Title: Why We Under Prepare for Hazards
1Why We Under Prepare for Hazards
Robert J. MeyerThe Wharton SchoolUniversity of
Pennsylvania
2An Eternal Problem Minimizing the Societal
Impact of Natural Disasters
- A modern dilemma advanced scientific knowledge
of the processes that generate natural disasters
and means to protect against them has done little
to reduce their damaging impact. - 2004 Tsunami (est. 224,000 dead) 2005 Hurricane
Katrina (100bn loss, 1300 dead) 2005 Earthquake
(Pakistan) 79,000 killed 1970 Cyclone, Bay of
Bengal 300,000 killed 1995 Kobe Earthquake
(Japan) 6,000 killed, 80bn loss.
3Why were these tragedies so bad?
- In almost all cases post-event analyses suggest
that the events need not have been as a damaging
as they were - Decision makers knew they were living in
risk-prone areas, knew what steps to take to
mitigate losses, and, often, could afford to
undertake them.
4Example New Orleans Close Call with Hurricane
Ivan, 2004
5Example
- September 13, 2004 Category-5 Hurricane Ivan is
near the West Coast of Cuba heading NW into the
Gulf, and 3 of 6 computer models predict a direct
hit on New Orleans in 3 days - Likely consequence catastrophe
6(No Transcript)
7September 14 Mayor Orders General Evacuation,
but discovers major flaws in evacuation system
Mayor Nagin said he would "aggressively
recommend" people evacuate, but that it would be
difficult to order them to, because at least
100,000 in the city rely on public transportation
and have no way to leave. Despite the potential
need for emergency housing, no shelters had been
opened in the city as of Tuesday night. Nagin
said the city was working on setting up a shelter
of "last resort" and added that the Superdome
might be used, but a spokesman for the stadium
said earlier Tuesday that it was not equipped as
a shelter.
8Good News
- Ivan spares New Orleans (coastal Alabamians and
Floridians not real happy, though). - New Orleans breathes sigh of relief
9Quiz
- If you were Ray Nagin, what should you have
learned from this close call? - a) That the city was fortunate to have averted a
catastrophe, hence immediate steps should be
taken to remedy the evacuation problems - b) The city is safe for another 40 years
- c) The city is inherently lucky
- d) What close call?
10One year later
11Two Months Later Wilma
- October 2005 Wilma becomes strongest hurricane
ever recorded in Atlantic basin, threatens South
Florida - South Floridians ordered to stock up (for the 4th
time that year) - Q What did residents learn from their own
earlier bout with Katrina and other storms?
12Apparently, very little
13(No Transcript)
14So why?
- Ultimately, decisions to undertake mitigation are
made by individuals for whom the best course of
personal action is highly uncertain - While one may be aware of aggregate risk, how
this translates to individual circumstances is
often ambiguous - There is inherent uncertainty about the
cost-effectiveness of mitigation investments,
which compete with other expenditures - The processes that allow us to make good
decisions in most walks of life fail when applied
to low-probability, high-consequence events
15The bottom line why we under-prepare
- We have limited abilities to recall the past,
have limited abilities to foresee the future, and
make mitigation decisions by imitating the
behavior of neighbors who are equally myopic
16Biases in learning from the past
- For most human endeavors, learning by
trial-and-error is an efficient way to develop
survival skills - The problem when TE processes are applied to
learning about mitigation in low-probability,
high-consequence, settings, it will lead us to
the wrong behaviors more often than the right
ones.
17The reasons
- One rarely sees positive benefits of investments
in mitigation (most experiences are false
alarms) - When hazards are encountered, the implications
they hold for optimal mitigation will tend to be
ambiguous
18Two major consequences
- Rapid extinguishing of normative mitigation
behaviors and - The prolonged persistence of superstitious
beliefs about mitigation
19Example Rapid forgetting and the Rebuilding of
Pass Christian, MS after Hurricane Camille
20Richelieu Apartments, Pass Christian,
Mississippi, August 1969
21Same Location after Hurricane Katrina (former
Pass Christian Shopping Center
22Example the flip side of recency learning too
much from recent disasters
23September 2005 Houston Braces for Hurricane Rita
24FEMA, State vow not to allow this to be another
Katrina
- Action 1.5 million Texans in Galveston/Houston
ordered to evacuate via staged plan
25Slight problem
- 2.8 million, not 1.5 million, try to leave.
- Takes up to 13 hours to drive 45 miles
- Problem exacerbated by broken down cars, need to
send relief supplies to people in cars - More die during evacuation than storm
26How observing past outcomes can be misleading
27The hurricane-proof Dome Home Pensacola Beach,
FL 2003
28The Dome Home after Ivan, September 2004
29(No Transcript)
30The Persistence of Mitigation Myths
31- A tornado is approaching your house. The best
way to prevent the house from suffering damage
is - Close all the doors and windows to create a tight
seal - Open a few windows to relieve pressure when the
funnel passes near or over - Neither of these actions will have any effect on
reducing damage
32- Opinions (95 Pennsylvanians)
- Close all the doors and windows (15)
- Open a few windows (55)
- Neither of these actions will have any effect on
reducing damage (30)
33Hurricanes in the Lab
34The Hurricane Simulation
- Respondents were endowed with a residence of
known value, and were paid at the end of the
simulation the difference between this endowment
and the cost of mitigation and storm repairs.
Mitigation measures do not improve the value of
the home--they only reduce storm losses. - At the start respondents are told their expected
length of tenure in the home and its location - Respondents could gather information about
hurricanes, mitigation, and make mitigation
purchases by clicking control buttons in the
simulation
35(No Transcript)
36(No Transcript)
37The Explanation
- In the absence of an unambiguous correct course
of action, mitigation decisions were driven by
short-run negative feedback - There was no evidence of learning either from
observing the misfortunes of others or close-call
encountersthe damage had to be real - In time lag effects vanished, but investments
remained well below optimum.
38Biases in seeing into the future
- As bad as we are at learning from the past, we
seem to be worse at accurately anticipating the
future consequences of current behaviors
39Key biases
- Projection bias we have a hard time envisioning
future hedonic states that are different from the
one we are in - Optimism Bias we are prone to imagine the are
prone to the best rather than worst-case
scenarios, causing errors in protective planning
40Examples New Orleans post 2004 Hurricane
planning, failure to evacuate in the face of
Hurricane Katrina
41Optimistic Planning and the 1935 Labor Day
Hurricane
42September 2,1935 (Labor Day)
- 675 WWI vets are in make-shift camps in the Fla
Keys, working to build a highway to Key
West - 7 AM Weather Bureau warns there is a CHANCE that
a hurricane MIGHT affect the area that night or
early Tuesdaybut it looks to be heading to Cuba
43The decision
- The only way to evacuate the Vets is by a train
from Miami - No train had been scheduled because of the
holiday a special one would have to be ordered. - the FERA supervisor in Jacksonville must decide
whether and when to order an evacuation
44The Decision
- The calculation it usually takes 2.5 hours to
ready a train and reach the camps - Hence, no need for an immediate evacuation if
the threat looks real come noon/early afternoon,
send the train (better be safe than sorry).
45What happened
- 130 PM Weather service revises forecastgales
to begin soon, hurricane conditions late that
night - 2PM Evacuation Train ordered
- Problem Cars are in Miami, Engine in Homestead
- Engine is Pointed in the Wrong Direction
- Train does not leave Homestead until 5PM
465 PM
477PM
488PM no further progress
4910 PM Landfall Long Key 200 mph 26.35
50Morning 452 Dead 279 VFW Camp Workers
51Biases in leaning from Others
- Given the tremendous uncertainty that surrounds
mitigation decisions, many homeowners tend to
make decisions by imitating the decisions of
others or following social norms - The problem, of course, is that such a heuristic
works only if the norms are rational
52Example the Wharton Earthquake Simulations
53Procedure
- Participants played a series of real-time games
in which they lived with other players in a
hypothetical country prone to earthquakes. - They could make investments in permanent
improvements that reduced damage from quakes - They were paid based on the initial value of
their home plus earnings minus earthquake damage
and mitigation investments
54The Screen Layout
55The Manipulations
- For half of all communities mitigation was
ineffective (optimal investment0), for half it
was highly effective (optimal100) - Ss played 3 blocks of 10-minute games
- After 1 warm up game, 1 player in each community
was secretly informed of the true effectiveness.
Other players knew that the community had an
informed player, but his/her identity was not
revealed
56(No Transcript)
57(No Transcript)
58Informed player is told that mitigation is highly
effective
59Informed player is told that mitigation is highly
effective
60(No Transcript)
61So why dont we prepare?
- As human decision makers we have evolved to be
quite skilled at learning quickly from frequent,
unambiguous, feedback, and planning for the short
term - Problem effective mitigation decisions requires
skills that are just the opposite to that for
example, a willingness to persistently invest in
costly actions that do not have an observable
positive payoff
62Solutions the obvious
- Legislation policies need to be put into place
that protect policy makers and residents from
themselves e.g. through building codes,
long-term commitments to funding, required
hazard-response plans - Education residents need to be taught not just
about hazard risks, but also trained to be better
long-term decision makers
63Solutions, the less obvious
- Problem forming effective legislation and
education programs requires us to know much more
than we currently do about human decision making
in low-probability, high-consequence settings.
While we know much about the physical science of
hazards, we know much less about the associated
psychological science. Bridging this gap should
be a major goal of research funding in the
natural hazards area in the years to come.