Title: Chapter 14: The Cutaneous Senses
1Chapter 14 The Cutaneous Senses
2Somatosensory System
- There are three parts
- Cutaneous senses - perception of touch and pain
from stimulation of the skin - Proprioception - ability to sense position of the
body and limbs - Kinesthesis - ability to sense movement of body
and limbs
3Mechanoreceptors
- Two types located close to surface of the skin
- Merkel receptor fires continuously while stimulus
is present. - Responsible for sensing fine details
- Meissner corpuscle fires only when a stimulus is
first applied and when it is removed. - Responsible for controlling hand-grip
4- Figure 14.1 A cross section of glabrous (without
hairs or projections) skin, showing the layers of
the skin and the structure, firing properties and
perceptions associated with the Merkel receptor
and Meissner corpuscle - two mechanoreceptors
that are near the surface of the skin.
5Mechanoreceptors - continued
- Two types located deeper in the skin
- Ruffini cylinder fires continuously to
stimulation - Associated with perceiving stretching of the skin
- Pacinian corpuscle fires only when a stimulus is
first applied and when it is removed. - Associated with sensing rapid vibrations and fine
texture
6- Figure 14.2 A cross section of glabrous skin,
showing the structure, firing properties and
perceptions associated with the Ruffini cylinder
and the Pacinian corpuscle - two mechanoreceptors
that are deeper in the skin.
7Pathways from Skin to Cortex
- Nerve fibers travel in bundles (peripheral
nerves) to the spinal cord. - Two major pathways in the spinal cord
- Medial lemniscal pathway consists of large fibers
that carry proprioceptive and touch information. - Spinothalamic pathway consists of smaller fibers
that carry temperature and pain information.
8Maps of the Body on the Cortex
- Body map (homunculus) on the cortex in S1 and S2
shows more cortical space allocated to parts of
the body that are responsible for detail. - Plasticity in neural functioning leads to
multiple homunculi and changes in how cortical
cells are allocated to body parts.
9- Figure 14.4 (a) The sensory homunculus on the
somatosensory cortex. Parts of the body with the
highest tactile acuity are represented by larger
areas on the cortex. (b) The somatosensory cortex
in the parietal lobe. The primary somatosensory
area, S1 (light shading), receives inputs from
the ventrolateral nucleus of the thalamus. The
secondary somatosensory area, S2 (dark shading),
is partially hidden behind the temporal lobe.
(Adapted from penfield Rasmussen, 1950).
10(No Transcript)
11- Figure 14.5 (a) Each numbered zone represents the
area in the somatosensory cortex that represents
one of the monkeys five fingers. The shaded area
on the zone for finger 2 is the part of the
cortex that represents the small area on the tip
of the finger shown in (b). (c) The shaded region
shows how the area representing the fingertip
increased in size after this area was heavily
stimulated over a 2-month period. (From Merzenich
et al., 1988)
12Perceiving Details
- Measuring tactile acuity
- Two-point threshold - minimum separation needed
between two points to perceive them as two units - Grating acuity - placing a grooved stimulus on
the skin and asking the participant to indicate
the orientation of the grating - Raised pattern identification - using such
patterns to determine the smallest size that can
be identified
13- Figure 14.7 Methods for determining tactile
acuity (a) two-point threshold (b) grating
acuity.
14Receptor Mechanisms for Tactile Acuity
- There is a high density of Merkel receptors in
the fingertips. - Merkel receptors are densely packed on the
fingertips - similar to cones in the fovea. - Both two-point thresholds and grating acuity
studies show these results.
15Cortical Mechanisms for Tactile Acuity
- Body areas with high acuity have larger areas of
cortical tissue devoted to them. - This parallels the magnification factor seen in
the visual cortex for the cones in the fovea. - Areas with higher acuity also have smaller
receptive fields on the skin.
16- Figure 14.10 Two-point thresholds for males.
Two-point thresholds for females follow the same
pattern. (From S. Weinstein, 1968.)
17- Figure 14.11 Receptive fields of monkey cortical
neurons that fire (a) when the fingers are
stimulated (b) when the hand is stimulated and
(c) when the arm is stimulated. (d) Stimulation
of two nearby points on the finger causes
separated activation on the finger area of the
cortex, but stimulation of two nearby points on
the arm causes overlapping activation in the arm
area of the cortex. (From Kandel Jessell, 1991
(a-c).
18Perceiving Vibration
- Pacinian corpuscle (PC) is primarily responsible
for sensing vibration. - Nerve fibers associated with PCs respond best to
high rates of vibration. - The structure of the PC is responsible for the
response to vibration - fibers without the PC
only respond to continuous pressure.
19- Figure 14.12 (a) When a vibrating pressure
stimulus is applied to the Pacinian corpuscle, it
transmits these pressure vibrations to the nerve
fiber. (b) When a continuous pressure stimulus is
applied to the Pacinian corpuscle, it does not
transmit the continuous pressure to the fiber.
(c) Lowenstein determined how the fiber fired to
stimulation of the corpuscle (at A), and to
direct stimulation of the fiber (at B) (Adapted
from Lowenstein, 1960)
20Perceiving Texture
- Katz (1925) proposed that perception of texture
depends on two cues - Spatial cues are determined by the size, shape,
and distribution of surface elements. - Temporal cues are determined by the rate of
vibration as skin is moved across finely textured
surfaces. - Two receptors may be responsible for this process
- called the duplex theory of texture perception
21Perceiving Texture - continued
- Past research showed support for the role of
spatial cues. - Recent research by Hollins and Reisner shows
support for the role of temporal cues. - In order to detect differences between fine
textures, participants needed to move their
fingers across the surface.
22- Figure 14.13 (a) Participants in Hollins and
Reisners (2000) experiment perceived the
roughness of two fine surfaces to be essentially
the same when felt with stationary fingers, but
(b) could perceive the difference between the two
surfaces when they were allowed to move their
fingers.
23- Figure 14.16 (a) Response of fibers in the
fingertips to touching a high-curvature stimulus.
The height of the profile indicates the firing
rate at different places across the fingertip.
(b) The profile of firing to touching a stimulus
with more gentle curvature. (From Goodwin, 1998)
24The Physiology of Tactile Object Perception -
continued
- Monkeys somatosensory cortex also shows neurons
that respond best to - grasping specific objects.
- paying attention to the task.
- Neurons may respond to stimulation of the
receptors, but attending to the task increases
the response.
25- Figure 14.18 Receptive fields of neurons in the
monkeys somatosensory cortex. (a) This neuron
responds best when a horizontally oriented edge
is presented to the monkeys hand. (b) This
neuron responds best when a stimulus moves across
the fingertip from right to left. (From Hyvarinin
Poranen, 1978)
26- Figure 14.19 The response of a neuron in a
monkeys parietal cortex that fires when the
monkey grasps a ruler but that does not fire when
the monkey grasps a cylinder. The monkey grasps
the objects at time 0. (From Sakata Iwamura,
1978)
27- Figure 14.20 Firing rate of a neuron in area S1
of a monkeys cortex to a letter being rolled
across the fingertips. The neuron responds only
when the monkey is paying attention to the
tactile stimulus. (From Hsiao, OShaughnessy,
Johnson, 1993)
28Pain Perception
- Pain is a multimodal phenomenon containing a
sensory component and an affective or emotional
component. - Three types of pain
- Nociceptive - signals impending damage to the
skin - Types of nociceptors respond to heat, chemicals,
severe pressure, and cold. - Threshold of eliciting receptor response must be
balanced to warn of damage, but not be affected
by normal activity.
29Types of Pain
- Inflammatory pain - caused by damage to tissues
and joints or by tumor cells - Neuropathic pain - caused by damage to the
central nervous system, such as - Brain damage caused by stroke
- Repetitive movements which cause conditions like
carpal tunnel syndrome
30- Figure 14.21 Nociceptive pain is created by
activation of nociceptors in the skin that
respond to different types of stimulation.
Signals from the nociceptors are transmitted to
the spinal cord and then from the dorsal root of
the spinal cord in pathways that lead to the
brain.
31Direct Pathway Model of Pain Perception
- Early model that stated nociceptors are
stimulated and send signals to the brain - Problems with this model
- Pain can be affected by a persons mental state.
- Pain can occur when there is no stimulation of
the skin. - Pain can be affected by a persons attention.
32Gate Control Model of Pain Perception
- The gate consists of substantia gelatinosa
cells in the spinal cord (SG- and SG). - Input into the gate comes from
- Large diameter (L) fibers - information from
tactile stimuli - Small diameter (S) fibers - information from
nociceptors - Central control - information from cognitive
factors from the cortex
33Gate Control Model of Pain Perception - continued
- Pain does not occur when the gate is closed by
stimulation into the SG- from central control or
L-fibers into the T-cell. - Pain does occur from stimulation from the
S-fibers into the SG into the T-cell. - Actual mechanism is more complex than this model
suggests.
34Cognitive and Experiential Aspects of Pain
- Expectation - when surgical patients are told
what to expect, they request less pain medication
and leave the hospital earlier - Placebos can also be effective in reducing pain.
- Shifting attention - virtual reality technology
has been used to keep patients attention on
other stimuli than the pain-inducing stimulation
35Cognitive and Experiential Aspects of Pain -
continued
- Content of emotional distraction - participants
could keep their hands in cold water longer when
pictures they were shown were positive - Experiment by Derbyshire to investigate
hypnotically induced pain. - Participants had a thermal stimulator attached
the to palm of their hand.
36Experiment by Derbyshire et al. - continued
- Three conditions
- Physically induced pain
- Hypnotically induced pain
- Control group that imagined painful stimulation
- Both subjective reports and fMRI scans showed
that hypnosis did produce pain perception.
37- Figure 14.24 The results of deWied and Verbatens
(2001) experiment showing that participants kept
their hands in cold water longer when looking at
positive pictures than when looking at neutral or
negative pictures.
38Opioids and Pain
- Brain tissue releases neurotransmitters called
endorphins. - Evidence shows that endorphins reduce pain.
- Injecting naloxone blocks the receptor sites
causing more pain. - Naloxone also decreases the effectiveness of
placebos. - People whose brains release more endorphins can
withstand higher pain levels.
39- Figure 14.28 (a) Naloxone reduces the effect of
heroin by occupying a receptor site normally
stimulated by heroin. (b) Stimulating sites in
the brain that cause the release of endorphins
can reduce the pain by stimulating opiate
receptor sites. (c) Naloxone decreases the pain
reduction caused by endorphins, by keeping the
endorphins from reaching the receptor sites.
40Pain in Social Situations
- Experiment by Eisenberger et al.
- Participants watched a computer game.
- Then, they were asked to play with two other
players who did not exist but were part of the
program. - The players excluded the participant.
- fMRI data showed increased activity in the
anterior cingulate cortex and participants
reported feeling ignored and distressed.
41Pain in Social Situations - continued
- Experiment by Singer et al.
- Romantically involved couples participated.
- The womans brain activity was measured by fMRI.
- The woman either received shocks or she watched
while her partner received shocks. - Similar brain areas were activated in both
conditions.