Title: Overconfidence: in psychology and finance
1Overconfidence in psychology and finance
2Introduction
- Traditional view Homo Economicus
- Reality investor behavior is difficult to
reconcile with rationality of predictions of
standard finance models (Glaser et al., 2003). - Explanation overconfidence
3Theoretical Insights into the Phenomenon of
Overconfidence
- Profession. Gender Differences. Overconfidence
in Children. Cultural impact - Overconfidence found in students, members of the
armed forces, CIA analysts, entrepreneurs,
clinical psychologists, bankers, executives,
managers, lawyers, and civil engineers. - No differences between men and women in
calibration. Men are more overconfident in their
performance estimates. - Overconfidence is already present in children.
- Cross-cultural differences in calibration.
Overconfidence in general knowledge is stronger
among Asian than among Western subject groups.
4Confidence
- Is strongly related to the notion of certainty,
and strength of the feeling of certainty is
embodied in the corresponding level of
confidence. - Zakay and Tsal (1993) feeling of confidence
accompany any type of mental activity. - Degree of confidence can vary from zero to
absolute level. - Can be expressed
- -gtverbally - very, totally, absolutely, quite
- -gtnumerically- 100, 9 to 10, 50/50 etc.
- Confidence internal to a subject, feeling of
certainty about some state of reality.
5Degree of Confidence as a Subjective Probability
- Decisions are based on beliefs concerning the
likelihood of uncertain events - These beliefs are expressed in numerical form as
subjective probabilities Subjective probabilities
- the probabilities that people generate in their
own minds to express their uncertainty about the
possibility of the occurrence of various events
or outcomes (Bar-Hillel, 2001). - Subjectivist point of view a probability is a
degree of belief in a proposition. - Expresses a purely internal state there is no
right, correct or objective probability
residing somewhere in reality against which
ones degree of belief can be compared.
6Degree of Confidence as a Subjective Probability
- Adequacy of subjective probabilities to objective
reality can be measured via a question with the
verifiable as correct or non-correct answer, and
a measure of the degree of confidence in that
judgment. - If accuracy does not match the level of
confidence, then a judgmental bias exists.
7Overconfidence
- Overconfidence concerns the fact that people
overestimate how much they actually know when
they are p percent sure that they have answered a
question correctly or predicted correctly, they
are in fact right on average less than p percent
of the time (Bar-Hillel, 2001). - Overconfidence arises from not knowing the limits
of ones knowledge (Conger and Wolstein, 2004). - Two types of overconfidence - specific and
generic. - gtSpecific overconfidence - overestimation of
the probability of a specific hypothesis
or a designated outcome gtGeneric
overconfidence - overestimation of the likelihood
of the hypothesis, considered to be the
most probable by the individual. - In this manner one draws distinction between the
estimation of the likelihood that ones answer is
correct and the likelihood of judgmental answer
(Pulford, 1996).
8Overconfidence
- Local and global confidences (May, 1887, 1988)
- gtLocal confidence - average confidence across
questions - gtGlobal confidence - confidence in the total
number, or percentage, of questions
individuals have answered correctly. - There are several psychological findings that are
often summarized under the concept of
overconfidence in financial literature
miscalibration, the better than average effect,
illusion of control, and unrealistic optimism
(Glaser and Weber, 2004) .
9Miscalibration
- In studies of calibration, participants answer a
series of multiple-choice questions and stipulate
their confidence of being correct for each
answer. - Calibration is tested by comparing the percentage
of questions that a participant has answered
correctly with the participants average
confidence in the answers to these questions. - Individuals are considered to be well calibrated
in their confidence in the correctness of their
knowledge, if the following condition is
satisfied over the long run, for all answers
assigned by them a given probability of being
correct (e.g., 0.8), the actual proportion
correct (80) equals the probability assigned
(Koriat, Lichtenstein, and Fischhoff, 1980). - Of those responses made with confidence P, about
P should be correct (Adams, 1957).
10Miscalibration
- Most of the people are not well-calibrated and
demonstrate miscalibration. - Miscalibration - a systematic deviation from
perfect calibration. It is defined as an
unwarranted belief in the correctness of ones
answer - Usually for all questions the proportion of
correct answers is lower than the assigned
probability. - Studies that analyze assessments of uncertain
quantities using the fractile method usually find
that peoples probability distributions are too
tight. - People are often asked to state a 90 percent
confidence interval for several uncertain
quantities. - Findings the percentage of surprises, i.e. the
percentage of true values that fall outside the
confidence interval, is higher than 10 percent
(Glaser and Weber, 2004).
11Better than average effect
- Based on the fact, that people tend to exaggerate
their talents. - Taylor and Brown (1988) document in their survey
that people have unrealistically positive views
of the self, i.e. they think about themselves as
possessing above the average abilities, compared
to other people e.g. with regard to skills or
positive personal traits. - 82 of a group of students rank themselves among
the 30 percent of drivers with the highest
driving safety (Svenson,1981).
12Illusion of control
- Illusion of control is embodied in the
exaggeration of the degree to which one can
control her fate. Subjects prone to the illusion
of control, tend to underestimate the role of
chance in human affairs and to misperceive games
of chance as games of skill (Kahneman and Riepe,
1998). - An expectancy of a personal success probability
inappropriately higher than the objective
probability would warrant (Langer, 1975)
13Unrealistic optimism
- Closely related to the illusion of control bias
is the phenomenon of unrealistic optimism about
future life events. - Mentally healthy people tend to exhibit
psychological biases that encourage optimism,
collectively known as positive illusions
(Johnson et al., 2006). - Most peoples beliefs are biased in the direction
of optimism (Kahneman and Riepe, 1998). Optimists
also underestimate the likelihood of bad outcomes
over which they have no control. Most
undergraduates, for example, believe that they
are less likely than their roommates to develop
cancer or to have a heart attack before the age
of fifty (Kahneman and Riepe, 1998). - The question whether all these concepts
(miscalibration, better than average effect,
illusion of control and unrealistic optimism) are
related is mainly unexplored.
14Factors influencing degree of overconfidence in
the experimental settings
- Keasey and Watson (1989) identified four factors
that have impact on the accuracy-confidence
relationship the complexity of the task, amount
of feedback given, motivation level of the
subjects, and skill of the subjects. - Hard-Easy Effect
- The hard-easy effect occurs when the degree of
overconfidence increases with the difficulty of
the questions, where the difficulty is measured
by the percentage of correct answers. - People are overconfident with general-knowledge
items of moderate or extreme difficulty. As tasks
get easier, overconfidence is reduced
(Lichtenstein et al., 1982).
15Factors influencing degree of overconfidence in
the experimental settings
- Is more information always better?
- Useless information tends to increase subjects
confidence. However this had no impact on the
improvement on accuracy. - Oskamp (1965) found that often people do not
realize uselessness of the information given to
them by the experimenter, and their confidence
increases without justification, whereas,
accuracy doesnt. - Peterson and Pitz (1986) theorized that when
several pieces of useful information are given
they reduce overconfidence. - Calibration and motivation
- The effect of increasing the stakes in judgment
tasks has been shown to have mixed results.
Fischhoff (1982) found no improvement in
judgment, but other researchers have found
changes in both directions (Kruglanski and
Freund, 1983).
16Factors influencing degree of overconfidence in
the experimental settings
- Experience, Feedback and Calibration
- Even experts, who by definition know a lot about
a specialized topic, are often unable to express
precisely how much they do not know (Russo,
Schoemaker, 1992). - Russo and Schoemaker (1992) claim that (job)
relevant experience to the confidence-quiz,
reduces overconfidence. - What concerns their professional experience,
experts better know what they dont know (Russo
and Schoemaker (1992). - However, in the study by Heath and Tversky
(1991), greater overconfidence was found for
tasks which subjects considered they had more
expertise in
17Factors influencing degree of overconfidence in
the experimental settings
- Experience, Feedback and Calibration
- It is one of the big mistakes to equate
experience and learning. Experience is
inevitable learning is not. The failure of
individuals to learn from the experience is the
cause of persisting, despite experience,
overconfidence (Ruso et al, 1992). - In order to learn individuals need to get
feedback about the accuracy of their answers. On
time received feedback can reduce bias towards
overconfidence in professionals (Russo et.al,
1992) .
18Measurement of Overconfidence
- There are two types of calibration assessment
techniques used in the psychological experiments
- 1) making probability judgments about
discrete propositions, and - 2) the calibration of probability density
functions assessed for uncertain numerical
quantities. - Measurement Of Calibration In Discreet
PropositionsTasks - Procedure there is a questionnaire offering no
alternatives, one alternative, two, three or more
alternatives. - Assessor either provides his own answer (no
alternatives) or selects from the given ones. - Then she states the probability that this choice
is correct.
19Calibration curve
- Derivation procedure 1st step is collection of
probability assessments for events whose correct
answer are known 2nd step is grouping of similar
assessments 3rd step computation of ratio of
correct items in each constructed category 4th
step is plotting of mean response of each
category against ratio of correct items. - Calibration is measured by how close points are
to the identity line. Distribution of all points
on the identity line would correspond to perfect
calibration. - Identity line - where confidence exactly equals
accuracy. - Overconfidence the proportions correct are less
than the assessed probabilities, so that the
calibration curve falls below the identity line - Underconfidence the proportions correct are
greater than the assessed probabilities and the
calibration curve lies above the identity line.
Example of a calibration curve with an identity
line (Source Pulford, 1996).
20Calibration Score
- The Brier (1950) score, or the probability score
(PS), is a measure of goodness of a set of
probability assessments. The Brier score is
defined as the average deviation between
predicted event probabilities and their real
outcomes. - r is the assessed probability of item i,
- c is the associated outcome. c1 if event occurs,
zero otherwise. - N is a number of forecasts of subject
- The smaller is the Brier score, the higher
accuracy in predictions it represents. - The range of PSB is the closed interval 0, 2
- Briers score doesnt discriminate between
overconfidence and underconfidence.
21Calibration score
- Bias Score
- To discriminate between over- and underconfidence
the bias score is used. Bias score is calculated
as the difference between the mean confidence
level across all questions and the proportion of
correct answers - Bias score average confidence average
correct - A positive bias score represents overconfidence,
and a negative bias score represents
underconfidence. - A bias score of zero indicates an accurately
calibrated person.
22Calibration of the PDF (The
fractile method)
- Uncertainty about the value of a continuous
quantity (e.g. what is the shortest distance from
England to Australia) may be expressed as
probability density function across the possible
values of that quantity (Lichtenstein et al.,
1982). - Subjects usually do not have to draw the entire
function. In this method, the assessor states
values of the uncertain quantity that are
associated with a small number of predetermined
fractiles of the distribution. - For the median or 0.5 fractile, for example the
assessor states a value of the quantity such that
the true value is equally likely to fall above or
below the stated value. - The 0.01 fractile is a value such that there is
only 1 chance in 100 that the true value is
smaller than the stated value.
23Calibration of the PDF (The
fractile method)
- There are two calibration measures for continuous
items. - Interquartile index is the percentage of items
for which the true value falls inside the
interquartile range (i.e., between the 0.25 and
0.75 fractiles). Interquartile index of a
perfectly calibrated person is 0.5. - Surprise index is the percentage of true values
that fall outside the most extreme fractiles
assessed. Large surprise index shows the
inability of the assessor state confidence bounds
wide enough to include as much as possible of the
true values this indicates overconfidence.
24Ways to Reduce Overconfidence and Improve
Calibration/ Debiasing
- Psychologists have tried to train subjects to
avoid judgmental errors, or in other words tried
to debias them. - Training assessors by giving them feedback about
their calibration has shown mixed results. - Adams and Adams (1958) have found modest
improvement after five training sessions .
Fishfoff (1982) reports some successful training
exercises, mostly using large amounts of
well-structured feedback.
25Ways to Reduce Overconfidence and Improve
Calibration/ Debiasing
- Lichtenstein et al., (1982) examined the example
of calibration of weather forecasters. They
conclude that continuance (they have been making
probabilistic forecasts for years),
repetitiveness of the task and the fact that the
outcome feedback for weather forecasters is well
defined and promptly received have high impact
on accuracy of their predictions. - Calibration might be improved for discrete
propositions tasks, but not for the
probabilities assessment by fractile method. - Overconfidence might also be reduced by asking
subjects, prior to the assessment of their
confidence, to think of the reasons why their
answers might be wrong.
26Overconfidence in Finance
- Recently, interest in the consequences of
traders overconfidence on financial decision
making and the functioning of financial markets,
has occurred in behavioral economics.
27Theoretical Overconfidence Finance Literature
- Behavioral finance theories incorporate findings
of psychology research into standard finance
models, and model psychological bias of
overconfidence. - Overconfidence is usually modeled as
overestimation of precision of ones information
(Glaser et al, 2003). Overconfident investors
underestimate the variance of their private
signals, which contain not only information but
also a random error. Stated equivalently
overconfident investors underestimate the
variance of the error term contained in their
signal. - This way of modeling overconfidence captures the
idea that people overestimate the precision of
their knowledge. - Some models assume that the degree of
overconfidence is a stable individual trait and
thus constant over time. However, other models
assume that overconfidence dynamically changes
over time (Glaser et al., 2003).
28Empirical and Experimental Tests of
Overconfidence Models
- Empirical studies, experiments, quasi-experiments
are used to test validity of overconfidence
models. - There are two directions in testing the empirical
validity of theoretical models model assumptions
and model predictions. - Model assumptions of investors overconfidence
can be evaluated by experiments and
questionnaires - Model predictions of the link between market
outcomes and investors overconfidence are tested
by correlation of proxies (e.g. gender, high
returns) or measures of overconfidence (bias
score) and economic variables (e.g. trading
volume). - Its important to mention, that studies using
proxies of overconfidence share the shortcoming
that overconfidence is never directly observed. - Experimental and quasi-experimental design allows
more confident inference about cause and effect
relations.
29Theoretical Models of Overconfidence in Financial
Markets
Macro-level impact of overconfidence (Market-level) Author Micro-level Impact (investor level) Author
Excess trading volume DeBondt and Thaler, 1985 Shiller (2000), Overconfident investors trade more aggressively, i.e. higher trading volume. Odean (1998), Gervais and Odean (2001)
Excess price volatility Scheinkman J.A. and Xiong, W (2003), Benos 1998, Reduction of expected utility DeLong, Shleifer, Summers, and Waldman (1990), Odean (1997)
Overconfidence causes asset price bubbles Scheinkman J.A. and Xiong, W (2003) Undiversified portfolios/ Riskier portfolio choice Odean (1997), Odean (1998)
Icreases market depth Odean (1998), Kyle and Wang (1997), and Benos (1998) Increased tendency to herd Hirshleifer, Subrahmanyam and Titman (1994)
Underrecaction or overreaction of markets to new info Odean (1997) Active portfolio management Lakonishok, Shleifer and Vishny (1992)
Return predictability of financial assets Daniel et al. (1998, 2001) Scheinkman J.A. and Xiong, W (2003) Higher expected returns DeLong, Shleifer, Smmers, and Waldman (1990)
30Empirical Findings and Experimental Results
Macro-level impact of Overconfidence Author Micro-level Impact Author
Trading (overconfidence generates trading) Odean (1997), Statman and Thorley (1997) Level of overconfidence vary with past market returns. Statman et al., 2004
High trading volume in the market. Statman, Thorley, and Vorkink (2004), Kim and Nofsinger (2003). Too much of trading activity. Men trade more than women. Barber and Odean, 2000 Barber and Odean, 2001.
Probability of Bubbles (Top rank belief variablehas a positive and significant effect on the probability of bubbles). Jörg Oechssler, Carsten Schmidt and Wendelin Schnedler (2007) There is relationship between degree of overconfidence, and degree of professionalism Maciejovsky et al., (2003) , Menkhoff et al. (2005) and Biais et al. (2004)
Underrecaction or overreaction of markets to new info Loughran et al. (1995) - overreaction Bernard et al. (1989) - underreaction. Unrealistically positive self-evaluation increases trading volume. Glaser and Weber (2004)
Higher degree of overconfidence reduces traders performance/welfare. Biais et al. (2004), Odean (1999), Barber and Odean (2002).
Mistakes in financial decision making Biais et al. (2004)
1 However when overconfidence was measured as
miscalibration Glaser and Weber (2004) do not
find relation between trading volume and
overconfidence. Same results are found in an
experiment by Biais et al. (2004).
31- Thank You For Your Attention