Reverend Bayes Sample Sizes and Statistical Analysis - PowerPoint PPT Presentation

About This Presentation
Title:

Reverend Bayes Sample Sizes and Statistical Analysis

Description:

Reverend Bayes Sample Sizes and Statistical Analysis Statisticians Statisticians are generally power mad, that is they want to minimise uncertainty around any ... – PowerPoint PPT presentation

Number of Views:163
Avg rating:3.0/5.0
Slides: 53
Provided by: davidto4
Category:

less

Transcript and Presenter's Notes

Title: Reverend Bayes Sample Sizes and Statistical Analysis


1
Reverend BayesSample Sizesand Statistical
Analysis
2
Statisticians
  • Statisticians are generally power mad, that is
    they want to minimise uncertainty around any
    effect estimate.
  • Two camps Frequentists and Bayesians.

3
Frequentists
  • The philosophy underlying most of our statistics
    is frequentist.
  • Frequentists produce the null hypothesis. The
    experiment is set up to prove the null
    hypothesis of no difference.
  • See themselves as more objective and
    scientific than Bayesians.

4
Frequentist Null hypothesis
  • This is nonsense!
  • If we truly believed in the null hypothesis we
    would not undertake a trial. We would just chose
    the cheapest treatment or give treatments
    according to patient preferences.
  • We usually have an idea about the likely effect
    of a treatment.

5
Reverend Bayes
  • A minister who lived in the 18th Century but
    dabbled in statistics.
  • Produced Bayes theorem, which includes prior
    beliefs in statistical calculations.
  • Was not published till 20 years after his death
    - truly publish or perish.

6
God and statistics
  • Frequentists believe the truth is out there and
    we are getting sample estimates of the truth.
  • Bayes believed only God can know the truth and
    as mere mortals we can only gain probability
    estimates of the truth, which is why he developed
    Bayes theorum.

7
Bayesians
  • Until recently Bayes approach only used in
    diagnostic testing in health research.
  • Widely used in other areas.
  • Not widely used partly because of computational
    difficulties but also many think it is
    unscientific.
  • More recently computational problems have been
    largely solved and increased interest in using
    the method.

8
Bayesian statistics
  • The Bayesian approach is attractive as it is
    similar to everyday decision making.
  • One uses prior experience to make a judgement and
    use new data to inform future decisions.

9
Bayesians vs Frequentists
  • When we seek to observe a 50 increase or
    decrease in essence this is a Bayesian approach
    as we have a prior belief that A may be 50 more
    effective than B.
  • If we had a belief in the null hypothesis then
    the sample size would be infinite to prove no
    difference.

10
Prior beliefs
  • Bayesians want to be more explicit about prior
    beliefs and include these in a design and
    analysis.
  • Data would have to be particularly strong to
    overturn a prior belief or weaker to confirm.

11
Bayesian Problems
  • Bayesians argue that one should keep doing a
    study until the confidence in the results are
    credible enough to stop the trial.
  • Problem that one cannot really plan a trial
    unless we have a prior sample size.

12
Not Scientific
  • Prior beliefs may be so incorrect that they could
    mislead research. Strong prior belief was HRT
    prevented heart disease. Shown to be untrue.
    Small trials showing this to be a fallacy would
    not overturn this strong belief.

13
GRIT Trial Bayesian trial
  • The design of this trial included prior beliefs
    on the effectiveness of early or late delivery of
    babies.
  • Data were analysed every 6 months (without p
    values) and presented to clinicians in order for
    them to change their minds and either randomise
    more patients or stop randomising.

14
Bayesian analysis
  • Expect to see more studies using Bayesian methods
    in the future.
  • Rapid area of statistical and economic research.

15
Statistical Outcomes
  • Two measures of effect.
  • Dichotomous yes/no dead/alive passed/failed.
  • Continuous blood pressure weight exam scores.

16
Binary outcomes
  • Basically in a RCT we can compare the percentages
    in the two groups.
  • If the percentages are significantly different
    this is due to the intervention.

17
Continuous outcomes
  • Scores, such as blood pressure, quality of life,
    test scores are compared. Usually the mean
    scores are compared although sometimes the
    medians are used.
  • Usually, mean scores have a normal or near
    normal distribution.

18
(No Transcript)
19
Standard deviation
  • This is calculated by taking the differences of
    individual scores from the mean squaring these
    differences and dividing by the number of
    observations.
  • The square root is the SD of this.

20
Effect sizes
  • The effect size is the difference between means
    divided by the standard deviation.
  • If students in Group A have a mean score of 60 vs
    50 in Group B and the standard deviation is 20
    the effect size is 0.5 (10/20).
  • Few new health care treatments get effect sizes
    GREATER than 0.5.

21
Relative Risks etc
  • Binary outcomes are often described in relative
    risk or odds ratios. Relative risk is if
    10/100 events in group A versus 5/100 in group B.
    A vs B RR 2 (10/5) B vs A RR 0.5 (5/10).
  • Odds ratios produce similar results for rare
    events.
  • Confidence intervals passing through 1 not
    statistically significant.

22
Sample sizes for trials
  • The bigger the better size matters in trials.
  • Most trials approach sample size estimation using
    a frequentist approach.

23
Background
  • Many trials are underpowered that is they are
    too small to detect a difference that is
    important.
  • This is commonly referred to as a Type II error.
  • At least 30 of trials published in major general
    journals are underpowered.
  • This is worse among other journals.

24
Meta-analysis of Hip Protectors (Ranked by Size)
Energy absorbing or unknow types
Community
Community
Nursing Home
Shell type Protectors
Community
Community
25
Hip protector trials
  • All trials (bar ours of course) were underpowered
    to detect large (e.g., 50) reductions in hip
    fractures.
  • Small positive trials tended to be published
    giving an overestimated effect of benefit.

26
Sample size estimation
  • Text books usually recommend the following
    approach to sample size estimation.
  • Define a clinically important difference in
    outcome between treatments
  • Design an experiment that is sufficiently large
    to show that such a difference is statistically
    significant.

27
Clinical Significance
  • The first problem is definition of what is
    clinically significant. This is usually
    unclear.
  • Any difference of death, for example, is pretty
    clinically significant.
  • To power a trial to reduce mortality by 1 death
    would require an almost infinitely large study.

28
Epidemiological Significance
  • A more common justification of sample size is
    observed effect sizes from epidemiological
    studies (which may be overestimates).
  • Or from meta-analyses of smaller trials (which
    again may over-estimate due to publication bias).

29
Statistical significance
  • What is statistical significance? Tradition in
    medical research states that p 0.05 or lower is
    significant. Difference between p 0.05 and p
    0.06 is trivial, one is significant and the other
    is not.
  • Other disciplines, economics, sometimes use p
    0.10.

30
P values
  • Originally Pearson constructed p values as a
    guide not as a cut off. The idea was that given
    what was known about a treatment (side-effects
    etc) the p value would add extra information as
    to whether one should accept the finding.
  • But p value 0.05 has become set in stone.

31
Fallacy of P values
  • If there is a treatment effect that is not
    statistically significant p 0.20 and the null
    hypothesis is accepted (I.e. there is no
    difference) you would have only a 20 chance of
    being correct and 80 of making the wrong
    decision.
  • Really one should go for a treatment that the
    data favours irrespective of the p value.

32
Significance
  • BOTH clinical and statistical significance are
    often arbitary constructs.
  • Economic significance can be less arbitrary.
  • One can ascertain an economic difference that
    makes sense.
  • To demonstrate cost neutrality is a significant
    endpoint.

33
Economic Significance
  • For example, a randomised trial of two methods of
    endometrial resection was powered to detect a 15
    difference in satisfaction.
  • Important clinical outcome was re-treatment
    rates.
  • An economic difference of significance was about
    8 in retreatment rates as this would be cost
    saving.

Torgerson Campbell BMJ 2000697.
34
Endometrial Resection
  • The trial was only sufficiently powerful to show
    a 12 difference in retreatment rates.
  • Trial showed a 4 difference (95 CI of 4 to
    11) but could not exclude an 8 difference.

Pinion et al. BMJ 1994309979-83.
35
Forget theory
  • What normally happens is Clinician says to
    statistician I can get 70 patients in a trial in
    a year.
  • Stato says needs to be bigger clinician has a
    couple of mates who can add 140 more.
    Statistician calculates difference that 210
    participants can show.

36
What should be done?
  • For a continuous outcome (e.g. Quality of Life,
    blood pressure) we should aim to detect AT LEAST
    half a standardize effect size, which needs 128
    participants.
  • Ideally we need to detect a somewhat smaller
    difference.
  • For dichotomous outcome we should have enough
    power to detect a halving or doubling.

37
Attrition and clustering
  • Do not forget to boost sample size to take into
    account loss to follow-up.
  • Depending on patient group this might range from
    5-30.
  • Finally, if it is a cluster trial total sample
    size needs to be inflated.

38
How to calculate a sample size
  • This is easy. Lots of tables or programmes will
    do this. For continuous outcomes a simple
    formulae is take standardised difference and
    divide the square of this into 32 (80 power) or
    42 (90 power).
  • E.g., 0.5 squared is 0.25 32/0.25 128 or
    42/0.25 168.

39
For binary outcomes
  • Look at sample size tables or use programme, but
    rule of thumb about 800 is needed for 80 power
    to show 10 difference between 40 and 50 or 50
    and 60. To see 5 difference quadruple sample
    size.

40
Cluster trials
  • For cluster trials we need to inflate the sample
    size to take into account the ICC of the
    clusters. 1(cluster size X ICC) design
    effect.
  • For example, a RCT of adult literacy classes mean
    size 8. ICC from a previous trial shows ICC of
    reading 0.3.

41
Cluster sample size
  • We want to detect 0.5 difference which for an
    individual RCT 128 for 80 power. Cluster size
    8 take 1 7.
  • 7 x 0.3 2.1 1 3.1 397 participants or t
    50 clusters of a mean of 8 per cluster.

42
Analysis
  • The first analysis that many people do is compare
    groups at baseline.
  • Typical many comparisons are made, for example, a
    paper of a trial in the most recent JAMA (Feb 4,
    2004) shows this typical baseline comparison
    table.

43
Baseline Tests (n 24 tests)
44
Baseline testing
  • Of the 24 comparisons 3 were statistically
    significant (I.e, p lt 0.05).
  • What should we do with this information?
  • Has randomisation failed?
  • It is useless information and an exercise in
    futility.

45
Baseline testing
  • Assuming randomisation has not been subverted,
    which in this case looks unlikely, then any
    differences will have occurred by chance they
    are random differences.

46
What is wrong with baseline testing?
  • Baseline testing will ALWAYS throw up chance
    differences. This can mislead the credulous into
    believing there is something wrong with the
    study. Also it can mislead some statisticians
    into correcting these baseline imbalances in
    the analysis.

47
Baseline variables What should be done?
  • Before the study starts specify in advance
    important co-variates to be used in the analysis
    (e.g., centre, age) and adjust for these
    IRRESPECTIVE of whether or not randomisation
    balances them out.

48
Interim Data Analysis
  • This is where the trial is analysed BEFORE
    completion.
  • This is done usually for ethical reasons so that
    a trial can be stopped early if there is an
    overwhelming benefit or harm.
  • Womens Health Initiative trial undertook an
    interim analysis and the trial was stopped
    because of harm.

49
Dangers of Interim Analysis
  • Sample size calculations assume 1 analysis.
    Repeated looks at the data WILL showed a
    significant differences, by chance, even when no
    difference exists.
  • The temptation is to stop the trial early when a
    statistical significance is achieved.
  • This could be a chance finding.

50
Interim Analysis
  • To avoid premature stopping of a trial interim
    analyses are usually undertaken by an independent
    committee with experience trialists.
  • Statistical significance is adjusted to take
    repeated looks of data into account (so p 0.01
    is significant rather than p 0.05).

51
Analysis
  • All point estimates should be bounded by
    confidence intervals as well as the exact p
    value. A single principal analysis should be
    stated in advance (e.g., the primary outcome was
    a reduction in ALL fractures) secondary analysis
    are for research interest only.

52
Summary
  • Sample size estimation is EASY. The difficult
    bit is determining the likely effect size to
    inform the calculations.
  • Analyses are more straightforward from RCTs than
    non-RCTs because you do not need to adjust for
    baseline co-variates.
Write a Comment
User Comments (0)
About PowerShow.com