The Effects of Reward Quality on Risk-sensitive Foraging Craft*, B.B., Church, A.C., Rohrbach, C.M., - PowerPoint PPT Presentation

1 / 1
About This Presentation
Title:

The Effects of Reward Quality on Risk-sensitive Foraging Craft*, B.B., Church, A.C., Rohrbach, C.M.,

Description:

Introduction Method Results Subjects Male, Sprague Dawley (a laboratory strain of Rattus norvegicus) rats; n=20 Apparatus The experimental procedure was conducted in ... – PowerPoint PPT presentation

Number of Views:42
Avg rating:3.0/5.0
Slides: 2
Provided by: KathyL84
Category:

less

Transcript and Presenter's Notes

Title: The Effects of Reward Quality on Risk-sensitive Foraging Craft*, B.B., Church, A.C., Rohrbach, C.M.,


1
The Effects of Reward Quality on Risk-sensitive
Foraging Craft, B.B., Church, A.C., Rohrbach,
C.M., Bennett, J.M.
Introduction
Method
Results
  • Subjects
  • Male, Sprague Dawley (a laboratory strain of
    Rattus norvegicus) rats n20
  • Apparatus
  • The experimental procedure was conducted in
    Lafayette operant conditioning chambers housed in
    a sound attenuating box.
  • The chambers (27.94 x 21.59 x 21.59 cm) had
    stainless steel grid flooring, stainless steel
    front and back walls, and a Plexiglas door,
    ceiling, and side walls.
  • Two bar presses, equidistant to the feeding tray,
    corresponded with either a variable (risk-prone)
    or constant (risk-averse) reward option.
  • Experimental Session
  • The Sugar Group (SG, n10) chose between a
    risk-prone (1 or 5 pellets, p0.5) and a
    risk-averse (3 pellets, p1.0) option that
    delivered a calorically rich reward (45 mg
    BioServe sugar pellets)
  • The Grain Group (GG, n10) chose between a
    risk-prone (1 or 5 pellets, p0.5) and
    risk-averse (3 pellets, p1.0) option that
    delivered a calorically poor reward (45 mg
    BioServe grain pellets).
  • Delay to reward was held constant at .1s.
  • Subjects were deprived to 90 of their
    free-feeding body weight.
  • Once subjects reached the target weight, choice
    preference was measured using operant
    conditioning chambers.

Figure 1. The mean number of choices for the
variable option during the final three sessions
of the experiment.
  • Risk-sensitive Foraging Theory
  • Risk-sensitive Foraging Theory (RSFT) was
    developed to explain a foragers shift in choice
    between a variable (risk-prone) or constant
    (risk-averse) option. In typical RSFT studies, a
    risk-averse choice yields a constant return,
    whereas a risk-prone choice yields a variable
    return. If an organism displays a risk-prone or
    risk-averse choice bias, the organism is said to
    be risk-sensitive.
  • Daily Energy Budget Rule
  • The Daily Energy Budget (DEB) rule was developed
    to describe qualitative shifts in
    risk-sensitivity and makes the following
    assumptions first, there is a non-linear
    relationship between a foragers fitness and the
    rate of gain, and second, risk-sensitivity is
    contingent on the relationship between a
    metabolic reference and the rate of caloric
    return from a food source. For example, as a
    forager reaches a negative energy budget, the DEB
    rule describes the organism as seeking a reward
    with the greatest amount of return despite the
    variability of the reward.
  • Past studies, primarily with insects, have
    manipulated reward quality by changing the mean
    reward amount or the concentration of calories in
    rewards, but support for the DEB rule in these
    experiments differs across species and procedures
    used. Contrary to insect studies, past
    experiments using rats have observed shifts in
    risk-sensitivity that are consistent with the DEB
    rule when the mean reward amount and response
    effort was manipulated.
  • Given that rats displayed shifts in choice as the
    result of fluctuations in the mean reward amount,
    it is possible that changes in the rate of
    caloric return could result in differences in
    risk-sensitivity that are consistent with the DEB
    rule. However, no studies have been conducted
    using mammalian species where the mean reward
    amount and response effort was controlled and
    caloric return was manipulated.

Conclusion
  • The results of the present study confirmed that
    subjects risk-sensitive choices were influenced
    by reward quality. In addition, these results are
    interesting in that they indicate that
    differences in the rate of caloric gain from a
    particular food source resulted in qualitative
    differences in risk-sensitivity in rats when the
    mean reward amount, delay to reward, response
    effort, and body mass were held constant.
  • Choice preferences in the current experiment are
    consistent with the predictions of the DEB rule.
    The DEB rule describes qualitative changes in
    risk-sensitivity as being the result of a
    foragers fitness and the rate of gain from a
    food source. In the current experiment, subjects
    in the SG received rewards with a higher rate of
    caloric gain and displayed a risk-averse
    strategy. However, subjects that received rewards
    with a lower rate of calorie gain made more
    variable option choices than the GG and were
    risk-indifferent.
  • One potential limitation of the current study was
    in regard to the type of rewards used. For
    instance, although the rate of caloric gain was
    different for the sugar and grain rewards,
    subjects in the current experiment could have
    been responding to the sweetness of the sugar
    pellets instead of the caloric value. Future
    studies should examine the potential effects of
    taste on risk-sensitivity by employing a sweet,
    nonnutritive option (i.e. saccharin or sucralose)
    along with a sugar and grain option.

Results
  • All data were analyzed using an alpha level of
    .05.
  • An Independent Samples t-Test was conducted to
    determine the difference between the frequency of
    risk-prone choices made by the SG (M4.76, SD4)
    and GG (M11.83, SD4.88) groups over the last
    five days of data collection.
  • The Independent Samples t-Test revealed a
    statistically significant difference between the
    groups risk-prone choices, t(18)3.54, p.002, r
    2 .41 see Figure 1.
  • Furthermore, a One-Sample t-Test was used to
    determine if the groups risk-prone responses
    deviate from chance alone. The test revealed a
    statistically significant choice preference for
    the SG, t(9) 4.14, p .003, r 2 .66. As for the
    GG, no statistically significant difference was
    observed, t(9) 1.19, p.27, r 2 .14.

Purpose
  • We hypothesized that  subjects in receiving grain
    pellets would become more risk-prone due to the
    low caloric gain of the grain pellets, whereas
    subjects in the receiving sugar pellets would
    become more risk-averse due to the high caloric
    return of the sugar pellets.
  • Furthermore, we hypothesized that subjects in
    both groups would display a risk-sensitive choice
    bias.  

Contact information craftb_at_spu.edu
Write a Comment
User Comments (0)
About PowerShow.com