Title: MORPHOLOGICAL FEATURES
1- MORPHOLOGICAL FEATURES
- All languages possess the same set of grammatical
categories (about 25) such as number (singular,
dual, or plural), gender (masculine, feminine, or
neuter), tense (past, present, or future),
aspect (perfect, or imperfect), etc. - When you begin learning a new language, you do
not have to learn a new set of grammatical
categories since all languages have the same
categories you only have to learn how these
categories are expressed in the new language. It
is probably the case that children learning their
first language have a similar advantage--they are
born with these categories built into their
cognitive system.
2- MORPHOLOGICAL FEATURES
- Grammatical categories are composed of sets of
morphological features. - Gender features
- Feminine
- Animate
- Masculine Feminine Neuter
- Feminine - -
- Animate -
- The morphological features are also universal,
providing a range of possible feature
combinations that languages can make use of. Of
course, all languages do not use the same
features and combinations of features. For
example, English and French do not distinguish
the dual number, and so the feature dual is
absent from the grammars of these languages.
3THE NOTION OF PARADIGM A paradigm is the set of
the morphological realizations of the contrastive
features of a given terminal node of the
morpho-syntax. Consider three features X, Z,
Y, of a given terminal node of the morpho-syntax
in a language L. We have the following
combinations
X X -X -X X X -X -X Z -Z Z -Z Z -Z Z -
Z Y Y Y Y -Y -Y -Y -Y Assume that each
terminal node has a different exponent (where
?n??n1)
X X -X -X X X -X -X Z -Z Z -Z Z -Z Z -
Z Y Y Y Y -Y -Y -Y -Y ?? ?? ?? ?? ?? ?? ???
???????? Thus, (?????????? is the paradigm
formed by the features X, Y, Z
4- DIFFERENT MORPHOLOGICAL TYPES OF LANGUAGES
(preliminaries) - Agglutinative languages vs. Fusional languages
- AGGLUTINATIVE LANGUAGES
- In a agglutinative language, for each single
morphological category, there is a vocabulary
item. - (15) Turkish
- ev-ler-den
- house-plural-ablative
- from the houses
- (16) Finnish
- kirja t ssa ni --gt kirjoissani
in my books - N PLLocativePossessive 1sg.
5- FUSIONAL LANGUAGES
- Strings of several morphological categories are
commonly represented by single vocabulary item. - (17) Latin
- dom-is
- house-pluralablative
- Latin I-II declensions
- Singular Plural
- N. port-a ami_at_c-us port-ae
amic-i_at_ - A. port-am amic-um port-a_at_s amic-o_at_s
- G. port-ae amic-i_at_ port-a_at_rum amic-o_at_rum
- D. port-a_at_ amic-o_at_ port-i_at_s amic-is
- Ab. port-a_at_ amic-o_at_ port-i_at_s amic-I_at_s
- door friend
6- See Appendix III on Portmanteaux morphology and
feature cumulation.
7- NOMINAL GRAMMATICAL CATEGORIES
- NUMBER
- Singular, Dual, Plural
- Nouns in some languages reflect the number of
objects to which they refer. English
distinguishes only two numbers, singular and
plural. The former is used to indicate singular
objects or referents that can be neither singular
nor plural (mass nouns like contemplation).
Plural sometimes refers to singular objects, too,
e.g. glasses, so the category is clearly
grammatical and not semantic. Some languages,
Arabic or Old Church Slavonic, for example, also
distinguish objects occurring in pairs by
assigning dual number to the noun and a few
languages distinguish a paucal, used for
referring to a few objects without specifying
number - .
- (1) Yupik Nouns
- Singular Dual Plural
- tafsi 'belt' tafsi-k 'belts' tafsi-t 'belts
- tuma 'trail' tum-k 'trails tum-t 'trails
- yuk 'person' yug-k people' yug-t 'people
- Singular Dual Plural
- (2) Plural -
- Dual - -
8- GENDER
- Some languages discriminate two types of gender.
There is natural gender, which relates to the
gender of the referent and distinguishes nouns
referring to males from those referring to
females. There is also grammatical gender, which
has nothing to do with natural gender, but is
only a system of noun classes . The Indo-European
languages generally combine the two, i.e. do not
distinguish one from the other so that in French,
for example, la table 'the table' reflects
feminine gender (purely grammatical) as does la
femme 'the woman' (combined natural and
grammatical). - The most common genders are Masculine and
Feminine but some languages have Neuter as well. - NOUN CLASS
- Noun class is often closely linked to grammatical
gender in Indo-European languages, the two
generally overlap. Noun class is an arbitrary set
of categories and all nouns must belong to one of
them. There is no semantic meaning attached to
them, although there is a tendency for nouns with
similar meanings or of the same gender to belong
to the same class, e.g. all feminine nouns tend
to belong to the same class, often the names of
trees or cities will mostly belong to the same
class. In Chinese and some African languages,
noun class can be based on the physical shape of
the referent. While there are always exceptions
to these tendencies, there is no exception to the
rule that all nouns must belong to some noun
class.
9( 3) Swahili noun classes and prefixes Â
 Singular noun class example Corresponding
plural class example 1. m- person m-toto
child 2. wa- people wa-toto children  3.
m- tree m-ti tree 4. mi- trees  mi-ti
trees  5. ji- big/round j-oka giant
snake 6. ma- big/round things, liquids  7.
ki- (small) thing ki-kapu basket 8. vi-
(small) things vi-ti stools  9. N-
name 10. N- names (of people,
insects) 11. u- abstract/mass nouns u-pepo
wind 12. ku- activity ku-soma
reading 13. pa- place mahali po-moja one
place 14. ku- location (towards) 15. mu-
location (inside)
10AGREEMENT Languages often have an agreement
system whereby adjectives modifying gendered
nouns must have an ending which reflects the
gender and number of the noun they modify. Verbs
also often reflect the gender of their subject
nouns and, sometimes, their object nouns as well.
Italian ( 4) molt-e ragazz-e bell-e sono
venut-e ieri many girls beautiful have come
yesterday molt-i ragazz-i bell-i sono venut-i
ieri many boys beautiful have come yesterday
11Noun classes and Agreement in Swahili (5) a. Agre
ement with modifiers wa-toto w-a-ngu
2-child 2-POSS-1sg 'my children'
ki-kapu ki-kubwa ki-moja ki-lianguka
7-basket  7-large   7-one   7-fell One
large basket fell down. b. Agreement with
subject  m-tu    a-li-kuja       m-shale
u-li-anguka 1-person 1-past-come      Â
3-nail  3-past-fall A person
came.              A nail fell. c. Agreement
with object  ni-li-m-tafuta Â
I-past-1-seek I looked for
him/her. d. Agreement with subject and
object Yu-le m-tu m-moja m-refu
a-li-ye-ki-soma  b-le ki-tabu ki-refu 1-that
1-person 1-one 1-tall 1-past-who-7-read 7-that
7-book 7-long That tall person who read that
long book. Â Â Wa-toto wa-na-ki-soma
ki-tabu  (SVO)  2-child 2-Pres-7-read Â
7-book                       or  Â
Ki-tabu wa-na-ki-soma wa-toto (OVS) 7-book
2-Pres-7-read  2-child   The children are
reading the book.
12- DEFINITENESS Definite, Indefinite
- Most languages also have a way of distinguishing
definite and indefinite objects. A definite
object is one that the speaker expects the
listener to already know about either from
previous discussion or from experience. If you
don't expect the listener to know what you are
talking about, you would say, for example, I
bought an armadillo today. If the listener can
see the armadillo or if you have already
mentioned it to the listener, you would normally
say I bought the armadillo today. - (6) Two Ways of Indicating Definiteness
- French
- une femme 'a woman' la femme 'the woman'
- un cachet 'a seal, stamp' le cachet 'the seal,
stamp' - Bulgarian
- Jena 'a woman' Jena-ta 'the woman'
- tSovek 'a man' tSovek-t 'the man'
- selo 'a village' selo-to 'the village'
13 - Features of Person
- (7) First person Second Person Third Person
- I You he/she
- Speaker - -
- Participant to
- speech act -
- (8) Palaung (Burma).
- 1. ar 'you and I
- 2. E 'we (three or more, including addressee)
- 3. O 'I
- 4. yar 'we two (not including addressee)
- 5. yE 'we (three or more, not including
addressee) - 6. mi 'you (singular)
- 7. par 'you two
- 8. pE 'you(three or more)
- 9. Un 'he or she
14- (9) Segmentation
- O mi Un
- I you he/she
- Ø-ar y-ar p-ar g-ar Dual
- Ø-e y-E p-E g-E Plural
-
- (10) Featural analysis
- Ø- y- p- g-
- Speaker - -
- Addressee - -
- Participant to
- speech act -
15- POSSESSION 1st, 2nd, 3rd Singular Plural
- The category of possession indicates that the
referent possesses the noun marked with this
category. The functions of this category are the
same as those of verbal person, i.e. 1st, 2nd,
3rd person singular and plural. English marks
possession with possessive pronouns my/mine,
your/yours, his, her/hers, its, etc. Other
languages, such as Turkish, use inflectional
affixes and 'conjugate' their nouns. - (11) Turkish Possessive Paradigm
- Singular Plural
- baba-m 'my father' baba-m-Iz 'our father'
- baba-n 'your father' baba-n-Iz 'your(pl)
father' - baba-sI 'his/her father' baba-lar-I 'their
father'
16- CASE PARADIGM (DECLENSION)
- Languages require a means of marking certain
grammatical relations in sentences that of the
Subject to the verb, the Direct Object to the
verb, the Indirect Object to the verb, the Means
to the verb. Languages possess a limited number
of adverbal relations which could be indicated by
but are in fact always represented by grammatical
means cases, adpositions, or both. These
adverbal relations include Locative, Origin, Goal
(all of which may be spatial) and several others.
Turkish uses a set of basic Cases. - Â
- (12) The Turkish Nominal Declension
- Case 'horse' 'my horse''horses' 'my horses'
- Nominative (Subject)at-e at-Im at-lar at-lar-Im
- Genitive ('of') at-In at-Im-In at-lar-In at-lar-I
m-In - Accusative (D. O.) at-I at-Im-I
at-lar-I at-lar-Im-I - Dative ('to /for') at-a at-Im-a
at-lar-a at-lar-Im-a - Locative ('in/at') at-da at-Im-da at-lar-da at-la
r-Im-da - Ablative ('from') at-dan at-Im-dan at-lar-dan at-
lar-Im-dan
17- (13) Russian Nominal Declension
- Case 'book' 'books' 'table' 'tables'
- Nominative (Subject) knig-a knig-y stol-e
stol-y - Genitive ('of') knig-y knig-e stol-a stol-ov
- Accusative (D. 0.) knig-u knig-y stol-e stol-y
- Dative ('to /for') knig-e knig-am stol-u stol-a
m - Locative ('in/at') knig-e knig-ax stol-e stol-
ax - Instrumental (by/with) knig-oj
knig-ami stol-om stol-ami
18- TURKISH CASE FUNCTIONS
- (14) a. Nominative Case Subject/Accusative Case
Object - Halil-e kitab-i oku-du
- Halil-Nom book-Obj read-Past
- 'Halil read the book
- b. Genitive Case Possession
- Halil-in evi simdi Mehmed-in
- Halil-Gen house now Mehmed-Gen
- Halil's house is now Mehmed's
- c. Dative Case Indirect Object
- Adam-a yemek verd-im
- man-to meat gave-I
- I gave meat to the man
- d. Dative Case Goal
- Halil ev-e gel-di
- Halil house-to come-Past
19- e. Ablative Case Source (The from-Case)
- kitab-I Halil-den ald-Im
- Book-Obj Halil-from got-I
- 'I got the book from Halil
- f. Ablative Case Origin (The from-Case)
- Mehmet Istanbul-dan gel-di
- Mehmet Istanbul-from come-Past
- 'Mehmet came from Istanbul
- g. Locative Case Location (The at-Case)
- Halil ev-de kal-dI
- Halil home-Loc remain-Past
- 'Halil stayed at home
- h. Instrumental Case (Russian) (The by/with
Case) - MaSa napisala pis'mo karandaS-om
- Masha wrote letter pencil-Inst
- Masha wrote the letter with a pencil'
20- VERBS
- TRANSITIVITY Transitive, Intransitive
- Verbs may either accept a direct object or not.
Those that do are transitive those that do not
are intransitive. Some verbs can belong to either
category depending on the context, as with the
English verb "to eat." - (19) ENGLISH John ate the fish John ate
21- TENSE
- Distant Past, Past, Present, Future, Distant
Future Tense indicates the time relative to the
speech act when an action or state took place.
Languages are limited to five tenses distant
past (past pluperfect), past, future, distant
past, and distant future (future perfect).
English has all five. - (20) English Tenses
-  Future  I will eat, I'm gonna eat, I eat
-  Future Perfective  I will have eaten
-  Present  I eat, am eating
-  Past  I ate, have eaten
-  Past Perfective  I had eaten
22- (21) Sanskrit. (The forms illustrated here are
all third person singular indicative active) - present imperfect future conditional
- 'cross' tarati atarat tarisyati atarisyat
- 'move' carati acarat carisyati acarisyat
- 'forsake' tyajati atyajat tyajisyatia tyajisyat
- 'hold dharati adharat dharisyatia dharisyat
- (22) Verb stems tar- cross,'
- car- 'move,'
- tyaj- 'forsake,'
- dhar- 'hold.'
- Third person singular present suffix -ati
- The imperfect is characterized by a prefix, a-,
and a distinct third person singular suffix, -at.
- The future is characterized by the suffix -isy-
and the personal suffix that is the same as in
the present tense. - The conditional combines aspects of both the
imperfect and the future it has the prefix a-,
which is found with past tenses, along with the
personal suffix -at, also found with past tenses,
and, between the stem and the personal suffix,
the conditional has the future morpheme -isy. - (23) past present future conditional
23- ASPECT Perfective, ImperfectiveSome languages
distinguish between whether an action or state is
completed or not rather than between whether it
occurred in the past. (Most languages distinguish
both tense and aspect to varying degrees.) - The Russian Aspect System
- (24) Imperfective Perfective
- MaSa pisala pis'ma MaSa na-pisala pis'ma
- i. 'Masha was writing letters ''Masha wrote the
letters' - ii. 'Masha wrote letters several times'
- MaSa piSet pis'ma MaSa na-piSet pis'ma
- i. 'Masha is writing letters' 'Masha will write
the letters' - ii. 'Mash writes letters'
24- Latin has six combinations of tense and aspect,
of which representative forms are given in (51). - (25) am-o am-av-i
- 'I love' 'I have loved'
- (present) Perfect)
- am-a-ba-m am-av-era-m
- 'I loved' 'I had loved'
- (imperfect) (pluperfect)
- am-a-b-o am-av-er-o
- 'I will love' 'I will have loved'
- (future) (future perfect)
- In feature terms
- (26) -perfect perfect
- -past
- -future present perfect
25- VOICE
- Active, Passive Most languages have a means of
shifting the semantic emphasis from the subject
to the object. If the subject is focussed, the
verb is placed in the active voice if the object
is focussed, the verb is placed in the passive
voice. - (27) Voice in Western IE Languages
-  LANGUAGE Active Voice  Passive Voice
-  ENGLISH  John eats the fish   The fish is
eaten by John - Â FRENCH Â Jean mange le poisson Le poisson est
mange de Jean -  GERMAN  Hans isst den Fisch   Der Fisch ist
von Hans gegessen
26- MOOD Indicative, Imperative, Subjunctive
- Verbs may also vary as to mood or modality. The
three major modalities are indicative, which mere
indicates that something occurs, imperative,
which demands that something occurs, and
subjunctive, which suggests that something might
occur, usually also implying that it doesn't
occur. - (28) The Moods of English
- Indicative John eats
- Imperative Eat!
- Subjunctive I would eat, Were I to eat
- FEATURES OF MOOD
- Classical Greek mood system (Jensen (1990)
- (29) indicative subjunctive optative imperative
- potential - -
- wish - -
-
27- CONJUGATION CLASS
- Verbs often belong to arbitrary lexical classes
like the nouns the verbal version is called
conjugation class. There is no semanic reason why
a verb belongs to one class or another they just
do. Latin (French, Italian, Spanish) verbs all
must bear a conjugation class marker. - (30) The Latin Conjugations
- I. Conjugation
- amo 'I love' amamus 'we love'
- amas 'you love' amatis 'yuse love'
- amat 's/he loves' amant 'they love'
- II. Conjugation
- moneo 'I advise' monemus 'we advise'
- mones 'you advise' monetis 'yuns advise'
- monet 's/he advises' monent 'they advise
- III. Conjugation
- tego 'I cover' tegimus 'we cover'
- tegis 'you cover' tegitis 'yall cover'
- tegit 's/he covers' tegunt 'they cover
28- See Appendix IV on morphological markedness