Title: Interlanguage
1- Interlanguage
- Interlanguage developed as a concept in Error
Analysis. - While Error Analysis has fallen out of favor,
studies of Interlanguage have not. Why? - Because Interlanguage can be studied empirically.
- Three areas of Interlanguage inquiry
- Systematicity / Variation
- Development sequences
- L1 influence
1
2- Interlanguage Systematicity
- Recall that an interlanguage is the language
system that the learner constructs out of the
linguistic data to which s/he has been exposed
(LFL, p. 60). - Many studies of consider the system of
interlanguages. While interlanguage may be
non-target-like, it may nevertheless be
systematic that is, rule-governed at any
particular moment. - Many scholars investigate the systematicity that
may underlie non-target-like interlanguages. - May underlie other non-target-like features
of interlanguage may be nonsystematic variation.
3- Interlanguage Systematicity
- Huebner, T. (1983). Linguistic systems and
linguistic change in an interlanguage. Studies in
Second Language Acquisition, 6(1), 3353. - Case study of the English IL development of a
Hmong man in Hawaii. 17 hours of recorded speech
examined, with attention given to two structures,
among them his use of da, something like the
English definite article the. - Yu tol da Jaepanii gow da cher.
- You told the Japanese woman that you were going
to the church. - Da used in 64 percent of NP environments
appropriate to the definite article.
4- Interlanguage Systematicity
- Da used in 64 percent of NP environments
appropriate to the definite article. - But what is the appropriate NP environment for
definite articles? What is the rule for the use
of the definite (the) and indefinite article
(a/n) in English? - Son 1 Dad gave me a car.
- Son 2 Not the car?Â
- Son 1 No, a Taurus. or
- Son 1 No, the Taurus.
- What is Son 2 referring to?Â
- What difference does the pair of replies given by
Son 1 indicate?
5- Interlanguage Systematicity
- What is the appropriate NP environment for
definite articles? - Noun phrases may refer to something specific
(specific referent, SR) and / or to something
known to the hearer (HK) or not (-SR -HK).
6- Interlanguage Systematicity
Quadrant 1 The lion is a noble animal (-SR,
HK). Quadrant 4 Used in equative and other
structures (-SR, -HK). Quadrant 3 Dad gave
me a Taurus (SR, -HK). A refers to a specific
Taurus, but not one known to the hearer.
7- Interlanguage Systematicity
Quadrant 3 Dad gave me a Taurus (SR, -HK). A
refers to a specific Taurus, but not one known to
the hearer. Quad 2 Dad gave me the Taurus.
(SR, HK) The refers to a specific Taurus
known to the hearer.
8- Interlanguage Systematicity
- Da used in 64 percent of correct NP
environments. - In most of the other 36 percent of NP
environments that required the definite article,
the NP was the topic of the sentence topic
whatever the sentence is about. The tree is
magnificent. - Huebner theorized that his informant had
developed an IL rule for the use of his definite
article as follows use da in NP environments
characterized by SR, HK, -Topic.
9- Interlanguage Systematicity
- use da in NP environments characterized by SR,
HK, -Topic. - Logic If it is the topic of the sentence, it is
known to the hearer by default, and therefore
doesnt need a definite article perhaps this is
a kind of simplification rule, to eliminate
redundant features / structures. If the NP is
not the topic, Huebners informant may have
theorized, it does need the definite article. - Conclusion IL systematic, if non-target-like.
10- IL Development Sequences, First Lge Learning
- First language development Brown, Roger.
(1973) A first language. Cambridge Harvard Univ.
Press. - HLAL, p. 3 Case study 4 years (diachronic /
longitudinal) 3 children from age 2 samples
every 1-2 weeks. Acquisition of 14 morphemes
- 1. -ing
- 3 on
- 3 in
- plural s
- past irregular
- possessive s
- uncontracted copula (I am)
- articles a the
- past regular
- 3rd person regular s
- 3rd person irregular has
- uncontracted aux (he is eating)
- contracted copula (Im)
- contracted aux (hes coming)
11- IL Development Sequences, First Lge Learning
- 0.86, 0.87, and 0.89 correlations among three
children for order of acquisition of 14
morphemes. - de Villiers, J., de Villiers, P. (1973). A
cross-sectional study of the acquisition of
grammatical morphemes. Journal of
Psycholinguistic Research 2(3), 267278. - Synchronic / cross sectional study 21 children
ages 16 40 months, two 1.5-hour sessions. - Three methods of analysis showed 0.78, 0.84, and
0.87 correlations among children for order of
acquisition of same 14 morphemes.
12- IL Development Sequences, First Lge Learning
- Structures most frequently produced in childs
environment not necessarily learned earlier. - Positive reinforcement had no impact on
acquisition of morphemes. - What can we conclude, epistemologically, from
these findings? I.e., what process explains
first language acquisition? - Language learning is not a behavioral process of
habit formation, but is driven by something else.
12
13- IL Development Sequences, SLA
- Very productive area of inquiry in SLA
- Methodological variations
- Longitudinal / diachronic (like Brown) small
number of subjects studied over a period of time
v. cross-sectional / synchronic (like de Villiers
de Villiers) large number of subjects at a
specific time. - Demographic variations
- children v. adult SLA
- Language variations
- Meisel, J.M., Clahsen, H., Pienemann, J.
(1981). On determining developmental stages in
natural second language acquisition. SSLA, 3,
109135.
14- IL Development Sequences, SLA
- Development stages variations (HLAL pp. 8293)
- Development sequences for
- Interrogative formation
- Negation formation
- Relative clause formation
- Morphemes (Dulay and Burt)
- Pragmatics (Rose)
15- IL Development Sequences, Dulay Burt
- Literature Review (pp. 3738) 2 pages!
- Error analysis reveals creative construction,
p. 37 children gradually construct rules for
speech they hear, guided by universal innate
mechanisms which cause them to formulate certain
types of hypotheses about the language system
being acquired, until the mismatch between what
they are exposed to and what they produce is
resolved. - The second episode, p. 38. Whats this?
- Their study of English morpheme acquisition by
three different groups of Spanish speakers. - What limits their conclusions, methodologically?
- Results may be driven by common first language.
16- IL Development Sequences, Dulay Burt
- Methods of Data Collection
- longitudinal or cross-sectional?
- who are subjects? how does this answer the
constraint of episode 2? - how did the researchers find subjects?
- how did they collect data?
- what is the Bilingual Syntax Measure (BSM) (pp.
3940)
17- IL Development Sequences, Dulay Burt
- Methods of Data Collection
- what morphemes (functors) did they study?
18- IL Development Sequences, Dulay Burt
- Methods of Data Analysis
- how was the data analyzed / coded?
- obligatory occasions
- 0 points if no morpheme supplied in O.O.
- She is dance___
- 1 point if morpheme is supplied, but it is
incorrect - She is dances
- 2 points if correct morpheme is supplied
- She is dancing.
19- IL Development Sequences, Dulay Burt
- Methods of Data Analysis
- group individual scores morpheme X summed
- total possible obligatory occasions
20- IL Development Sequences, Dulay Burt
- Results
21- IL Development Sequences, Dulay Burt
- Results
- What do the declining lines mean re the
acquisition of specific morphemes?
What do they mean relative to one another? i.e.
to the Spanish and Chinese language groups?
Note correlations, p. 50 0.95 / 0.96 What do
these mean?
22- IL Development Sequences, Dulay Burt
- Related Studies
23- IL Development Sequences, Dulay Burt
- Related Studies
24- IL Development Sequences, Dulay Burt
- Related Studies
What conclusions can we draw?
25- IL Development Sequences, Dulay Burt
- Conclusion
- Speakers of numerous native languages (Spanish,
Cantonese, Greek, Persian, Italian, etc.), at
various ages, acquire English morphemes in
remarkably similar orders. - These studies provide a strong indication that
universal cognitive mechanisms are the basis for
the childs organization of a target language,
and that it is the L2 system, rather than the L1
system that guides the acquisition process (p.
52).
26- IL Development Sequences, Dulay Burt
- Critique
- Meisel, Clahsen, Pienemann (1981) challenge
Dulay and Burt. By focusing exclusively on
target language structures (she is dancing), DB
ignore systematic non-target-like grammars that
may exist in informants interlanguages.
27- Interlanguage First Language Influence
- MarkednessÂ
- Phenomenon A in some language is more marked than
phenomenon B if the presence of A implies the
presence of B, but the presence of B does not
imply the presence of A (implicational
relations). - Marked or unmarked?
- Sarah is a senior.
- Sarah is a student.
- If she is a senior, she must be a student, so
this proposition is marked. - If she is a student, then she may be a senior,
but we dont know, so this proposition is
unmarked.
28- Interlanguage First Language Influence
- MarkednessÂ
- Phenomenon A in some language is more marked than
phenomenon B if the presence of A implies the
presence of B, but the presence of B does not
imply the presence of A (implicational
relations). - Markedness Differential Hypothesis
- Those areas of the target language which
different from the native language and are more
marked than the native language will be
difficult. - Those areas of the target language which are
different from the native language, but are not
more marked than the native language, will not be
difficult.
29- Interlanguage First Language Influence
- MarkednessÂ
- English and French both have the phoneme / ž /.
- pleasure / jamais
- In English words, / ž / never occurs syllable
initial (distinguished from words borrowed from
French, like Jacques). - BUT (markedness studies show), the syllable
initial / ž / is not marked in English. Thus,
while the phonology of French is different than
English (relative to the distribution of the
phoneme / ž /), that particular phonology will
not be difficult for native English speakers to
learn.
30- Interlanguage First Language Influence
- MarkednessÂ
- Eckman, F. (1977). Markedness and the contrastive
analysis hypothesis. Language Learning, 27,
315-330.