Title: The Woman
1Chapter 9
Prosocial Behavior
2Chapter Outline
- Defining Prosocial Behavior
- Goals of Prosocial Behavior
- Gaining Genetic and Material Benefits
- Gaining Social Status and Approval
- Managing Self-Image
- Managing Our Moods and Emotions
- Does Pure Altruism Exist?
3Defining Prosocial Behavior
Prosocial Behavior
Benevolence
Pure Altruism
4Type of Behavior
Defining Prosocial Behavior
Definition
Example
Prosocial Behavior
Benevolence
Pure Altruism
5Type of Behavior
Defining Prosocial Behavior
Definition
Example
Any action intended to benefit another
(regardless of motive)
Giving a large tip to a waiter to impress your
boss with your generosity
Prosocial Behavior
Benevolence
Pure Altruism
6Type of Behavior
Defining Prosocial Behavior
Definition
Example
Prosocial Behavior
Benefits another intentionally for no external
reward
Anonymously sending 20 to a charity to make
yourself feel good inside
Benevolence
Pure Altruism
7Type of Behavior
Defining Prosocial Behavior
Definition
Example
Prosocial Behavior
Spontaneo-usly jumping on a railroad track to
help a stranger who has fallen
Benefits another intentionally for no external or
internal reward
Benevolence
Pure Altruism
8Goals of Prosocial Behavior
Defining Prosocial Behavior
- Gaining Genetic and Material Benefits
- Gaining Social Status and Approval
- Managing Self-Image
- Managing Our Moods and Emotions
9Gaining Genetic and Material Benefits
10Insights into the Evolution of Help
Gaining Genetic and Material Benefits
- Inclusive Fitness -
- The ability of ones genes to survive in ones
own offspring AND in any relatives one helps. - Animals share more genes with siblings gt nephews
gt cousins - Helping a close relative promotes the survival of
those genes
11Genetic Relatedness and Helping
Gaining Genetic and Material Benefits
- Would you lend your car to your brother?
- What about your grandfather?
- What about a cousin?
- What about an attractive stranger?
- Michael Cunningham and his colleagues asked
people whether they would be willing to help
other people in different situations
12Cunningham et al., (1995)
80
60
Percentage Volunteering to Help
40
20
0
High(parents, siblings, children)
Mod. (grand-parents)
Low (first cousins)
None (attractive strangers)
Degree of Relatedness
13Insights into the Evolution of Help
Gaining Genetic and Material Benefits
- Reciprocal Aid -
- Help that occurs in return for prior help
- Other animals will help non-relatives if they
live in close proximity and can better survive by
sharing. - People working for organizations who provide more
benefits work harder for the firm.
14Gaining Genetic and Material Benefits
Learning to Help The sense of We
Similarity and Familiarity
Types of Helpers, Victims, and Need
15Expanded sense of we
Gaining Genetic and Material Benefits
- Children exposed to others of different ethnic
and religious backgrounds later develop a feeling
of we-ness with the larger human family
(Piliavin et al., 1981)
European Gentiles who helped Jews escape from
Nazis were exposed to more different ethnicities
growing up than were non-helpers (Oliner
Oliner, 1988)
16Similarity and Familiarity
Gaining Genetic and Material Benefits
- Similarity may be a cue to genetic relatedness
(our relatives look like us) - In 34 studies, 29 found significantly higher
helping for similar over dissimilar others
(Dovidio, 1984)
17Similarity and Familiarity
Gaining Genetic and Material Benefits
- Familiarity may also be a cue to genetic
relatedness - If familiarity is associated with shared genes,
helping familiar others would have generally
helped relatives (Schroeder, Penner, Dovidio,
Piliavin, 1995) - In animal and human societies, familiarity
increases helping.
18Types of Victims, Helpers, and Need
Gaining Genetic and Material Benefits
- The tendency to help relatives is stronger when
the help is more related to survival
Participants in one study were asked to imagine
scenarios like the following
19Types of Victims, Helpers, and Need
Burnstein, Crandall, Kitayama, 1994
- There are three people asleep in different rooms
of a burning house
- A cousin
- A grandfather
- An acquaintance
- You have time to rescue only one.
- Which do you save?
20Types of Victims, Helpers, and Need
Burnstein, Crandall, Kitayama, 1994
- There are three people who need you to run a
small errand to the store
- A cousin
- A sister
- An acquaintance
- You have time to help only one.
- Whose errand do you run?
21Burnstein, Crandall, Kitayama (1994)
3.0
For everyday help, people tended to help close
relatives more than non-relatives
2.5
Tendency to Help
2.0
1.5
1.0
High(parents, siblings, children)
Mod. (grand-parents)
Low (first cousins)
None (acquaintances)
Degree of Relatedness
22Burnstein, Crandall, Kitayama (1994)
3.0
The difference became even more pronounced in
life-or-death situations
2.5
Tendency to Help
2.0
1.5
1.0
High(parents, siblings, children)
Mod. (grand-parents)
Low (first cousins)
None (acquaintances)
Degree of Relatedness
23Gaining Social Status and Approval
24Social Responsibility norm -
Gaining Social Status and Approval
- Societal rule that people should help those who
need their assistance.
25Bystanders as sources of help
Gaining Social Status and Approval
- The bystander effect
- individuals who observe an emergency are less
likely to help when others are present than when
they are alone. - Diffusion of responsibility -
- Tendency for each group member to dilute personal
responsibility for acting by spreading it among
all other group members.
26Bystanders as sources of help
Gaining Social Status and Approval
The Kitty Genovese Story
27Bystanders as sources of information about helping
Gaining Social Status and Approval
- Pluralistic Ignorance -
- Phenomenon that occurs when bystanders to an
emergency, trying to look poised, give misleading
cues to others that no help is needed
28Bystanders as sources of information about helping
Gaining Social Status and Approval
- In one study, researchers pumped smoke into a lab
while students filled out a questionnaire. - Some students were left alone
- Some with 2 other real participants
- Some with 2 other confederates who pretended
nothing was wrong
29Latane Darley (1968)
80
60
Percentage Reporting Smoke
40
20
0
Alone
With 2 other real subjects
With 2 calm confederates
30Bystanders as sources of information about helping
Gaining Social Status and Approval
- Results suggest that people look to others to
provide information. - If no one else seems upset, that suggests this
isnt an emergency.
31Bystanders as sources of approval or disapproval
Gaining Social Status and Approval
- Sometimes people assume help would be seen as an
unwelcome intrusion. - When a woman fighting with a man shouted I
dont even know you! - help more likely than if
she shouted - I dont know why I married you! (Shotland
Straw, 1976)
32Effects of Onlookers on Decisions to Help
Others as Sources of Help
Others as Sources of Whether Helping is Called For
Others as Sources of Approval or Disapproval for
Helping
The Helping Decision
33Gaining Social Status and Approval
Need for Approval Awareness of the Helping Norm
Helping Models
Gender and Help
34Need for Approval
Gaining Social Status and Approval
- Students who score higher on a scale measuring
the need for approval from others donate more
money - But only if the donation is public
35Awareness of the Helping Norm
Gaining Social Status and Approval
- Very young children are not aware of the norm
requiring people to help those in need. - Between 6 and 9 years of age, they become aware
of the norm. - At this age, they will help IF an adult is
present (Froming, Allen, Jensen, 1985)
36Helping Models
Gaining Social Status and Approval
- Observing another giving help can teach young
children about the helping norm. - For adults, a model can remind them of the
helping norm.
37Gender and Help
Gaining Social Status and Approval
- Women are universally perceived as kinder, more
soft-hearted, and more helpful (Williams Best,
1990)
But over 90 of Carnegie Hero awards go to men
(for saving, or attempting to save, the life of
another). Why?
38Managing Self-Image
39Managing Self-Image
Personal Norms
Religious Codes
Labeling
Self-Focus
Deciding Not to Help Friends or to Seek their Help
40Personal Norms Religious Codes
Managing Self-Image
- Personal Norm -
- Internalized beliefs and values that combine to
form a persons inner standards for behavior. - (example I believe that I personally should give
blood to the blood bank)
41Personal Norms Religious Codes
Managing Self-Image
- People who define themselves as highly committed
to their spiritual beliefs are, compared to the
less committed, twice as likely to volunteer time
to help the needy (Gallup, 1984)
42Labeling and Self-focus
Managing Self-Image
- Labeling a child as kind and helpful increases
his or her later willingness to donate prizes to
other children (Grusec Redler, 1980)
43SELF FOCUS
Higher Helping Likelihood
? Internal Focus on Value of Helping
? Is Victims Need Legitimate
? Is Victims Need Prominent
? Are My Personal Problems Small
Self-Focus
Lower Helping Likelihood
44Deciding not to help friends or seek help
Gaining or Maintaining Social Status
- In one study, students gave more clues to friends
than to strangers playing a non self relevant
game
But, they gave more clues to strangers when the
task was said to reflect intelligence (Tesser
Smith, 1980) WHY?
45Failing to seek needed help
Focus on Social Dysfunction
- Gender Males are generally less likely to
request help than females.
Age Children after age 7 and 8 realize that
asking for help may reflect poorly on their
esteem. After age 60, there is another drop.
46Failing to seek needed help
Focus on Social Dysfunction
- Self-esteem High self-esteem individuals are
LESS likely to seek help.
KEY people fail to ask for needed help to avoid
lower self-esteem.
47Managing Our Emotions and Moods
48Managing Emotional Arousal in Emergencies
Managing our Emotions and Moods
- Arousal/Cost-reward model -
- View that observers of suffering help to relieve
their own personal distress (Dovidio, Piliavin,
Gaertner, Schroeder, Clark, 1991) - We will help in emergency if
49Arousal/Cost-Reward Model of Helping
if Arousal is strong
if Cost of Helping is Small
Increased Chance That Help Will Be Offered
Increased Chance That Help Will Be Offered
Observation of Another in Clear Need of Aid
Increased Negative Emotional Arousal
if Rewards are Large
if We connection
50Managing Emotional Arousal in Non- Emergencies
Managing our Emotions and Moods
- Mood management hypothesis -
- Idea that people use helping tactically to manage
their moods - Throughout life, we learn that helping others can
lead to rewards. - This reward makes us feel good, and we learn to
use helping to manage our mood.
51Managing Moods and Emotions
Presence of Sadness
Costs/Benefits of Helping
Gourmets and Gourmands
52Presence of Sadness
Managing our Emotions and Moods
- Helping can be increased by events triggering
temporary sadness - Reminiscing about unhappy experiences
- Reading depressing statements
- Failing at a task
- Witnessing harm to another
53Costs/benefits of Helping
Managing our Emotions and Moods
- Students in one study were put into either
- happy
- sad or
- neutral mood
- Then given an opportunity to help a non-profit
organization
54Costs/benefits of Helping
Managing our Emotions and Moods
- The benefits of helping were either
- Low - help was for Little League
- High - American Cancer Society
- Costs of helping were either
- Low Sit at donations desk
- High Collect door-to-door
55Benefit
Low
High
Low
Cost
High
Happy students helped more than those in a
neutral mood, with little regard for costs and
benefits
80
60
Volunteering
40
20
0
Happy
Neutral
Mood
Weyant (1976)
56Benefit
Low
High
But students in a sad mood only helped when
benefits were high, and costs were low
Low
Cost
High
80
60
Volunteering
40
20
0
Happy
Sad
Neutral
Mood
Weyant (1976)
57Gourmets and Gourmands
Managing our Emotions and Moods
- Those in sad moods are like gourmets - with
hearty appetites but picky tastes - They help more if the opportunity is a good one
but less if it is a poor one.
People in a positive mood act like gourmands - of
hearty appetite, but indiscriminate taste. They
avail themselves of any opportunity to help
58Gourmets and Gourmands
Managing our Emotions and Moods
- People in a positive mood seem to view themselves
and their environments in positive terms.
When everything is positive, we are generally
less concerned with scanning the details of the
environment.
59Does Pure Altruism Exist?
- Components
- Perspective-Taking - The process of mentally
putting oneself in anothers position. - Empathic Concern - Compassionate feelings caused
by taking the perspective of a needy other.
60Does Pure Altruism Exist?
- Students in one experiment were given the
opportunity to help a suffering student (Elaine)
by taking her place in an electric shock
experiment. - Half could easily escape the whole situation by
leaving immediately. - The other half would have to stay and watch as
she received the remaining shocks.
61Does Pure Altruism Exist?
- Students given the easy opportunity to escape
usually took it. - But if they felt high empathy for Elaine
- They were more likely to help her out.
- Dan Batson and colleagues argue that empathy
engages pure altruism, and overrides selfish
motivations.
62An egoistic alternative
- Cialdini and his colleagues argue that there is
an egoistic explanation of these findings - Empathy causes an observer to feel kinship with
the victim, thus tapping into a basic selfish
motivation to serve myself by serving those who
share my genes.