Emotional Development - PowerPoint PPT Presentation

1 / 66
About This Presentation
Title:

Emotional Development

Description:

Children s ability to choose the most effective strategies for managing their ... anger/frustration and positive affect Laboratory observations have been used to ... – PowerPoint PPT presentation

Number of Views:254
Avg rating:3.0/5.0
Slides: 67
Provided by: JohnWesle6
Category:

less

Transcript and Presenter's Notes

Title: Emotional Development


1
Emotional Development
  • How Children Develop (3rd ed.)
  • Siegler, DeLoache Eisenberg
  • Chapter 10

2
Emotional Intelligence
  • A set of abilities that contribute to competent
    social functioning
  • Being able to motivate oneself and persist in the
    face of frustration
  • Control impulses and delay gratification
  • Identify and understand ones own and others
    feelings
  • Regulate ones moods
  • Regulate the expression of emotion in social
    interactions
  • Empathize with others emotions

3
Emotional Intelligence
  • A better predictor than IQ of how well people
    will do in life, especially in their social lives
  • In research by Walter Mischel, preschoolers
    abilities to delay gratification were found to
    predict their social, emotional, and academic
    competence many years later.

4
Overview
  • I. The Development of Emotions in Childhood
  • II. Regulation of Emotion
  • III. Individual Differences in Emotion and its
    Regulation
  • IV. Childrens Emotional Development in the
    Family
  • V. Culture and Childrens Emotional Development
  • VI. Childrens Understanding of Emotion

5
I. The Development of Emotions in Childhood
  • A. Theories on the Nature and Emergence of
    Emotion
  • B. The Emergence of Emotion in the Early Years
    and Childhood

6
I. The Development of Emotions in Early Childhood
  • Emotion is characterized by a motivational force
    or action tendency and by changes in physiology,
    subjective feelings, and overt behavior.
  • Although most psychologists share this general
    view of emotion, they often do not agree on the
    relative importance of its key components.
  • There is considerable debate, for example, about
    the basic nature of emotions, including whether
    they are innate or partly learned, and about when
    and in what form different emotions emerge during
    infancy.

7
A. Theories on the Nature and Emergence of
Emotion
Discrete Emotions Theory
The Functionalist Approach
Research supports both perspectives to some
degree, and no one theory has emerged as
definitive.
8
Discrete Emotions Theory
  • Argues that
  • Emotions are innate and are discrete from one
    another from very early in life.
  • Each emotion is packaged with a specific and
    distinctive set of bodily and facial reactions.

9
The Functionalist Approach
  • Emphasizes the role of the environment in
    emotional development
  • Proposes that the basic function of emotions is
    to promote action toward achieving a goal
  • Maintains that emotions are not discrete from one
    another and vary somewhat based on the social
    environment

10
B. The Emergence of Emotion in the Early Years
and Childhood
  • To make their interpretations of infants
    emotions objective, researchers have devised
    highly elaborate systems for coding and
    classifying the emotional meaning of infants
    facial expressions.
  • These systems identify emotions first by coding
    dozens of facial cues and then by analyzing the
    combination in which these cues are present.
  • Nonetheless, it is often hard to determine
    exactly which emotions infants are experiencing.
  • It is particularly difficult to differentiate
    among the various negative emotions that young
    infants express.

11
Characteristics of Some Families of Emotions
12
1. Positive Emotions
  • Smiling is the first clear sign of happiness that
    infants express.
  • Young infants smile from their earliest days, but
    the meaning of their smiles appears to change
    with age.
  • Social Smiles are directed toward people and
    first emerge as early as 6 to 7 weeks of age.

13
1. Positive Emotions
  • At about 7 months, infants start to smile
    primarily at familiar people, rather than at
    people in general.
  • After about 3 or 4 months of age, infants laugh
    as well as smile during a variety of activities.
  • During the second year of life, children start to
    clown around and are delighted when they can make
    other people laugh.

14
2. Negative Emotions
  • The first negative emotion that is discernible in
    infants is generalized distress.
  • By 2 months of age, facial expressions of anger
    or sadness can be differentiated from
    distress/pain in some contexts.
  • By the second year of life, differentiating
    between infants anger and other negative
    emotions is no longer difficult.

15
Distress
  • The interpretation of negative emotions is
    complicated by the fact that infants sometimes
    display negative emotions that seem incongruent
    with the situation they are experiencing.
  • It has been suggested that young infants are
    experiencing undifferentiated distress when they
    evidence negative emotion and that anger and
    distress/pain are not differentiated in most
    contexts.

16
Fear
  • The first clear signs of fear emerge at around 6
    or 7 months, when unfamiliar people no longer
    provide comfort and pleasure similar to that
    provided by familiar people.
  • The fear of strangers intensifies and lasts until
    about age 2 but is quite variable across
    individuals and contexts.
  • Other fears are also evident at around 7 months
    and tend to decline after 12 months.

17
Evidence of Fear in Young Children
18
Separation Anxiety
  • Refers to feelings of distress that children,
    especially infants and toddlers, experience when
    they are separated, or expect to be separated,
    from individuals to whom they are attached
  • It is a salient and important type of fear and
    distress that tends to increase from 8 to 13 or
    15 months and then begins to decline.
  • This pattern is observed across many cultures.

19
Separation Anxiety
20
Anger
21
3. The Self-Conscious Emotions
  • Feelings such as guilt, shame, embarrassment, and
    pride that relate to our sense of self and our
    consciousness of others reactions to us.
  • Emerge during the second year of life
  • At about 15 to 24 months of age, some children
    start to show embarrassment when they are made
    the center of attention.
  • By 3 years of age, childrens pride is
    increasingly tied to their level of performance.
  • The situations likely to induce self-conscious
    emotions in children vary somewhat across
    cultures.

22
Guilt and Shame
  • Guilt is associated with empathy for others and
    involves feelings of remorse and regret and the
    desire to make amends.
  • Shame does not seem to be related to concern
    about others.
  • Shame and guilt can be distinguished fairly
    early, but whether children experience guilt or
    shame partly depends on parental practices.

23
4. Normal Emotional Development in Childhood
  • From early to middle childhood, acceptance by
    peers and achieving goals become increasingly
    important sources of happiness and pride.
  • School-age childrens fears are generally related
    to real-life important issues rather than
    imaginary creatures.
  • By the early school years, childrens perceptions
    of others motives and intentions are important
    in determining whether or not they will be
    angered.
  • Children overall become less intense and less
    emotionally negative with age in the preschool
    and early school years.

24
Emotions in Adolescence
  • Adolescence is a time of greater negative emotion
    than middle childhood.
  • Although the increase in the frequency and
    intensity of negative emotions and the decrease
    in positive emotions is small for most
    adolescents, a minority experience a major
    increase in the occurrence of negative emotions,
    often in their relations with their parents.

25
5. Depression
  • The rate of clinical depression, which is less
    than 3 prior to adolescence, is 15 or higher
    from age 15-18.
  • An addition 11 of U.S. youth experience less
    serious symptoms of depression.
  • Hispanic children report more symptoms of
    depressions than do Euro- or African Americans.
  • Children with depression frequently exhibit
    behavior problems.

26
Depression
27
Depression
  • Possible causes of depression include genetic
    factors, maladaptive belief symptoms, feelings of
    powerlessness, negative beliefs and
    self-perceptions, and the lack of social skills
    .
  • Family factors also contribute to depression
  • In many cases depression is likely due to a
    combination of personal vulnerability and
    external stressful factors.
  • Antidepressant drugs are most common treatment

28
Gender and Depression
  • Starting at age 13 to 15 in the United States,
    girls begin showing higher rates of clinical
    depression than do boys.
  • Stressors for girls include concerns about body
    image, early puberty, worries about peer
    acceptance and an increased tendency to ruminate
    on symptoms of their distress and on the meaning
    of their distress.

29
II. Regulation of Emotion
  • A. The Development of Emotional Regulation
  • B. The Relation of Emotional
  • C. Regulation to Social Competence and Adjustment

30
A. The Development of Emotional Regulation
  • The process of initiating, inhibiting, or
    modulating internal feeling states,
    emotion-related physiological processes, and
    emotion-related cognitions or behaviors in the
    service of accomplishing ones goals.
  • Its emergence in childhood is a long, slow
    process.

31
1. Shift from Caregiver Regulation to
Self-Regulation
  • In the first months of life, parents help
    infants regulate their emotional arousal by
    controlling their exposure to stimulating events.
  • By 6 months, infants can reduce their distress by
    averting their gaze and sometimes by
    self-soothing, which is engaging in stylized or
    repetitive rubbing or stroking of their bodies or
    clothing.
  • Between ages 1 and 2, infants increasingly turn
    their attention to non-distressing objects or
    people to distract themselves from sources of
    distress.

32
1. Shift from Caregiver Regulation to
Self-Regulation
  • Over the course of the early years, children
    become more likely to rely on themselves rather
    than their parents when they must delay
    gratification.
  • In addition, they become increasingly able to
    rely on language to manage their emotional
    arousal and to regulate their expression of
    negative emotions.
  • Childrens improving self-regulation is due at
    least in part to the increasing maturation of the
    neurological system.
  • They are also influenced by increases in adults
    expectations of children and to age-related
    improvement in the ability to inhibit motor
    behavior.

33
2. Use of Cognitive Strategies to Control
Negative Emotions
  • Whereas younger children use behavior strategies
    like distracting themselves with play, older
    children also employ cognitive strategies such as
    mentally distracting themselves from negative
    events or trying to see things in a positive
    light.
  • As children age, they are better able to use
    cognitive strategies to adjust to emotionally
    difficult situations.

34
3. Selection of Appropriate Regulatory Strategies
  • The ability to select cognitive or behavioral
    strategies that are appropriate for the situation
    or stressor is aided by
  • Childrens increasing capacity to distinguish
    between stressors that can be controlled and
    those that cannot be.
  • Childrens ability to choose the most effective
    strategies for managing their reactions to these
    stressors.

35
B. The Relation of Emotional Regulation to
Social Competence and Adjustment
  • Social competence is the ability to achieve
    personal goals in social interactions while
    simultaneously maintaining positive relationships
    with others.
  • Emotional regulation has important
    consequences for social competence.

36
III. Individual Differences in Emotion and its
Regulation
  • A. Temperament

37
A. Temperament
  • The constitutionally based individual differences
    in emotional, motor, and attentional reactivity
    and self-regulation that demonstrate consistency
    across situations, as well as relative stability
    over time.
  • Differences in the various aspects of childrens
    emotional reactivity that emerge early in life
    are labeled as dimensions of temperament.

38
Infant Temperament
  • Stella Chess and Alexander Thomas conducted
    pioneering longitudinal research on infant
    temperament.
  • Three categories (based on parents reports)
  • Easy babies (40) adjusted readily to new
    experiences, quickly established routines, and
    generally were cheerful in mood and easy to calm
  • Difficult babies (10) were slow to adjust to
    new experiences, likely to react negatively and
    intensely to stimuli and events, and irregular in
    their bodily functions
  • Slow-to-warm-up babies (15) were somewhat
    difficult at first but became easier over time
  • The remaining infants did not fit into these
    categories.
  • Some dimensions of temperament showed stability
    over time and predicted how children were doing
    years later.

39
Infant Temperament
  • In contrast to Thomas and Chesss approach, many
    contemporary psychologists believe that it is
    important to
  • Assess positive and negative emotion as separate
    components of temperament
  • Differentiate among types of negative
    emotionality
  • Assess different types of regulatory capacity
  • Recent research suggests that infant temperament
    is captured by six dimensions
  • Fearful distress, irritable distress, attention
    span and persistence, activity level, positive
    affect, and rhythmicity

40
Examples of Items in Mary Rothbarts Temperament
Scales
41
Examples of Items in Mary Rothbarts Temperament
Scales
42
1. Stability of Temperament Over Time
  • Children who as infants showed behavioral
    inhibition with novel stimuli also showed
    elevated levels of fear in novel situations at
    age 2 and elevated levels of social inhibition at
    age 4 ½.
  • It is important to note, however, that some
    aspects of temperament tend to be more stable
    than others.

43
2. Temperament and Social Adjustment
  • In a longitudinal study conducted in New Zealand,
    young children who were negative, impulsive, and
    unregulated had more problems as young adults
    with adjustment, including unemployment and
    conflict with roommates, than did their peers
    with other temperaments.
  • Behavioral inhibition in infancy, a
    temperamentally-based style of responding
    characterized by the tendency to be particularly
    fearful and restrained when dealing with novel or
    stressful situations associated with problems
    such as anxiety, depression, and social
    withdrawal at older ages.

44
2. Temperament and Social Adjustment
  • Different problems with adjustment are associated
    with different temperaments.
  • However, childrens adjustment depends on how
    their temperament fits with the demands and
    expectations of the social environments they are
    in, a concept described as goodness of fit.
  • Moreover, the childs temperament and the
    parents socialization efforts seem to affect
    each other over time.

45
Measuring Temperament
  • A number of different methods are used to assess
    temperament
  • Parents or other adults periodically report on
    fearfulness, anger/frustration and positive
    affect
  • Laboratory observations have been used to assess
    behavioral inhibition, emotionality and
    regulatory capacities
  • Physiological measures (heartrate, EEG, cortisol
    level) have proven useful for measuring aspects
    of temperament

46
IV. Childrens Emotional Development in the
Family
  • A. Quality of the Childs Relationships with
    Parents
  • B. Parental Socialization of Emotional Responding

47
Personality
  • Refers to the pattern of behavioral and emotional
    propensities, beliefs and interests, and
    intellectual capacities that characterize an
    individual
  • Has its roots in temperament but is shaped by
    interactions with the social and physical world
  • Chief among these interactions are childrens
    relationships with their parents and their
    parents socialization practices.

48
A. Quality of Childs Relationships with Parents
  • The quality of childrens relationships with
    their parents seems to influence their sense of
    security and how they feel about themselves and
    other people.
  • In turn, these feelings affect childrens
    emotionality, understanding of emotion, emotional
    self-regulation, and emotional responses to
    people and events in their world.

49
B. Parental Socialization of Childrens
Emotional Responding
  • Socialization refers to the processes by which
    individuals, through experience with others,
    develop the skills and ways of thinking and
    feeling, as well as standards and values, that
    allow them to adapt to their group and live with
    other people.
  • Parents, teachers, and other adults are
    important socializers for children, although
    other children, the media, and social
    institutions can play a role in socialization.

50
1. Parents Expression of Emotion
  • The emotions to which children are exposed may
    affect their level of distress and arousal
  • The consistent and open expression of positive
    emotion in the home is associated with positive
    outcomes.
  • In families in which negative emotions are
    predominant, children tend to exhibit low levels
    of social competence and to express negative
    emotions themselves.

51
1. Parents Expression of Emotion
  • The expression of emotion by parents influences
    childrens views about themselves and others in
    their social world.
  • It also provides children with a model of when
    and how to express emotion and may affect
    childrens understanding of what types of
    emotional expressions are appropriate and
    effective in interpersonal relations.
  • Children also influence the expression of emotion
    in the home.

52
2. Parents Reactions to Childrens Emotions
  • Parents who respond to their childrens sadness
    and anxiety by dismissing or criticizing their
    feelings communicate to their children that their
    feelings are not valid.
  • In turn, their children tend to be less
    emotionally and socially competent than children
    whose parents are emotionally supportive.

53
3. Parents Discussion of Emotion
  • Family conversations about emotion are an
    important aspect of childrens emotional
    socialization.
  • Family discussions of emotion are especially
    likely to occur when a family member is
    experiencing a negative emotion.
  • They are more likely to foster childrens
    understanding of emotion if they are supportive
    rather than hostile.

54
V. Culture and Childrens Emotional Development
55
The Role of Culture
  • Although people in all cultures are likely to
    experience many similar emotions, research shows
    that the degree to which different emotions are
    expressed varies considerably across cultures.
  • One reason may be genetic, in that people from
    different racial or ethnic groups may tend to
    have somewhat different temperaments.
  • Parents ideas about the usefulness of expressing
    different emotions also vary across cultures and
    in different subcultures in the United States.

56
The Role of Culture
  • Cultural differences in parenting practices may
    contribute to cross-cultural differences in
    infants expression of emotion.
  • For example, American mothers appear more likely
    than Japanese mothers to encourage childrens
    emotional expressiveness in situations of
    conflict and distress, and corresponding
    cross-cultural differences are observed in
    childrens responses to hypothetical vignettes.

57
The Role of Culture
  • Cultures also differ in the degree to which they
    promote or discourage specific emotions, and
    these differences are often reflected in parents
    socialization of emotion.
  • For example, the Tamang in Nepal are seldom
    supportive when their children experience
    negative emotional arousal because of the
    cultural value on keeping calm and clear of
    emotion.
  • However, parental behavior that is nonsupportive
    by U. S. standards does not appear to have a
    negative impact of the childrens social
    competence within the context of this culture.

58
VI. Childrens Understanding of Emotion
  • A. Identifying the Emotions of Others
  • B. Understanding the Causes of Emotion
  • C. Childrens Understanding of Real and False
    Emotions
  • D. Understanding Simultaneous and Ambivalent
    Emotions

59
A. Identifying the Emotions of Others
  • The first step in the development of emotional
    knowledge is the recognition of different
    emotions in others.
  • By 4 to 7 months, infants can distinguish certain
    emotional expressions, such as happiness and
    surprise.
  • At 8 to 12 months, children demonstrate social
    referencing, the use of a parents facial,
    gestural, or vocal cues to decide how to deal
    with novel, ambiguous, or possibly threatening
    situations.
  • By age 3, children in laboratory studies
    demonstrate a rudimentary ability to label a
    fairly narrow range of emotional expression.

60
A. Identifying the Emotions of Others
  • Young children are best at labeling happiness,
    with the ability to distinguish among different
    negative emotions gradually appearing the late
    preschool and early school years.
  • Most children cannot label more complex emotions
    until early- to mid-elementary school.
  • The ability to discriminate and label different
    emotions helps children respond appropriately to
    their own and others emotions.

61
B. Understanding the Causes and Dynamics of
Emotions
  • A variety of studies have shown rapid development
    over the preschool and school years in childrens
    understanding of the kinds of situations that
    typically evoke different emotions in others.
  • Two-year-olds can identify happy situations in
    stories, although children are not accurate in
    identifying sad situations until age 4.

62
B. Understanding the Causes and Dynamics of
Emotions
  • In everyday discussions of emotions, 2-year-olds
    mention emotions in appropriate ways.
  • By age 4 to age 6, childrens explanations for
    why peers experience negative emotions in
    real-life situations in their school are somewhat
    similar to those of adults.
  • Children get more skilled at explaining the
    causes of emotion across the preschool and school
    years.
  • With age, children also come to understand that
    people can feel emotions based on reminders of
    past events.

63
C. Childrens Understanding of Real and False
Emotions
  • Childrens understanding of the difference
    between real and false emotions improves
    considerably from age 3 to age 5.
  • Over the preschool and elementary school years,
    children develop a more refined understanding of
    emotional display rules a social groups
    informal norms about when, where, and how much
    one should show emotions and when and where
    displays of emotion should be suppressed or
    masked.
  • Cognitive development, social factors such as
    gender, and parents beliefs and behaviors all
    likely contribute to childrens understanding and
    use of display rules.

64
Facial display figures used in the assessment of
expression regulation
65
Display Rules
  • Children in 1st, 3rd, 5th, and 10th grade
    listened to stories designed to elicit display
    rules.
  • Then they were asked to predict and explain what
    the story protagonists would say and what facial
    expressions the protagonists would show in the
    emotion-laden situations.
  • Childrens knowledge of how and when to control
    emotional displays increased between grades 1-5
    and then leveled off.
  • Their understanding was greater for verbal
    display rules, whereby children monitor, falsify,
    and inhibit their speech, than for facial display
    rules
  • Children also understood prosocial display rules
    better than self-protective display rules.

66
Display Rules
Write a Comment
User Comments (0)
About PowerShow.com