Single session analysis using FEAT - PowerPoint PPT Presentation

1 / 100
About This Presentation
Title:

Single session analysis using FEAT

Description:

Single session analysis using FEAT David Field Thanks to . Tom Johnstone, Jason Gledhill, FMRIB Single session or first level FMRI analysis In FMRI you ... – PowerPoint PPT presentation

Number of Views:129
Avg rating:3.0/5.0
Slides: 101
Provided by: DavidT109
Category:

less

Transcript and Presenter's Notes

Title: Single session analysis using FEAT


1
Single session analysis using FEAT
  • David Field
  • Thanks to.
  • Tom Johnstone, Jason Gledhill, FMRIB

2
Single session or first level FMRI analysis
  • In FMRI you begin with 1000s of time courses,
    one for each voxel location
  • Lots of preprocessing is applied to maximise
    the quality of the data
  • Then, each voxel time course is modelled
    independently
  • the model is a set of regressors (EVs) that vary
    over time
  • the same model is applied to every voxel time
    course
  • if (part of) the model fits the voxel time course
    well the voxel is said to be active

241
256
250
234
260
242
254
3
Plan for today
  • Detailed look at the process of modelling a
    single voxel time course
  • The input is a voxel time course of image
    intensity values and the design matrix, which is
    made up of multiple EVs
  • The GLM is used to find the linear combination of
    EVs that explains the most variance in the voxel
    time course
  • The output is a PE for each EV in the design
    matrix
  • Preprocessing of voxel time courses
  • this topic probably only makes much sense if you
    understand 1) above, which is why I am breaking
    with tradition and covering this topic second
    rather than first
  • Implementing a single session analysis in FSL
    using FEAT (workshop)
  • Note There is no formal meeting in week 3 of the
    course, but the room will be open for you to
    complete worksheets from today and last week
  • at least one experienced FSL user will be here to
    help

4
Preprocessing.
5
(No Transcript)
6
  • The BET brain extraction option refers to the 4D
    functional series
  • This will not run BET on the structural
  • Normally turn this on
  • Brain extraction for the structural image that
    will be used as the target for registration has
    to be performed by you, before you use FEAT to
    set up the processing pipeline

7
  • On the Misc tab you can toggle balloon help
  • Balloon help will tell you what most of the
    options mean if you hover the mouse over a button
    or tickbox
  • But it gets annoying after a while
  • If progress watcher is selected then FEAT opens a
    web browser that shows regular updates of the
    stage your analysis has reached

8
Motion Correction MCFLIRT
  • Aims to make sure that there is a consistent
    mapping between voxel X,Y,Z position and actual
    anatomical locations
  • Each image in the 4D series is registered using a
    6 DOF rigid body spatial transform to the
    reference image (by default, the series mid
    point)
  • This means every image apart from the reference
    image is moved slightly
  • MCFLIRT plots the movements that were made as
    output

9
MCFLIRT output
Head rotation
Head translation
Total displacement
10
The total displacement plot
displacement is calculated relative to the volume
in the middle of the time series
Relative displacement is head position at each
time point relative to the previous time point.
Absolute displacement is relative to the
reference image.
11
The total displacement plot
Why should you be particularly concerned about
high values in the relative motion plot?
The first thing to do is look at the range of
values plotted on the y axis, because MCFLIRT
auto-scales the y axis to the data range
12
Slice timing correction
Each slice in a functional image is acquired
separately. Acquisition is normally interleaved,
which prevents blending of signal from adjacent
slices Assuming a TR of 3 seconds and, what is
the time difference between the acquisition of
adjacent slices?
A single functional brain area may span two or
more slices
13
Why are the different slice timings a problem?
  • The same temporal model (design matrix) is fitted
    to every voxel time course
  • therefore, the model assumes that all the voxel
    values in a single functional volume were
    measured simultaneously
  • Given that they are not measured simultaneously,
    what are the implications?
  • Consider two voxels from adjacent slices
  • both voxels are from the same functional brain
    area
  • this time the TR is 1.5, but the slice order
    differed from the standard interleaved procedure,
    so there is a 1 sec time gap between acquisition
    of two voxels in the adjacent slices that cover
    the functional brain area

14
  • Blue line real HRF in response to a brief event
    at time 0

Blue squares intensity values at a voxel first
sampled at 0.5 sec, then every 1.5 sec thereafter
(TR 1.5)
Red circles intensity values at a voxel from an
adjacent slice first sampled at 1.5 sec, then
every 1.5 sec thereafter (TR 1.5)
15
These are the two voxel time courses that are
submitted to the model
16
The model time course is yoked to the mid point
of the volume acquisition (TR), so there will be
a better fit for voxels in slices acquired at or
near that time.
These are the two voxel time courses that are
submitted to the model
17
Slice timing solutions
  • Any ideas based on what was covered earlier?
  • Including temporal derivatives of the main
    regressors in the model allows the model to be
    shifted in time to fit voxels in slices acquired
    far away from the mid point of the TR
  • But,
  • this makes it difficult to interpret the PEs for
    the derivatives do they represent slice timing,
    or do they represent variations in the underlying
    HRF?
  • and, you end up having to use F contrasts instead
    of t contrasts (see later for why this is to be
    avoided if possible)

18
Slice timing solutions
  • Any ideas based on what was covered earlier?
  • Use a block design with blocks long enough that
    the BOLD response is summated over a long time
    period
  • But,
  • Not all experimental stimuli / tasks can be
    presented in a block design
  • So,
  • another option is to shift the data from
    different slices in time by small amounts so that
    it is as if all slices were acquired at once
  • this is what the preprocessing option in FEAT
    does

19
Shifting the data in time - caveats
  • But if the TR is 3 seconds, and you need to move
    the data for a given slice by, e.g., 1 sec, you
    dont have a real data point to use
  • get round this by interpolating the missing time
    points to allow whole voxel time courses to be
    shifted in time so effectively they were all
    sampled at once at the middle of the TR
  • But
  • Interpolation works OK for short TR, but it does
    not work well for long TR (gt 3 sec?)
  • So this solution only works well when slice
    timing issues are relatively minor
  • There is a debate about whether to do slice
    timing correction before or after motion
    correction
  • FSL does motion correction first
  • some people advise against any slice timing
    correction
  • Generally, if you have an event related design
    then use it, but make sure you check carefully
    with the scanner technician what order your
    slices were acquired in!

20
Temporal filtering
  • Filtering in time and/or space is a
    long-established method in any signal detection
    process to help "clean up" your signal
  • The idea is if your signal and noise are present
    at separable frequencies in the data, you can
    attenuate the noise frequencies and thus increase
    your signal to noise ratio

21
(No Transcript)
22
AAAAAAAVVVVVVVRRRRRRRAAAAAAAVVVVVVVRRRRRRR
23
AAAAAAAVVVVVVVRRRRRRRAAAAAAAVVVVVVVRRRRRRR
Time
24
Raw data
25
After low pass filter
26
Very low frequency component, suggests that high
pass filter also needed
27
Low and high frequencies removed
28
Setting the high pass filter for FMRI
  • The rule recommended by FSL is that the lowest
    setting for the high pass filter should be equal
    to the duration of a single cycle of the design
  • In an ABCABCABC block design, it will be equal to
    ABC
  • If you set a shorter duration than this you will
    remove signal that is associated with the
    experiment
  • If you set a higher duration than this then any
    unsystematic variation (noise) in the data with a
    periodic structure lower in frequency than the
    experimental cycle time will remain in voxel time
    courses
  • In complicated event related designs lacking a
    strongly periodic structure there is a subjective
    element to the setting of the high pass filter

29
Setting the high pass filter for FMRI
  • Do not use experimental designs with many
    conditions where the duration of a single
    experimental cycle is very long
  • e.g. ABCDEABCDE, where ABCDE 300 seconds
  • setting the high pass to 300 sec will allow a lot
    of the low frequency FMRI noise to pass through
    to the modelling stage
  • Furthermore, the noise can easily become
    correlated with the experimental time course
    because you are using an experimental time course
    that has a similar frequency to that of FMRI
    noise
  • In any signal detection experiment, not just
    FMRI, you need to ensure that the signal of
    interest and noise are present at different
    temporal frequencies

30
Low pass filter?
  • As well as removing oscillations with a longer
    cycle time than the experiment, you can also
    elect to remove oscillations with a higher cycle
    time than the experiment
  • high frequency noise
  • In theory this should enhance signal and reduce
    noise, and it was practiced in the early days of
    FMRI
  • However, it has now been demonstrated that
    because FMRI noise has temporal structure (i.e.
    it is not white noise), the low pass filter can
    actually enhance noise relative to signal
  • The temporal structure in the noise is called
    temporal autocorrelation and is dealt with in
    FSL using FILM prewhitening instead of low pass
    filtering
  • in another break with the traditional structure
    of FMRI courses temporal autocorrelation and
    spatial smoothing will be covered after t and F
    contrasts

31
After preprocessing and model fitting
  • You can now begin to answer the questions you set
    out to answer.

32
Which voxels activated in each experimental
condition?
  • In the auditory / visual stimulus experiment, how
    do you decide if a voxel was more activated
    during the visual stimulus than the baseline?
  • If the visual condition PE is gt 0 then the voxel
    is active
  • but gt 0 has to take into account the noise in the
    data
  • We need to be confident that if you repeated the
    experiment many times the PE would nearly always
    be gt 0
  • How can you take the noise into account and
    quantify confidence that PE gt 0?
  • PE / residual variation in the voxel time course
  • this is a t statistic, which can be converted to
    a p value by taking into account the degrees of
    freedom
  • The p value is the probability of a PE as large
    or larger than the observed PE if the true value
    of the PE was 0 (null hypothesis) and the only
    variation present in the data was random
    variation
  • FSL converts t statistics to z scores,
    simplifying interpretation because z can be
    converted to p without using deg of freedom

33
Why include effects of no interest in the model?
  • Also called nuisance regressors
  • Imagine an experiment about visual processing of
    faces versus other classes of object
  • Why add EVs to the design matrix based on the
    time course of
  • head motion parameters
  • image intensity spikes
  • physiological variables
  • galvanic skin response, heart rate, pupil
    diameter
  • Answer to reduce the size of the residual error
    term
  • t will be bigger when PE is bigger, but t will
    also be bigger when error is smaller
  • You can model residual variation that is
    systematic in some way, but some of the residual
    variation is truly random in nature, e.g. thermal
    noise from the scanner, and cannot be modelled out

34
t contrasts
  • Contrast is short for Contrast of Parameter
    Estimates (COPE)
  • it means a linear sum of PEs. The simplest
    examples are implicit contrasts of individual
    PEs with baseline. Using the example from the
    interactive spreadsheet
  • visual PE 1 auditory PE 0
  • visual PE 0 auditory PE 1
  • To locate voxels where the visual PE is larger
    than the auditory PE
  • visual PE 1 auditory PE -1
  • To locate voxels where the auditory PE is larger
    than the visual PE
  • visual PE -1 auditory PE 1

35
t contrasts
  • The value of the contrast in each voxel is
    divided by an estimate of the residual variation
    in the voxel time course (standard error)
  • produces a t statistic of the contrast
  • residual variation is based on the raw time
    course values minus the predicted values from the
    fitted model
  • Activation maps (red and yellow blobs
    superimposed on anatomical images) are produced
    by mapping the t value at each voxel to a colour
  • crudely, thresholding is just setting a value of
    t below which no colour is assigned.

36
What will the 1 1 contrast give you?
37
F contrasts
  • These can be used to find voxels that are active
    in any one or more of a set of t contrasts
  • Visual Auditory Tactile
  • 1 -1 0
  • 1 0 -1
  • F contrasts are bidirectional (1 -1 also implies
    -1 1)
  • rarely a good thing in practice..
  • If the response to an event was modelled with the
    standard HRF regressor plus time derivative then
    you can use an F contrast to view both components
    of model of the response to the event on a single
    activation map
  • If you are relying on the time derivative to deal
    with slice timing correction then you are
    strongly advised to do this

38
Thresholding / multiple comparisons problem
39
Temporal autocorrelation
  • The image intensity value in a voxel at time X is
    partially predictable from the same voxel at
    times X-1, X-2, X-3 etc (even in baseline scans
    with no experiment)
  • why is this a problem?
  • it makes it hard to know the number of
    statistically independent observations in a voxel
    time course
  • life would be simple if the number of
    observations was equal to the number of
    functional volumes
  • temporal autocorrelation results in a true N is
    lower than this
  • Why do we need to know the number of independent
    observations?
  • because calculating a t statistic requires
    dividing by the standard error, and the standard
    error is SD / square root (N-1)
  • Degrees of freedom are needed to convert t stats
    to p values
  • If you use the number of time points in the voxel
    time course as N then p values will be too small
    (false positive)

40
Measuring autocorrelation
41
Measuring autocorrelation
42
Measuring autocorrelation
43
Measuring autocorrelation (SPSS style)
44
Plot the correlation against the degree of shift
45
Temporal autocorrelation
  • Generally, the value of a voxel at time t is
    partially predictable from nearby time points
    about 3-6 seconds in the past
  • So, if you use a very long TR, e.g. 6, then you
    mostly avoid the problem as the original time
    points will have sufficient independence from
    each other
  • Voxel values are also predictable from more
    distant time points due to low frequency noise
    with periodic structure
  • but the high pass filter should deal with this
    problem

46
Autocorrelation FILM prewhitening
  • First, fit the model (regressors of interest and
    no interest) to the voxel time course using the
    GLM
  • (ignoring the autocorrelation for the moment)
  • Estimate the temporal autocorrelation structure
    in the residuals
  • note if model is good residuals noise?
  • The estimated structure can be inverted and used
    as a temporal filter to undo the autocorrelation
    structure in the original data
  • the time points are now independent and so N
    the number of time points (volumes)
  • the filter is also applied to the design matrix
  • Refit the GLM
  • Run t and F tests with valid standard error and
    degrees of freedom
  • Prewhitening is selected on the stats tab in FEAT
  • it is computationally intensive, but with a
    modern PC it is manageable and there are almost
    no circumstances where you would turn this option
    off

47
Spatial smoothing
  • FMRI noise varies across space as well as time
  • smoothing is a way of reducing spatial noise and
    thereby increasing the ratio of signal to noise
    (SNR) in the data
  • Unlike FMRI temporal noise, FMRI spatial noise is
    more like white noise, making it easier to deal
    with
  • it is essentially random, essentially independent
    from voxel to voxel, and has as mean of about
    zero
  • therefore if you average image intensity across
    several voxels, noise tends to average towards
    zero, whereas signal that is common to the voxels
    you are averaging across will remain unchanged,
    dramatically improving the signal to noise ratio
    (SNR)
  • A secondary benefit of smoothing is to reduce
    anatomical variation between participants that
    remains after registration to the template image
  • this is because smoothing blurs the images

48
Spatial smoothing FWHM
  • FSL asks you to specify a Gaussian smoothing
    kernel defined by its Full Width at Half Maximum
    (FWHM) in mm
  • To find the FWHM of a Gaussian
  • Find the point on the y axis where the function
    attains half its maximum value
  • Then read off the corresponding x axis values

49
Spatial smoothing FWHM
  • The Gaussian is centred on a voxel, and the value
    of the voxel is averaged with that of adjacent
    voxels that fall under the Gaussian
  • The averaging is weighted by the y axis value of
    the Gaussian at the appropriate distance

50
No smoothing
4 mm
9 mm
51
Effects of Smoothing on activations
Unsmoothed Data
Smoothed Data (kernel width 5 voxels)
52
When should you smooth? When should you not?
  • Smoothing is a good idea if
  • You're not particularly concerned with
    voxel-by-voxel resolution.
  • You're not particularly concerned with finding
    small (less than a handful of voxels) clusters
  • You want (or need) to improve your
    signal-to-noise ratio
  • You're averaging results over a group, in a brain
    region where functional anatomy and organization
    isn't precisely known
  • You want to use p-values corrected for multiple
    comparisons with Gaussian field theory (as
    opposed to False Discovery Rate)
  • this is the Voxel option in FSL and the FWE
    option in SPM
  • Smoothing kernel should be small (or no
    smoothing) if
  • You need voxel-by-voxel resolution
  • You believe your activations of interest will
    only be a few voxels in size
  • You're confident your task will generate large
    amounts of signal relative to noise
  • You're working primarily with single-subject
    results
  • You're mainly interested in getting
    region-of-interest data from very specific
    structures that you've drawn with high resolution
    on single subjects

53
How do you determine the size of the kernel?
  • Based on functional voxel size? Or brain
    structure size?
  • A little of both, it seems.
  • The matched filter theorem, from the signal
    processing field, tells us that if we're trying
    to recover a signal (like an activation) in noisy
    data (like FMRI), we can best do it by smoothing
    our data with a kernel that's about the same size
    as our activation.
  • Trouble is, though, most of us don't know how big
    our activations are going to be before we run our
    experiment
  • Even if you have a particular structure of
    interest (say, the hippocampus), you may not get
    activation over the whole region - only a part
  • A lot of people set FWHM to functional voxel size
    2

54
(No Transcript)
55
Old slides beyond this point
56
Slice timing correction
  • Each functional volume that forms part of the 4D
    time series is made up of slices
  • Each slice is acquired at a different point in
    time relative to the start of the TR
  • e.g., slice 1 at 100 msec, slice 2 at 200 msec,
    etc
  • For each slice, its the same time point relative
    to the start of the TR in every volume
  • So, the interval between successive acquisitions
    is constant for every voxel
  • But the actual time of acquisition is different
    for every slice
  • The model of the time course assumes that within
    each volume, every slice was acquired
    simultaneously at the mid point of the TR
  • so, the model is likely to fit better for one
    slice than all the others (bad)
  • To use slice timing correction, you will need to
    tell FSL the order your slices were acquired in
  • interleaved is the most common, but ask your
    scanner technician!
  • Adjustment is to the middle of the TR period

57
Slice timing correction
  • For each voxel, slice-timing correction examines
    the time course and shifts it by a small amount
  • This requires interpolating between the time
    points you actually sampled to infer a more
    detailed version of the time course
  • The more detailed time course can have small
    shifts applied to it that are slightly different
    for each voxel, depending on the actual order the
    slices were acquired in
  • This allows you to make the assumption in your
    modelling that every voxel in each volume was
    acquired simultaneously

58
Slice timing correction
  • The problem this tries to solve is more severe if
    you have a longer TR (e.g. 4 seconds)
  • two adjacent slices in an interleaved sequence
    could be sampled almost 2 seconds apart
  • But temporal interpolation also becomes dodgy
    with longer TRs ?
  • For block designs (stimuli that are long relative
    to the TR, e.g. TR 2 sec, stimulus lasts 16
    sec) slice timing errors are not a significant
    factor influencing the fitting of a model to the
    data
  • For event related designs (brief stimuli
    separated by variable pauses), slice timing
    correction is important
  • People argue about whether to do slice timing
    correction before or after motion correction
  • FSL does motion correction first
  • some people advise against any slice timing
    correction

59
Temporal derivatives
  • In the FEAT practical you will add temporal
    derivatives of the HRF convolved experimental
    time courses to the design matrix
  • what is the purpose of this?
  • Each experimental time course is convolved with a
    model of the HRF
  • this is to build the delay and blurring of the
    blood flow response relative to the neural
    response into the model
  • but the delay varies between brain areas and
    between people

60
Temporal Derivatives
  • The green line is the first temporal derivative
    of the blue line
  • its rate of change
  • the positive max of the derivative is earlier
    than the normal HRF peak
  • the negative max of the derivative is later than
    the normal HRF peak
  • If fitting the model results in a positive beta
    weight on a derivative this implies that the HRF
    peak is earlier in that voxel
  • A negative beta weight for the derivative implies
    a later peak than typical

61
Temporal derivatives
  • The basic HRF shape (blue on the previous slide)
    has some physiological underpinning (in visual
    cortex)
  • But the use of the derivative to model faster /
    slower responses is just a mathematical
    convenience
  • The second temporal derivative (dispersion in
    time) can be used to model haemodynamic responses
    that are thinner or fatter in time than the
    basic shape
  • The three functions together are sometimes called
    the informed basis set by SPM users
  • the blue line is referred to as canonical, but
    in fact it is only canonical for primary visual
    cortex
  • The informed basis set can only model slight
    departures from the canonical response shape
  • If you are interested in the prefrontal cortex of
    the elderly youll need to use a more flexible
    basis set to model the temporal dynamics of the
    response
  • or use a block design where timing issues are
    less severe

62
(No Transcript)
63
(No Transcript)
64
(No Transcript)
65
(No Transcript)
66
Cluster size based thresholding
  • Intuitively, if a voxel with a Z statistic of
    1.96 for a particular COPE is surrounded by other
    voxels with very low Z values this looks
    suspicious
  • unless you are looking for a very small brain
    area
  • Consider a voxel with a Z statistic of 1.96 is
    surrounded by many other voxels with similar Z
    values, forming a large blob
  • Intuitively, for such a voxel the Z of 1.96 (p
    0.05) is an overestimate of the probability of
    the model fit to this voxel being a result of
    random, stimulus unrelated, fluctuation in the
    time course
  • The p value we want to calculate is the
    probability of obtaining one or more clusters of
    this size or larger under a suitable null
    hypothesis
  • one or more gives us control over the multiple
    comparisons problem by setting the family wise
    error rate
  • p value will be low for big clusters
  • p value will be high for small clusters

67
Comparison of voxel (height based) thresholding
and cluster thresholding
?
space
Significant Voxels
No significant Voxels
? is the height threshold, e.g. 0.001 applied
voxelwise (will be Z about 3)
68
Comparison of voxel (height based) thresholding
and cluster thresholding
?
space
Cluster significant
Cluster not significant
k?
k?
K? is the probability of the image containing 1
or more blobs with k or more voxels (and you can
control is at 0.05) The cluster size, in voxels,
that corresponds to a particular value of K?
depends upon the initial value of height
threshold ? used to define the number of clusters
in the image and their size It is usual to set
height ? quite low when using cluster level
thresholding, but this arbitrary choice will
influence the outcome
69
Dependency of number of clusters on choice of
height threshold
The number and size of clusters also depends upon
the amount of smoothing that took place in
preprocessing
70
(No Transcript)
71
  • Nyquist frequency is important to know about
  • Half the sampling rate (e.g. TR 2 sec is 0.5 Hz,
    so Nyquist is 0.25 hz, or 4 seconds)
  • No signal higher frequency than Nyquist can be
    present in the data (important for experimental
    design)
  • But such signal could appear as an aliasing
    artefact at a lower frequency

72
(No Transcript)
73
Overview
  • Todays practical session will cover processing
    of a single functional session from a single
    participant using FEAT
  • FEAT is an umbrella program that brings together
    various other FSL programs into a customisable
    processing pipeline
  • for example, it makes use of BET and FLIRT, which
    were programs covered in week 1
  • Definitions of single session and multi
    session
  • We will also make use of an interactive
    spreadsheet that demonstrates how the general
    linear model (GLM) can be used to locate active
    regions of the brain given your predictions about
    the time course of activation
  • The lecture will provide theoretical background
    for each processing step
  • There is no formal meeting in week 3 of the
    course, but the room will be open for you to
    complete worksheets from today and last week
  • at least one experienced FSL user will be here to
    help

74
Overview of single session FMRI
  • The data is a 4D functional time series
  • Many thousands of spatial locations (voxels)
  • Each voxel has a time course defined by a single
    intensity value per TR ( per volume acquired)
  • The task is to model the changes in image
    intensity over time separately for each voxel
  • mass univariate approach
  • begin with a set of regressors (design matrix /
    model)
  • regressors usually reflect the time course of
    experimental conditions
  • find the best linear combination of regressors to
    explain each voxel time course (basically,
    multiple regression)
  • Before modelling the 4D time series a number of
    preprocessing steps are applied to the data
  • remove unwanted sources of variation from the
    time series
  • increase the signal to noise ratio

75
Voxel-wise single session modelling
  • After the data has been optimised by
    preprocessing you search for voxels where the
    time course of image intensity changes is
    correlated with the experimental time course
  • activation
  • This is achieved using the General Linear Model
    (GLM)
  • similar to multiple regression
  • The input to the GLM is the data, plus a set of
    explanatory variables called the Design Matrix
  • sometimes EVs are included to model sources of
    variation that are of no interest to the
    experimenter
  • this is to reduce the residual (error) variance
  • The GLM is fitted independently for each voxel
    timecourse
  • ignores the spatial structure in the brain

76
(No Transcript)
77
Regressor 0 (stimulus on)
Regressor 1 (stimulus off)
78
(No Transcript)
79
(No Transcript)
80
(No Transcript)
81
(No Transcript)
82
The voxel time courses are standardised so that
beta weights are comparable between voxels
83
If there is structure in the residual time
courses something important has not been modelled
Experimental time course regressors no longer
square wave because convolved with HRF model
84
Autocorrelation FILM prewhitening
  • First, fit the GLM
  • Estimate the temporal autocorrelation structure
    in the residuals
  • The estimated structure can be inverted and used
    as a temporal filter to undo the autocorrelation
    structure in the data
  • the filter is also applied to the design matrix
  • Refit the GLM
  • DOF n-1 will now correctly reflect what is really
    free to vary in the timecourse
  • Prewhitening is selected on the stats tab in FEAT
  • it is computationally intensive, but with a
    modern PC it is manageable and there are almost
    no circumstances where you would turn this option
    off

85
(No Transcript)
86
(No Transcript)
87
(No Transcript)
88
(No Transcript)
89
(No Transcript)
90
(No Transcript)
91
(No Transcript)
92
(No Transcript)
93
(No Transcript)
94
(No Transcript)
95
(No Transcript)
96
(No Transcript)
97
(No Transcript)
98
(No Transcript)
99
(No Transcript)
100
Temporal filtering
  • Filtering in time and/or space is a
    long-established method in any signal detection
    process to help "clean up" your signal
  • The idea is if your signal and noise are present
    at separable frequencies in the data, you can
    attenuate the noise frequencies and thus increase
    your signal to noise ratio

I could illustrate this by drawing a low
frequency sinusoid called noise on the board, or
with matlab. Then draw a high frequency one
called signal underneath. Draw a third where they
are added together, and point out that the two
sinusoids could be seperated mathematically, even
if you did not know apriori their amplitudes and
frequencies. In a second example I make noise and
signal have similar frequency and show that when
added together they are inseperable. This is
key point of FMRI data analysis and guiding
principle in experimental design.
Write a Comment
User Comments (0)
About PowerShow.com