Title: PSY 369: Psycholinguistics
1PSY 369 Psycholinguistics
- Language Production
- Experimentally elicited speech errors
2Your speech error collections
- How did it go?
- What interesting things did you notice?
- What difficulties did you encounter?
- Etc.
3Problems with speech errors
- Not an on-line technique.
- We only remember (or notice) certain types of
errors. - People often dont (notice or) write down errors
which are corrected part way through the word,
e.g. wo..wring one.
4Problems with speech errors
- Even very carefully verified corpora of speech
errors tend to list the error and then the
target. - However, there may be several possible targets.
- Saying there is one definitive target may limit
conclusions about what type of error has actually
occurred. - Evidence that we are not very good at perceiving
speech errors.
5Problems with speech errors
- How well do we perceive speech errors?
- Ferber (1991)
- Method
- Transcripts of TV and radio were studied very
carefully to pick out all the speech errors.
- The errors spotted by the subjects were compared
with those that actually occurred.
6Problems with speech errors
- How well do we perceive speech errors?
- Ferber (1991)
- Results
- Subjects missed 50 of all the errors
- And of the half they identified
- 50 were incorrectly recorded (i.e. only 25 of
speech errors were correctly recorded). - Conclusion We are bad at perceiving errors.
7Experimental speech errors
- Can we examine speech errors in under more
controlled conditions? - SLIP technique speech error elicitation
technique - Motley and Baars (1976)
8Say the words silently as quickly as you can Say
them aloud if you hear a ring
9dog bone
10dust ball
11dead bug
12doll bed
13darn bore
barn door
14Experimental speech errors
- This technique has been found to elicit 30 of
predicted speech errors. - Lexical Bias effect error frequency affected by
whether the error results in real words or
non-words
More likely
wrong loot FOR long root rawn loof
FOR lawn roof
15Experimental speech errors
- Influence of semantics (Motley, 1980)
- Hypothesis
- If preceded by phonologically and semantically
biasing material (PS) - If preceded by only phonologically biasing
material (P).
Predicted to be more likely
16Experimental speech errors
- Influence of semantics (Motley, 1980)
- Method 2 matched lists
- 20 word pairs as targets for errors
- e.g. bad mug ? mad bug
- Each preceded by 4 - 7 neutral filler word pairs
red cars
rainy days
small cats
mashed buns
mangy bears
angry insect
angled inset
- Then 4 interference word pairs
- 2 phonological PLUS
ornery fly
older flu
bad mug
- or
- semantically neutral controls (P)
17Experimental speech errors
- Influence of semantics (Motley, 1980)
- Results More errors in the Semantic and
Phonological (SP) condition than in the
Phonological (P) condition. - Conclusion
- Semantic interference may contribute to a
distortion of the sound of a speakers intended
utterance
18Freudian slips
- The psycholinguistic approach
- Assume that the mechanics of slips can be
studied linguistically without reference to their
motivation. (Boomer and Laver, 1968) - Freudian approach
- Held that speech errors arise from the
concurrent action - or perhaps rather, the
opposing action - of two different intentions - Intended meaning disturbing intention ? speech
error
19Freudian slips
- In the case of female genitals, in spite of many
versuchungen temptations - I beg your pardon,
versuche experiments - From a politician I like Heath. Hes tough -
like Hitler - (shocked silence from reporters) -
Did I say Hitler? I meant Churchill.
- Are these cases of disturbing intentions or
merely cases of lexical substitution
(phonologically or semantically related words)?
20Freudian slips
- Of the 94 errors listed in Psychopathology of
Everyday Life 85 were made in normal speech.
- 51 (60) involved lexical substitution in which
the substituting word was either similar in
phonological form (27) to the intended word or
related in meaning (22).
21Freudian slips
- Of the 94 errors listed in Psychopathology of
Everyday Life 85 were made in normal speech.
- Only 10/94 of the errors reported by Freud were
spoonerisms, and 4 were from Meringer and Mayer,
1895 (an early, linguistically oriented study). - E.g. Eiwess-scheibchen (small slices of egg
white) ? Eischeissweibchen (lit.
egg-shit-female) - Alabasterbüchse (alabaster box) ?
Alabüsterbachse (büste breast)
22Freudian slips
- Hence, it appears that Freuds theory can be
translated into the language of modern
psycholinguistic production models without
excessive difficulty.
23Experimental Freudian slips?
- Motley Baars (1979)
- Hypothesis Spoonerisms more likely when the
resulting content is congruous with the
situational context. - Method 90 males, same procedure previously used
by Motley, 1980 (SLIP). - 3 Conditions
- Electricity - expecting to get shocked
- Sex - researcher provocatively attired female
- Neutral
24Experimental Freudian slips?
- Same word pairs in all conditions
- spoonerism targets were non-words (e.g. goxi furl
? foxy girl), targets preceded by 3
phonologically biasing word pairs not
semantically related to target words - Some resulting errors were sexually related (S),
some were electrically related (E) - Bine foddy -gt fine body
- Had bock -gt bad shock
25(No Transcript)
26car tires
27cat toys
28can tops
29cup trays
30cool tits
tool kits
31Experimental Freudian slips?
- Results (number of errors, by type)
- Electricity set 69 E, 31 S
- Sex set 36 E, 76 S
- Neutral set 44 E, 41 S
- Hence errors were in the expected direction.
- Conclusion subjects speech encoding systems are
sensitive to semantic influences from their
situational cognitive set.
32Experimental Freudian slips?
- Hypothesis subjects with high levels of sex
anxiety will make more sex spoonerisms than
those with low sex anxiety. - Method
- 36 males selected on the basis of high, medium,
low sex anxiety (Mosher Sex-Guilt Inventory). - SLIP task same as previous experiment but with 2
additional Sex targets and 9 Neutral targets.
33Experimental Freudian slips?
- Results looked at difference scores (Sex -
Neutral) - High sex anxiety gt medium gt low.
- Overall Sex spoonerisms gt Neutral spoonerisms.
- Conclusion appears to support Freuds view of
sexual anxiety being revealed in Slips of the
Tongue - BUT the experimenters (Baars and Motley) went on
to show that any type of anxiety, not just sexual
produced similar results. - SO anxiety was at play but it was more general,
so the priming was more global.
34From thought to speech
Jane threw the ball to Bill
- What do speech errors suggest?
- Productivity Units
- Advanced planning
35Conclusions
- Speech errors have provided data about the units
of speech production.
- Phonology - consonants, vowels, and consonant
clusters (/fl/) can be disordered as units. Also,
phonetic features. - Syllables which have morphemic status can be
involved in errors. Separation of stem morphemes
from affixes (inflectional and derivational). - Stress? Stress errors could be examples of blends.
36Conclusions
- Speech errors have provided data about the units
of speech production.
- Syntax -grammatical rules may be applied to the
wrong unit, but produce the correct pronunciation
(e.g. plural takes the correct form /s/, /z/, or
/iz/. - Indicates that these parts of words are marked as
grammatical morphemes. - Phrases (e.g. NP) and clauses can be exchanged or
reversed. - Words - can exchange, move, or be mis-selected.
37From thought to speech
Message level
- Propositions to be communicated
- Selection and organization of lexical items
- Morphologically complex words are constructed
- Sound structure of each word is built
38From thought to speech
Message level
- Propositions to be communicated
- Not a lot known about this step
- Typically thought to be shared with comprehension
processes, semantic networks, situational models,
etc.
Syntactic level
Morphemic level
Phonemic level
Articulation
39From thought to speech
Message level
- Grammatical class constraint
- Most substitutions, exchanges, and blends involve
words of the same grammatical class - Slots and frames
- A syntactic framework is constructed, and then
lexical items are inserted into the slots
Syntactic level
Morphemic level
Phonemic level
Articulation
40From thought to speech
Ross
Emily
Rachel
It was such a happy moment when Ross kissed
Rachel
41From thought to speech
Ross
Emily
Rachel
Oops! I mean kissed Emily.
42From thought to speech
- LEXICON
- ROSS
- KISS
- EMILY
- RACHEL
Spreading activation
43From thought to speech
- LEXICON
- ROSS
- KISS
- EMILY
- RACHEL
If the word isnt the right grammatical class, it
wont fit into the slot.
- Grammatical class constraint
44From thought to speech
Message level
- Grammatical class constraint
- Most substitutions, exchanges, and blends involve
words of the same grammatical class - Slots and frames
- Other evidence
- Syntactic priming
Syntactic level
Morphemic level
Phonemic level
Articulation
45Syntactic priming
- Bock (1986) syntactic persistance tested by
picture naming
Hear and repeat a sentence
Describe the picture
46Syntactic priming
- a The ghost sold the werewolf a flower
- Bock (1986) syntactic persistance tested by
picture naming
- b The ghost sold a flower to the werewolf
- a The girl gave the teacher the flowers
- b The girl gave the flowers to the teacher
47Syntactic priming
- In real life, syntactic priming seems to occur as
well - Branigan, Pickering, Cleland (2000)
- Speakers tend to reuse syntactic constructions of
other speakers - Potter Lombardi (1998)
- Speakers tend to reuse syntactic constructions of
just read materials
48From thought to speech
Message level
- Stranding errors
- I liked he would hope you
- I hoped he would like you
Syntactic level
- The inflection stayed in the same location, the
stems moved - Inflections tend to stay in their proper place
- Do not typically see errors like
- The beeing are buzzes
- The bees are buzzing
Morphemic level
Phonemic level
Articulation
49From thought to speech
Message level
- Closed class items very rare in exchanges or
substitutions - Two possibilities
- Part of syntactic frame
- High frequency, so lots of practice, easily
selected, etc.
Syntactic level
Morphemic level
Phonemic level
Articulation
50From thought to speech
Message level
- Consonant vowel regularity
- Consonants slip with other consonants, vowels
with vowels, but rarely do consonants slip with
vowels - The implication is that vowels and consonants
represent different kinds of units in
phonological planning
Syntactic level
Morphemic level
Phonemic level
Articulation
51From thought to speech
Message level
- Consonant vowel regularity
- Frame and slots in syllables
- Similar to the slots and frames we discussed with
syntax
Syntactic level
Morphemic level
Phonemic level
Articulation
52From thought to speech
PHONOLOGICAL FRAME
- LEXICON
- /d/, C
- /g/, C
- , V
Word
Syllable
Onset
Rhyme
V
C
C
53From thought to speech
Message level
- Consonant vowel regularity
- Frame and slots in syllables
- Evidence for the separation of meaning and sound
Syntactic level
- Tip of the tongue
- Picture-word interference
Morphemic level
Phonemic level
Articulation
54Tip-of-the-tongue
Uhh It is a.. You know.. A.. Arggg. I can
almost see it, it has two Syllables, I think it
starts with A ..
- TOT
- Meaning access
- No (little) phonological access
- What about syntax?
55Tip-of-the-tongue
- The rhythm of the lost word may be there without
the sound to clothe it or the evanescent sense
of something which is the initial vowel or
consonant may mock us fitfully, without growing
more distinct. (James, 1890, p. 251)
56Tip-of-the-tongue
- Low-frequency words (e.g., apse, nepotism,
sampan), prompted by brief definitions. - On 8.5 of trials, tip-of-the-tongue state
ensued - Had to guess
- word's first or last letters
- the number of syllables it contained
- which syllable was stressed
57Tip-of-the-tongue
- Total of 360 TOT states
- 233 "positive TOTs" (subject was thinking of
target word, and produced scorable data - 127 "negative TOTs" (subject was thinking of
other word, but could not recall it) - 224 similar-sound TOTs (e.g., Saipan for sampan)
- 48 had the same number of syllables as the
target - 95 similar-meaning TOTs (e.g., houseboat for
sampan). - 20 had same number of syllables as target.Â
58Tip-of-the-tongue
- Similar words come to mind about half the time
- but how much is just guessing?
- First letter correct 50-71 of time (vs. 10 by
chance) - First sound 36 of time (vs. 6 by chance)
59Tip-of-the-tongue
- Results suggest a basic split between
semantics/syntax and phonology - People can access meaning and grammar but not
pronunciation
60Tip-of-the-tongue
- Semantics
- Syntax
- grammatical category (part of speech)
- e.g. noun, verb, adjective
- Gender
- e.g. le chien, la vache le camion, la voiture
- Number
- e.g. dog vs. dogs trousers vs. shirt
- Count/mass status
- e.g. oats vs. flour
61Tip-of-the-tongue
- Vigliocco et al. (1997)
- Subjects presented with word definitions
- Gender was always arbitrary
- If unable to retrieve word, they answered
- How well do you think you know the word?
- Guess the gender
- Guess the number of syllables
- Guess as many letters and positions as possible
- Report any word that comes to mind
- Then presented with target word
- Do you know this word?
- Is this the word you were thinking of?
62Vigliocco et al (1997)
- Scoring
- TOT
- Both reported some correct information in
questionnaire - And said yes to recognition question
- - TOT
- Otherwise
63Vigliocco et al (1997)
- Results
- TOT 84 correct gender guess
- - TOT 53 correct gender guess
- chance level
- Conclusion
- Subjects often know grammatical gender
information even when they have no phonological
information - Supports split between syntax and phonology in
production
64MODELS OF PRODUCTION
- As in comprehension, there are serial (modular)
and interactive models - Serial models - Garrett, Levelt et al.
- Interactive models - Stemberger, Dell
- Levelts monitoring stage (originally proposed by
Baars) can explain much of the data that is said
to favour interaction between earlier levels
65Doing it in time
- Strongest constraint may be fluency
- Have to get form right under time pressure.
- Incrementality
- Work with what youve got
- Flexibility allows speaker to say something
quickly, also respond to changing environment. - Modularity
- Work only with what youve got
- Regulate flow of information.
66Comparing models
- Central questions
- Are the stages discrete or cascading?
- Discrete must complete before moving on
- Cascade can get started as soon as some
information is available - Is there feedback?
- Top-down only
- Bottom up too
- How many levels are there?
67From thought to speech
- How does a mental concept get turned into a
spoken utterance? - Levelt, 1989, 4 stages of production
- Conceptualising we conceptualise what we wish to
communicate (mentalese). - Formulating we formulate what we want to say
into a linguistic plan. - Lexicalisation
- Lemma Selection
- Lexeme (or Phonological Form) Selection
- Syntactic Planning
- Articulating we execute the plan through muscles
in the vocal tract. - Self-monitoring we monitor our speech to assess
whether it is what we intended to say, and how we
intended to say it.
68A model of sentence production
- Three broad stages
- Conceptualisation
- deciding on the message ( meaning to express)
- Formulation
- turning the message into linguistic
representations - Grammatical encoding (finding words and putting
them together) - Phonological encoding (finding sounds and putting
them together) - Articulation
- speaking (or writing or signing)
69Levelts model
- Four broad stages
- Conceptualisation
- deciding on the message ( meaning to express)
- Formulation
- turning the message into linguistic
representations - Grammatical encoding (finding words and putting
them together) - Phonological encoding (finding sounds and putting
them together) - Articulation
- speaking (or writing or signing)
- Monitoring (via the comprehension system)
70Levelts model
- Network has three strata
- conceptual stratum
- lemma stratum
- word-form stratum
71Levelts model
- Tip of tongue state when lemma is retrieved
without word-form being retrieved
- Formulation involves lexical retrieval
- Semantic/syntactic content (lemma)
- Phonological content (word-form)
72Levelts model
has stripes
is dangerous
Lexical concepts
TIGER (X)
Lexicon
Noun
countable
tigre
Lemmas
Fem.
Lexemes
/tigre/
/t/
/I/
/g/
Phonemes
73Conceptual stratum
- Conceptual stratum is not decomposed
- one lexical concept node for tiger
- instead, conceptual links from tiger to
stripes, etc.
has stripes
is dangerous
TIGER (X)
74Lexical selection
- First, lemma activation occurs
- This involves activating a lemma or lemmas
corresponding to the concept - thus, concept TIGER activates lemma tiger
TIGER (X)
Noun
countable
tiger
Fem.
75Lexical selection
- First, lemma activation occurs
- This involves activating a lemma or lemmas
corresponding to the concept - thus, concept TIGER activates lemma tiger
TIGER (X)
LION (X)
tiger
lion
- But also involves activating other lemmas
- TIGER also activates LION (etc.) to some extent
- and LION activates lemma lion
76Lemma selection
- Selection is different from activation
- Only one lemma is selected
- Probability of selecting the target lemma
(tiger) - ratio of that lemmas activation to the total
activation of all lemmas (tiger, lion, etc.) - Hence competition between semantically related
lemmas
TIGER (X)
LION (X)
tiger
lion
77Morpho-phonological encoding (and beyond)
- The lemma is now converted into a phonological
representation - called word-form (or lexeme)
- If tiger lemma plus plural (and noun) are
activated - Leads to activation of morphemes tigre and s
- Other processes too
- Stress, phonological segments, phonetics, and
finally articulation
/tigre/
/t/
/I/
/g/
78Models assumptions
- Modularity
- Later processes cannot affect earlier processes
- No feedback between the word-form (lexemes) layer
and the grammatical (lemmas) layer - Also, only one lemma activates a word form
- If tiger and lion lemmas are activated, they
compete to produce a winner at the lemma stratum - Only the winner activates a word form
- The word-forms for the losers arent accessed
79Experimental tests
- Picture-word interference task
- Participants name basic objects as quickly as
possible - Distractor words are embedded in the object
- participants are instructed to ignore these words
tiger
80Basic findings
- Semantically related words can interfere with
naming - e.g., the word TIGER in a picture of a LION
tiger
81Basic findings
- However, form-related words can speed up
processing - e.g., the word liar in a picture of a LION
liar
82time
- Experiments manipulate timing
- picture and word can be presented simultaneously
83liar
liar
time
- Experiments manipulate timing
- picture and word can be presented simultaneously
- or one can slightly precede the other
- We draw inferences about time-course of processing
84Schriefers, Meyer, and Levelt (1990)
- Auditory presentation of distractors
- DOT phonologically related
- CAT semantically related
- SHIP unrelated word
- SOA (Stimulus onset asynchrony) manipulation
- -150 ms (word 150 ms picture)
- 0 ms (i.e., synchronous presentation)
- 150 ms (picture 150ms word)
85Schriefers, Meyer, and Levelt (1990)
- Auditory presentation of distractors
- DOT phonologically related
- CAT semantically related
- SHIP unrelated word
Early Only Semantic effects
86Schriefers, Meyer, and Levelt (1990)
- Auditory presentation of distractors
- DOT phonologically related
- CAT semantically related
- SHIP unrelated word
Late Only Phonological effects
87Interpretation
- Early semantic inhibition
- Late phonological facilitation
- Fits with the assumption that semantic processing
precedes phonological processing - No overlap
- suggests two discrete stages in production
- an interactive account might find semantic and
phonological effects at the same time
88Dells interactive account
- Dell (1986) presented the best-known interactive
account - other similar accounts exist
- Network organization with
- 3 levels of representation
- Semantics (decomposed into features)
- Words and morphemes
- phonemes (sounds)
- These get selected and inserted into frames
89- Dell (1986)
- A moment in the production of
- Some swimmers sink
90Dell (1986)
information
91Dell (1986)
- e.g., the semantic features mammal, barks,
four-legs activate the word dog
FURRY
BARKS
MAMMAL
- this activates the sounds /d/, /o/, /g/
- these send activation back to the word level,
activating words containing these sounds (e.g.,
log, dot) to some extent
dog
log
dot
/a/
/g/
/d/
/l/
/t/
this activation is upwards (phonology to syntax)
and wouldnt occur in Levelts account
92Evidence for Dells model
- Mixed errors
- Both semantic and phonological relationship to
target word - Target cat
- semantic error dog
- phonological error hat
- mixed error rat
- Occur more often than predicted by modular models
- if you can go wrong at either stage, it would
only be by chance that an error would be mixed
93Dells explanation
- The process of making an error
- The semantic features of dog activate cat
- Some features (e.g., animate, mammalian) activate
rat as well - cat then activates the sounds /k/, /ae/, /t/
- /ae/ and /t/ activate rat by feedback
- This confluence of activation leads to increased
tendency for rat to be uttered - Also explains the tendency for phonological
errors to be real words - Sounds can only feed back to words (non-words not
represented) so only words can feedback to sound
level
94Why might interaction occur?
- Cant exist just to produce errors!
- So what is feedback for?
- Perhaps because the same network is used in
comprehension - So feedback would be the normal comprehension
route - Alternatively, it simply serves to increase
fluency in lemma selection - advantageous to select a lemma whose phonological
form is easy to find
95Evidence against interactivity
- Schriefers, Meyer, and Levelt (1990)
- DOT phonologically related
- CAT semantically related
- SHIP unrelated word
Early Only Semantic effects
Late Only Phonological effects
96Evidence against interactivity
- Schriefers, Meyer, and Levelt (1990)
- Also looked for any evidence of a mediated
priming effect
DOG (X)
CAT (X)
dog
cat
hat
/cat/
/hat/
/t/
/a/
/k/
/h/
97Evidence for interactivity
- A number of recent experimental findings appear
to support interaction under some circumstances
(or at least cascading models) - Damian Martin (1999)
- Cutting Ferreira (1999)
- Peterson Savoy (1998)
98Evidence for interactivity
- Damian and Martin (1999)
- Picture-Word interference
- The critical difference
- the addition of a semantic and phonological
condition - Picture of Apple
- peach (semantically related)
- apathy (phonologically related)
- apricot (sem phono related)
- couch (unrelated)
- (also no-word control, always fast)
peach
99Results
- early semantic inhibition
100Results
- early semantic inhibition
- late phonological facilitation (0 and 150 ms)
101Results
- early semantic inhibition
- late phonological facilitation (0 and 150 ms)
- Shows overlap, unlike Schriefers et al.
102Evidence for interactivity
- Cutting and Ferreira (1999)
- Picture-Word interference
- The critical difference
- Used homophone pictures
- Related distractors could be to the depicted
meaning or alternative meaning - game
- dance
- hammer (unrelated)
- Only tested -150 SOA
103Evidence against interactivity
- Cutting and Ferreira (1999)
BALL (X)
BALL (X)
DANCE (X)
GAME (X)
ball
ball
dance
game
/ball/
Cascading Prediction
dance
ball
/ball/
104Results
- Cutting and Ferreira (1999)
- Early semantic inhibition
105Results
- Cutting and Ferreira (1999)
- Early semantic inhibition
- Early Facilitation from a phonologically mediated
distractor
- Evidence of cascading information flow (both
semantic and phonological information at early
SOA)
106Evidence for interactivity
- Peterson Savoy
- Slightly different task
- Prepare to name the picture
- If ? comes up name it
?
107Evidence for interactivity
- Peterson Savoy
- Slightly different task
- Prepare to name the picture
- If ? comes up name it
- If a word comes up instead, name the word
liar
- Manipulate
- Word/picture relationship
- SOA
108Evidence for interactivity
- Peterson Savoy
- Used pictures with two synonymous names
Dominant
subordinate
- Used words that were phonologically related to
the non dominant name of the picture
sofa
couch
109Evidence for interactivity
- Peterson Savoy
- Found evidence for phonological activation of
near synonyms - Participants slower to say distractor soda than
unrelated distractor when naming couch - Soda is related to non-selected sofa
- Remember that Levelt et al. assume that only one
lemma can be selected and hence activate a
phonological form - Levelt et als explanation Could be erroneous
selection of two lemmas?
110Evidence for interactivity
- Summary
- These the findings appears to contradict the
discrete two-step account of Levelt et al.
111Can the two-stage account be saved?
- Evidence for interaction is hard to reconcile
with the Levelt account - However, most attempts are likely to revolve
around the monitor - Basically, people sometimes notice a problem and
screen it out - Levelt argues that evidence for interaction
really involves special cases, not directly
related to normal processing
112Overall summary
- Levelt et al.s theory of word production
- Strictly modular lexical access
- Syntactic processing precedes phonological
processing - Dells interactive account
- Interaction between syntactic and phonological
processing - Experimental evidence is equivocal, but
increasing evidence that more than one lemma may
activate associated word-form
113Summary
- Levelt et al.s theory of word production
- Strictly modular lexical access
- Syntactic processing precedes phonological
processing - Dells interactive account
- Interaction between syntactic and phonological
processing - Experimental evidence is equivocal, but
increasing evidence that more than one lemma may
activate associated wordform
114Caramazzas alternative
- Caramazza and colleagues argue against the
existence of the lemma node - instead they propose a direct link between
semantic level and lexeme - syntactic information is associated with the
lexeme - Also assumes separate lexemes for written and
spoken production - This is really a different issue
115- Much evidence comes from patient data
- But also evidence from the independence of
syntactic and phonological information in TOT
states - see discussion of Vigliocco et al.
- also Caramazza and Miozzo (Cognition, 1997 see
also replies by Roelofs et al.)
116From thought to speech
- How does a mental concept get turned into a
spoken utterance? - Levelt, 1989, 4 stages of production
- Conceptualising we conceptualise what we wish to
communicate (mentalese). - Formulating we formulate what we want to say
into a linguistic plan. - Lexicalisation
- Lemma Selection
- Lexeme (or Phonological Form) Selection
- Syntactic Planning
- Articulating we execute the plan through muscles
in the vocal tract. - Self-monitoring we monitor our speech to assess
whether it is what we intended to say, and how we
intended to say it.
117Models of production
- As in comprehension, there are serial (modular)
and interactive models - Serial models - Garrett, Levelt et al.
- Interactive models - Stemberger, Dell
- Levelts monitoring stage (originally proposed by
Baars) can explain much of the data that is said
to favour interaction between earlier levels
118An model of sentence production
- Three broad stages
- Conceptualisation
- deciding on the message ( meaning to express)
- Formulation
- turning the message into linguistic
representations - Grammatical encoding (finding words and putting
them together) - Phonological encoding (finding sounds and putting
them together) - Articulation
- speaking (or writing or signing)
119An model of sentence production
- Experimental investigations of some of these
issues - Time course - cascading vs serial
- Picture word interference
- Separation of syntax and semantics
- Subject verb agreement
- Abstract syntax vs surface form
- Syntactic priming
120Conversational interaction
the horse raced past the barn
the kids swam across the river
Conversation is more than just two side-by-side
monologues.
121Conversational interaction
The horse raced past the barn
Really? Why would it do that?
Conversation is a specialized form of social
interaction, with rules and organization.
122Conversation
- Herb Clark (1996)
- Joint action
- People acting in coordination with one another
- doing the tango
- driving a car with a pedestrian crossing the
street - The participants dont always do similar things
- Autonomous actions
- Things that you do by yourself
- Participatory actions
- Individual acts only done as parts of joint
actions
123Conversation
- Speaking and listening
- Traditionally treated as autonomous actions
- Contributing to the tradition of studying
language comprehension and production separately - Clark proposed that they should be treated as
participatory actions
124Conversation
- Speaking and listening
- Component actions in production and comprehension
come in pairs
Speaking
Listening
- B attends to As vocalizations
- A formalizes utterances for B
- B identifies As utterances
- The actions of one participant depend on the
actions of the other
125Conversation
- Face-to-face conversation - the basic setting
- Features
Immediacy
Medium
Control
- Co-presence
- Visibility
- Audibility
- Instantaneity
- Evanescence
- Recordlessness
- Simultaneity
- Extemporaneity
- Self-determination
- Self-expression
- Other settings may lack some of these features
- e.g., telephone conversations take away
co-presence and visibility, which may change
language use
126Meaning and understanding
- ABBOTT Super Duper computer store. Can I help
you? - COSTELLO Thanks. I'm setting up an office in my
den, and I'm thinking about buying a computer. - ABBOTT Mac?
- COSTELLO No, the name is Lou.
- ABBOTT Your computer?
- COSTELLO I don't own a computer. I want to buy
one. - ABBOTT Mac?
- COSTELLO I told you, my name is Lou.
- ABBOTT What about Windows?
- COSTELLO Why? Will it get stuffy in here?
- ABBOTT Do you want a computer with windows?
- COSTELLO I don't know. What will I see when I
look in the windows? - ABBOTT Wallpaper.
- COSTELLO Never mind the windows. I need a
computer and software. - ABBOTT Software for windows?
- COSTELLO No. On the computer! I need something I
can use to write proposals, track expenses and
run my business. What have you got? - ABBOTT Office.
127Meaning and understanding
- COSTELLO Yeah, for my office. Can you recommend
anything? - ABBOTT I just did.
- COSTELLO You just did what?
- ABBOTT Recommend something.
- COSTELLO You recommended something?
- ABBOTT Yes.
- COSTELLO For my office?
- ABBOTT Yes.
- COSTELLO OK, what did you recommend for my
office? - ABBOTT Office.
- COSTELLO Yes, for my office!
- ABBOTT I recommend office with windows.
- COSTELLO I already have an office and it has
windows!OK, lets just say, I'm sitting at my
computer and I want to type a proposal. What do I
need? - ABBOTT Word.
- COSTELLO What word?
- ABBOTT Word in Office.
- COSTELLO The only word in office is office.
- ABBOTT The Word in Office for Windows.
128Meaning and understanding
- COSTELLO Which word in office for windows?
- ABBOTT The Word you get when you click the blue
"W. - COSTELLO I'm going to click your blue "w" if you
don't start with some straight answers. OK,
forget that. Can I watch movies on the Internet? - ABBOTT Yes, you want Real One.
- COSTELLO Maybe a real one, maybe a cartoon.
What I watch is none of your business. Just tell
me what I need! - ABBOTT Real One.
- COSTELLO If it?s a long movie I also want to see
reel 2, 3 and 4. Can I watch them? - ABBOTT Of course.
- COSTELLO Great, with what?
- ABBOTT Real One.
- COSTELLO OK, I'm at my computer and I want to
watch a movie.What do I do? - ABBOTT You click the blue "1.
- COSTELLO I click the blue one what?
- ABBOTT The blue "1.
- COSTELLO Is that different from the blue "W"?
- ABBOTT The blue 1 is Real One and the blue W is
Word. - COSTELLO What word?
129Meaning and understanding
- ABBOTT The Word in Office for Windows.
- COSTELLO But there are three words in "office
for windows"! - ABBOTT No, just one. But it?s the most popular
Word in the world. - COSTELLO It is?
- ABBOTT Yes, but to be fair, there aren't many
other Words left. It pretty much wiped out all
the other Words. - COSTELLO And that word is real one?
- ABBOTT Real One has nothing to do with Word.
Real One isn't even Part of Office. - COSTELLO Stop! Don't start that again. What
about financial bookkeeping you have anything I
can track my money with? - ABBOTT Money.
- COSTELLO That's right. What do you have?
- ABBOTT Money.
- COSTELLO I need money to track my money?
- ABBOTT It comes bundled with your computer.
- COSTELLO What's bundled to my computer?
- ABBOTT Money.
130Meaning and understanding
- COSTELLO Money comes with my computer?
- ABBOTT Yes. No extra charge.
- COSTELLO I get a bundle of money with my
computer? How much? - ABBOTT One copy.
- COSTELLO Isn't it illegal to copy money?
- ABBOTT Microsoft gave us a license to copy
money. - COSTELLO They can give you a license to copy
money? - ABBOTT Why not? THEY OWN IT!
- (LATER)
- COSTELLO How do I turn my computer off??
- ABBOTT Click on "START".
131Meaning and understanding
- Common ground
- Knowledge, beliefs and suppositions that the
participants believe that they share - Members of cultural communities
- Shared experiences
- What has taken place already in the conversation
- Common ground is necessary to coordinate
speakers meaning with listeners understanding
132Structure of a conversation
- Conversations are purposive and unplanned
- Typically you cant plan exactly what youre
going to say because it depends on another
participant - Conversations look planned only in retrospect
- Conversations have a fairly stable structure
133Structure of a conversation
- Joe (places a phone call)
- Kevin Miss Pinks office - hello
- Joe hello, is Miss Pink in
- Kevin well, shes in, but shes engaged at the
moment, who is it? - Joe Oh its Professors Worths secretary, from
Pan-American college - Kevin m,
- Joe Could you give her a message for me
- Kevin certainly
- Joe um Professor Worth said that, if Miss Pink
runs into difficulties, .. On Monday afternoon,
.. With the standing subcommittee, .. Over the
item on Miss Panoff,
- Kevin Miss Panoff?
- Joe Yes, that Professor Worth would be with Mr
Miles all afternoon, .. So she only had to go
round and collect him if she needed him, - Kevin ah, thank you very much indeed,
- Joe right
- Kevin Panoff, right you are
- Joe right
- Kevin Ill tell her,
- Joe thank you
- Kevin bye bye
- Joe bye
134Structure of a conversation
- Action sequences smaller joint projects to
fulfill a goal - Adjacency pairs
- Opening the conversation
- Kevin Miss Pinks office - hello
- Joe hello, ..
- Exchanging information about Pink
- Joe.., is Miss Pink in
- Kevin well, shes in, but shes engaged at the
moment
135Structure of a conversation
- Action sequences smaller joint projects to
fulfill a goal - Adjacency pairs
- Exchanging the message from Worth
- Joe um Professor Worth said that, if Miss Pink
runs into difficulties, .. On Monday afternoon,
.. With the standing subcommittee, .. Over the
item on Miss Panoff, - Closing the conversation
- Kevin Ill tell her,
- Joe thank you
- Kevin bye bye
- Joe bye
136Opening conversations
- Need to pick who starts
- Turn taking is typically not decided upon in
advance - Potentially a lot of ways to open, but we
typically restrict our openings to a few ways - Address another
- Request information
- Offer information
- Use a stereotyped expression or topic
137Opening conversations
- Has to resolve
- The entry time
- Is now the time to converse?
- The participants
- Who is talking to whom?
- Their roles
- What is level of participation in the
conversation? - The official business
- What is the conversation about?
138Identifying participants
- Conversation often takes place in situations that
involve various types of participants and
non-participants
139Taking turns
- Typically conversations dont involve two (or
more) people talking at the same time
- Individual styles of turn-taking vary widely
- Length of a turn is a fairly stable
characteristic within a given individuals
conversational interactions - Standard signals indicate a change in turn a
head nod, a glance, a questioning tone
140Taking turns
- Typically conversations dont involve two (or
more) people talking at the same time - Three implicit rules (Sacks et al, 1974)
- Rule 1 Current speakers selects next speaker
- Rule 2 Self-selection if rule 1 isnt used,
then next speaker can select themselves - Rule 3 current speaker may continue (or not)
- These principles are ordered in terms of priority
- The first is the most important, and the last is
the least important - Just try violating them in an actual conversation
(but debrief later!)
141Taking turns
- Typically conversations dont involve two (or
more) people talking at the same time
- Use of non-verbal cues
- Drop of pitch
- Drawl on final syllable
- Termination of hand signals
- Drop in loudness
- Completion of a grammatical clause
- Use of stereotyped phrase
- you know
142Negotiating topics
- Keep the discourse relevant to the topic
(remember Grices maxims) - Coherence again
- Earlier we looked at coherence within a speaker,
now we consider it across multiple speakers - Must use statements to signal topic shifts
143Closing conversations
- Closing statements
- Must exit from the last topic, mutually agree to
close the conversation, and coordinate the
disengagement - signal the end of conversation (or topic)
- okay
- Justifying why conversation should end
- I gotta go
- Reference to potential future conversation
- later dude
144Summary
- People use language for doing things with each
other, and their use of language is itself a
joint action. Clark (1996, pg387) - Conversation is structured
- But, that structure depends on more than one
individual - Models of language use (production and
comprehension) need to be developed within this
perspective