Introduction Telicity and Aspect
- Three morphological, syntactic, and/or semantic
properties seem to play a role in denoting events
as ongoing, recurring, and bounded - Morphological aspect (also known as Viewpoint
aspect) - Object number and definiteness
5Introduction Telicity and Aspect
- Three morphological, syntactic, and/or semantic
properties seem to play a role in denoting events
as ongoing, recurring, and bounded - Morphological aspect (also known as Viewpoint
aspect) - Object number and definiteness
- Telicity
6Morphological Aspect
- Morphological aspect (also known as viewpoint
aspect) occurs when there is some morphological
marker indicating whether an event is completed
(perfective) or ongoing (imperfective) - Mario fixed the tire
- Mario was fixing the tire
7Object Number and Definiteness
- Singular objects, either definite or indefinite,
may denote either bounded or unbounded events - Fred pushed a/the cart (unbounded)
- Fred baked a/the cake (bounded)
- Typically, plural objects are associated with
ongoing or repeated events - Fred baked cakes
8Telicity
- Telicity is a semantic property of verbs and/or
predicates that refers to events that have (or
don't have) and inherent end point - Susan arrived (telic)
- Susan drew well (atelic)
- Susan drew a circle (telic)
- Susan drew a circle every day (atelic)
- Susan pushed the cart (atelic)
- Susan pushed the cart to the store (telic)
9Previous Research and Findings with Native
English Sepakers
- We are exploring a series of interrelated
questions about on-line comprehension - What information do comprehenders (readers and
listeners) use to determine aspectual
information about an event? - What is the source of this information?
- At what point in the process of comprehending
does this information become available?
10Previous Research and Results with Native
Speakers of English
- Research methods
- Self-paced reading experiments
- Speaker detection experiments
- Probe recognition experiments
- Forced-choice tests
- Analysis of corpus data
11Previous Research with Native Speakers of English
- Brief overview of our methods and main findings
- We used garden-path (GP) sentences like those
below in self-paced reading experiments - The actor tripped on the stage amused the
audience - ... rested ...
- ... captured ...
- ... admired ...
- Reading times are faster for on after tripped
and rested
12Previous Research and Results with Native
Speakers of English
- Brief overview of our main findings
- Comprehenders use subcategorization information
(e.g. transitive/intransitive) immediately for
parsing decisions, but they do not use
information on telicity for parsing - In self-paced reading tasks, comprehenders read
the words following atelic verbs faster than
words following telic verbs, but sentence-final
words are read faster after telic verbs
13Story Comprehension
- In a story comprehension task, participants read
a story one sentence at a time, pressing a key
when ready for the next sentence. - The firemen rescued a survivor.
- The firemen were rescuing a survivor.
- The firemen rescued survivors.
- The firemen were rescuing survivors.
- Probe rescue survivor
14Story Comprehension Results
- The telicity of the verb and the object type had
an effect on response times - Response times were faster after telic verbs than
after atelic verbs - Response times were faster with singular than
with plural objects for immediate probes - The effect of predicate telicity (VerbObject)
appears only for delayed probes, not immediate
probes
15Other Results
- Atelic verbs are more flexible aspectually than
telic verbs - In an atelic context, the reading time on a
sentence-final word is longer when the
sentence-final word is singular than when its
plural - Tony worked in a bakery. He made a cake.
- Tony worked in a bakery. He made cakes.
16General Summary
- Comprehenders make immediate use of verb
subcategorization for parsing but not verb
telicity - Probe recognition times are faster after telic
verbs - Reading times are faster after atelic verbs
- The telicity of a predicate depends on a variety
of factors, including the semantics of the verb,
the number of the object, adverbials, etc.
17Extending the Research
- Are our results valid only for speakers of
English, or are these characteristics true
cross-linguistically? - To answer these questions, we are beginning to
explore two areas - Experiments in other languages
- Experiments with non-native speakers of English
to investigate the influence of L1 on the
processing of English
18Extending the ResearchPilot Study with ESL
Students
- Saving the Children
- The tornado struck the school with little
warning, - It was destroyed.
- The firemen didn't think
- there would be many survivors.
- Suddenly, one of the men heard voices in the
wreckage. - Did the tornado strike a school?
19Extending the ResearchPilot Study with ESL
Students
- Participants read one sentence at a time on the
screen and press a key when they are ready for
the next sentence - A question appears on the screen either
immediately after the sentence or three sentences
later - Reading times (and errors) are recorded
20Participants
- 63 native speakers of English
- 24 ESL students, native speakers of
- 7 speakers of aspectually explicit languages
- Polish (1), Russian (4), Turkish (2)
- 4 speakers of an aspectually non-explicit
language Mandarin - 13 speakers of other languages
21Results for Native Speakers of English
22Results for Non-native Speakers of English
23Comparison of Native and Non-native Speakers of
English
- Overall target sentence reading times
- Native speakers of English 2112 ms
- Non-native speakers of English 3274 ms
- For native speakers, morphological aspect and
verb telicity had an effect on reading times - For non-native speakers, object number had an
effect on reading times
24Examining Morphological Aspect
- Although the effect of morphological aspect on
reading times seems large for non-native
speakers, the difference is not significant. - To investigate this further, we selected two
subsets from our sample of non-native speakers - Speakers of L1s in which aspect is marked
morphologically (Russian, Polish, Turkish) - Speakers of a language in which aspect is not
marked morphologically (Mandarin)
25Results Native and Non-native Speakers of
English
26Tentative Conclusions
- Reading times are generally faster in contexts
that allow fewer options (telic verbs, singular
objects) - The extent to which the L1 explicitly marks
morphological aspect influences the way
English-language learners attend to explicit cues
in processing English
27Future Directions
- Increase the sample size of non-native speakers
of different L1's, especially those with
different verbal and nominal morphology - Prepare materials in other languages (e.g.
Mandarin, Russian, Turkish) to compare processing
strategies across languages
28Acknowledgements
- We would like to thank
- J. Magliano and M.C. Schleich for sharing their
story comprehension materials with us. See J.P.
Magliano and M.C. Schleich (2000), Verb aspect
and situation models. Discourse Processes, 29,
83-112. - Tina Bollettieri for scoring the CELT test of
English grammar - Joanna Musial for providing experimental results
on the role of context for native speakers of
English