Title: fMRI Analysis with emphasis on the General Linear Model
1fMRI Analysiswith emphasis on the General Linear
Model
Jody Culham Brain and Mind Institute Department
of Psychology University of Western Ontario
http//www.fmri4newbies.com/
Last Update January 18, 2012 Last Course
Psychology 9223, W2010, University of Western
Ontario
2Part 1
3What data do we start with
These s are from an obsolete scanner. With a
modern 3T, we can get 3X the slices
- 12 slices 64 voxels x 64 voxels 49,152 voxels
- Each voxel has 136 time points (volumes)
- Therefore, for each run, we have 6.7 million data
points - We often have several runs for each experiment
4Why do we need stats?
- We could, in principle, analyze data by voxel
surfing move the cursor over different areas and
see if any of the time courses look interesting
5Why do we need stats?
- Clearly voxel surfing isnt a viable option.
Wed have to do it 49,152 times in this data set
and it would require a lot of subjective
decisions about whether activation was real - This is why we need statistics
- Statistics
- tell us where to look for activation that is
related to our paradigm - help us decide how likely it is that activation
is real
The lies and damned lies come in when you write
the manuscript
6Predicted Responses
- fMRI is based on the Blood Oxygenation Level
Dependent (BOLD) response - It takes about 5 sec for the blood to catch up
with the brain - We can model the predicted activation in one of
two ways - shift the boxcar by approximately 5 seconds (2
images x 2 seconds/image 4 sec, close enough) - convolve the boxcar with the hemodynamic response
to model the shape of the true function as well
as the delay
PREDICTED ACTIVATION IN OBJECT AREA
PREDICTED ACTIVATION IN VISUAL AREA
BOXCAR
7Types of Errors
p value probability of a Type I error e.g., p
lt.05 There is less than a 5 probability that a
voxel our stats have declared as active is in
reality NOT active
Slide modified from Duke course
8Statistical Approaches in a Nutshell
- t-tests
- compare activation levels between two conditions
- use a time-shift to account for hemodynamic lag
- correlations
- model activation and see whether any areas show a
similar pattern
- Fourier analysis
- Do a Fourier analysis to see if there is energy
at your paradigm frequency
Fourier analysis images from Huettel, Song
McCarthy, 2004, Functional Magnetic Resonance
Imaging
9Effect of Thresholds
r 0 0 of variance p lt 1
10Complications
- Not only is it hard to determine whats real, but
there are all sorts of statistical problems
- Potential problems
- data may be contaminated by artifacts (e.g., head
motion, breathing artifacts) - .05 49,152 2457 significant voxels by
chance alone - many assumptions of statistics (adjacent voxels
uncorrelated with each other adjacent time
points uncorrelated with one another) are false
Whats wrong with these data?
r .24 6 of variance p lt .05
11The General Linear Model (GLM)
- GLM definition from Huettel et al.
- a class of statistical tests that assume that the
experimental data are composed of the linear
combination of different model factors, along
with uncorrelated noise - Model
- statistical model
- Linear
- things add up sensibly (11 2)
- note that linearity refers to the predictors in
the model and not necessarily the BOLD signal - General
- many simpler statistical procedures such as
correlations, t-tests and ANOVAs are subsumed by
the GLM
12Benefits of the GLM
- GLM is an overarching tool that can do anything
that the simpler tests do - allows any combination of contrasts (e.g., intact
- scrambled, scrambled - baseline), unlike
simpler methods (correlations, t-tests, Fourier
analyses) - allows more complex designs (e.g., factorial
designs) - allows much greater flexibility for combining
data within subjects and between subjects - allows comparisons between groups
- allows counterbalancing orders within and between
subjects - allows modelling of known sources of noise in the
data (e.g., error trials, head motion)
13Part 2
- Composition of a Voxel Time Course
14A Simple Experiment
- Lateral Occipital Complex
- responds when subject views objects
Blank Screen
Intact Objects
Scrambled Objects
TIME
One volume (12 slices) every 2 seconds for 272
seconds (4 minutes, 32 seconds) Condition
changes every 16 seconds (8 volumes)
15Whats real?
A.
C.
B.
D.
16Whats real?
- I created each of those time courses based by
taking the predictor function and adding a
variable amount of random noise
signal
noise
17Whats real?
Which of the data sets below is more convincing?
18Formal Statistics
- Formal statistics are just doing what your
eyeball test of significance did - Estimate how likely it is that the signal is real
given how noisy the data is - confidence how likely is it that the results
could occur purely due to chance? - p value probability value
- If p .03, that means there is a .03/1 or 3
chance that the results are bogus - By convention, if the probability that a result
could be due to chance is less than 5 (p lt .05),
we say that result is statistically significant - Significance depends on
- signal (differences between conditions)
- noise (other variability)
- sample size (more time points are more
convincing)
19Lets create a time course for one LO voxel
20Well begin with activation
Response to Intact Objects is 4X greater than
Scrambled Objects
21Then well assume that our modelled activation is
off because a transient component
22Our modelled activation could be off for other
reasons
- All of the following could lead to inaccurate
models - different shape of function
- different width of function
- different latency of function
23Reminder Variability of HRF
Intersubject variability of HRF in M1 Handwerker
et al., 2004, NeuroImage
24Now lets add some variability due to head motion
25though really motion is more complex
- Head motion can be quantified with 6 parameters
given in any motion correction algorithm - x translation
- y translation
- z translation
- xy rotation
- xz rotation
- yz rotation
- For simplicity, Ive only included parameter one
in our model - Head motion can lead to other problems not
predictable by these parameters
26Now lets throw in a pinch of linear drift
- linear drift could arise from magnet noise (e.g.,
parts warm up) or physiological noise (e.g.,
subjects head sinks)
27and then well add a dash of low frequency noise
- low frequency noise can arise from magnet noise
or physiological noise (e.g., subjects cycles of
alertness/drowsiness) - low frequency noise would occur over a range of
frequencies but for simplicity, Ive only
included one frequency (1 cycle per run) here - Linear drift is really just very low frequency
noise
28and our last ingredient some high frequency noise
- high frequency noise can arise from magnet noise
or physiological noise (e.g., subjects breathing
rate and heartrate)
29When we add these all together, we get a
realistic time course
30Part 3
31Now lets be the experimenter
- First, we take our time course and normalize it
using z scores - z (x - mean)/SD
- normalization leads to data where
- mean zero
- SD 1
Alternative You can transform the data into
BOLD signal change. This is usually a better
approach because its not dependent on variance
32Wake Up!!!!!
If you only pay attention to one slide in this
lecture, it should be the next one!!!
33We create a GLM with 2 predictors
?1
?2
fMRI Signal
Residuals
Design Matrix
Betas
x
what we CAN explain
what we CANNOT explain
how much of it we CAN explain
x
our data
Statistical significance is basically a ratio of
explained to unexplained variance
34Implementation of GLM in SPM
Many thanks to Øystein Bech Gadmar for creating
this figure in SPM
? Time
- SPM represents time as going down
- SPM represents predictors within the design
matrix as grayscale plots (where black low,
white high) over time - GLM includes a constant to take care of the
average activation level throughout each run - SPM shows this explicity (BV may not)
35Effect of Beta Weights
- Adjustments to the beta weights have the effect
of raising or lowering the height of the
predictor while keeping the shape constant
36Dynamic Example
37The beta weight is NOT a correlation
- correlations measure goodness of fit regardless
of scale - beta weights are a measure of scale
small ß large r
small ß small r
large ß small r
large ß large r
38We create a GLM with 2 predictors
when ?12
when ?20.5
Betas
x
Design Matrix
what we CAN explain
what we CANNOT explain
how much of it we CAN explain
x
our data
Statistical significance is basically a ratio of
explained to unexplained variance
39Correlated Predictors
- Where possible, avoid predictors that are highly
correlated with one another - This is why we NEVER include a baseline predictor
- baseline predictor is almost completely
correlated with the sum of existing predictors
r -.53
r -.53
r -.95
Two stimulus predictors
Baseline predictor
40Which model accounts for this data?
x ß 1
x ß 0
OR
x ß 1
x ß 0
x ß 0
x ß -1
- Because the predictors are highly correlated, the
model is overdetermined and you cant tell which
beta combo is best
41Orthogonalizing Regressors
42Contrasts in the GLM
- We can examine whether a single predictor is
significant (compared to the baseline)
43Contrasts
? balanced
- Conjunction of contrasts
- e.g., (1 -1 0) AND (1 0 -1)
- (Bio motion - Nonbio motion) AND (Bio motion gt
control) - more rigorous than balanced contrast
- hypothetical (but not actual) conjunction p
value multiple of independent p values - e.g., .01 x .01 .001
44A Real Voxel
- Heres the time course from a voxel that was
significant in the Intact -Scrambled comparison
45Maximizing Your Power
signal
noise
- As we saw earlier, the GLM is basically comparing
the amount of signal to the amount of noise - How can we improve our stats?
- increase signal
- decrease noise
- increase sample size (keep subject in longer)
46How to Reduce Noise
- If you cant get rid of an artifact, you can
include it as a predictor of no interest to
soak up variance
Example Some people include predictors from the
outcome of motion correction algorithms
Corollary Never leave out predictors for
conditions that will affect your data (e.g.,
error trials)
This works best when the motion is uncorrelated
with your paradigm (predictors of interest)
47Reducing Residuals
48Part 3
- Deconvolution of Event-Related Designs
- Using the GLM
49Convolution of Single Trials
Neuronal Activity
BOLD Signal
Haemodynamic Function
Time
Time
Slide from Matt Brown
50Fast fMRI Detection
Slide from Matt Brown
51DEconvolution of Single Trials
Neuronal Activity
BOLD Signal
Haemodynamic Function
Time
Time
Slide from Matt Brown
52Deconvolution Example
- time course from 4 trials of two types (pink,
blue) in a jittered design
53Summed Activation
54Single Stick Predictor
- single predictor for first volume of pink trial
type
55Predictors for Pink Trial Type
- set of 12 predictors for subsequent volumes of
pink trial type - need enough predictors to cover unfolding of HRF
(depends on TR)
56Predictor Matrix
. . .
57Predictors for the Blue Trial Type
- set of 12 predictors for subsequent volumes of
blue trial type
58Predictor x Beta Weights for Pink Trial Type
- sequence of beta weights for one trial type
yields an estimate of the average activation
(including HRF)
59Predictor x Beta Weights for Blue Trial Type
- height of beta weights indicates amplitude of
response (higher betas larger response)
60Linear Deconvolution
Miezen et al. 2000
- Jittering ITI also preserves linear independence
among the hemodynamic components comprising the
BOLD signal.
61Fast fMRI Estimation
- Pros
- Produces time course
- Does not assume specific shape for hemodynamic
function - Robust against trial history biases (though not
immune to it) - Compound trial types possible
- Cons
- Complicated
- Unrealistic assumptions about linearity if trials
are too close in time - BOLD is non-linear with inter-event intervals lt 6
sec. - Nonlinearity becomes severe under 2 sec.
- Sensitive to noise
62Part 4
- Dealing with Faulty Assumptions
63Whats this ing reviewer complaining about?!
- Particularly if you do voxelwise stats, you have
to be careful to follow the accepted standards of
the field. In the past few years the following
approaches have been recommended by the stats
mavens - Correction for multiple comparisons
- Random effects analyses
- Correction for serial correlations
64Dead Salmon
poster at Human Brain Mapping conference, 2009
- 130,000 voxels
- no correction for multiple comparisons
65Fishy Headlines
66Correction for Multiple Comparisons
With conventional probability levels (e.g., p lt
.05) and a huge number of comparisons (e.g., 64 x
64 x 12 49,152), a lot of voxels will be
significant purely by chance e.g., .05 49,152
2458 voxels significant due to chance How can
we avoid this?
- Bonferroni correction
- divide desired p value by number of comparisons
- Example
- desired p value p lt .05
- number of voxels 50,000
- required p value p lt .05 / 50,000 ? p lt
.000001 - quite conservative
- can use less stringent values
- e.g., Brain Voyager can use the number of voxels
in the cortical surface - small volume correction use more liberal
thresholds in areas of the brain which you
expected to be active
67Correction for Multiple Comparisons
- Gaussian random field theory
- Fundamental to SPM
- If data are very smooth, then the chance of noise
points passing threshold is reduced - Can correct for the number of resolvable
elements (resels) rather than number of voxels
Slide modified from Duke course
68- Cluster correction
- falsely activated voxels should be randomly
dispersed - set minimum cluster size to be large enough to
make it unlikely that a cluster of that size
would occur by chance - some algorithms assume that data from adjacent
voxels are uncorrelated (not true) - some algorithms (e.g., Brain Voyager) estimate
and factor in spatial smoothness of maps - cluster threshold may differ for different
contrasts
- Test-retest reliability
- Perform statistical tests on each half of the
data - The probability of a given voxel appearing in
both purely by chance is the square of the p
value used in each half - e.g., .001 x .001 .000001
- Alternatively, use the first half to select an
ROI and evaluate your hypothesis in the second
half.
69- False discovery rate (Genovese et al, 2002,
NeuroImage) - controls the proportion of rejected hypotheses
that are falsely rejected - standard p value (e.g., p lt .01) means that a
certain proportion of all voxels will be
significant by chance (1) - FDR uses q value (e.g., q lt .01), meaning that a
certain proportion of the activated (colored)
voxels will be significant by chance (1) - works in theory, though in practice, my lab
hasnt been that satisfied
Is the region truly active?
Yes
No
Type I Error
HIT
Yes
Does our stat test indicate that the region is
active?
Type II Error
Correct Rejection
No
70- 6) Poor mans Bonferroni
- Jack up the threshold till you get rid of the
schmutz (especially in air, ventricles, white
matter) - If you have a comparison where one condition is
expected to produce much more activity than the
other, turn on both tails of the comparison - Jodys rule of thumb If ya cant trust the
negatives, can ya trust the positives?
Example MT localizer data Moving rings gt
stationary rings (orange) Stationary rings gt
moving rings (blue)
71Correction for Temporal Correlations
Statistical methods assume that each of our time
points is independent. In the case of fMRI, this
assumption is false. Even in a screen saver
scan, activation in a voxel at one time is
correlated with its activation within 6
sec This fact can artificially inflate your
statistical significance.
72Autocorrelation function
To calculate the magnitude of the problem, we can
compute the autocorrelation function For a voxel
or ROI, correlate its time course with itself
shifted in time Plot these correlations by the
degree of shift
original
73BV can correct for the autocorrelation to yield
revised (usually lower) p values
BEFORE
AFTER
74BV Preprocessing Options
75Temporal Smoothing of Data
- We have the option in our software to temporally
smooth our data (i.e., remove high temporal
frequencies) - However, I recommended that you not use this
option - Now do you understand why?
76Clarification
- correction for temporal correlations is NOT
necessary with random effects analyses, only for
fixed effects and individual subjects analysis
77Collapsed Fixed Effects Models
- assume that the experimental manipulation has
same effect in each subject - treats all data as one concatenated set with one
beta per predictor (collapsed across all
subjects) - e.g., Intact 2
- Scrambled .5
- strong effect in one subject can lead to
significance even when others show weak or no
effects - you can say that effect was significant in your
group of subjects but cannot generalize to other
subjects that you didnt test
78Separate Subjects Models
- one beta per predictor per subject
- e.g., JC Intact 2.1
- JC Scrambled 0.2
- DQ Intact 1.5
- DQ Scrambled 1.0
- KV Intact 1.2
- KV Scrambled 1.3
- weights each subject equally
- makes data less susceptible to effects of one
rogue subject
79Random Effects Analysis
- Typical fMRI stats test whether the differences
between conditions are significant in the sample
of subjects we have tested - Often, we want to be able to generalize to the
population as a whole including all potential
subjects, not just the ones we tested - Random effects analyses allow you to generalize
to the population you tested - Brain Voyager recommends you dont even toy with
random effects unless youve got 10 or more
subjects (and 50 is best) - Random effects analyses can really squash your
data, especially if you dont have many subjects.
Sometimes we refer to the random effects button
as the make my activation go away button. - Though standards were lower in the early days of
fMRI, today its virtually impossible to publish
any group voxelwise data without random effects
analysis - You dont have to worry about it if youre using
the ROI approach because (1) presumably the ROI
has already been well-established across multiple
labs and (2) posthoc analyses of results in an
ROI approach allow you to generalize to the
population (assuming you include individual
variance)
underpaid graduate students in need of a few
bucks!
80Fixed vs. Random Effects GLM
Sample Data 1
Sample Data 2
Subject Intact beta Scram beta Diff
1 4 3 1
2 2 3 -1
3 4 1 3
SUM 10 7 3
Subject Intact beta Scram beta Diff
1 4 3 1
2 2 1 1
3 4 3 1
SUM 10 7 3
- Fixed Effects GLM cannot tell the difference
between these data sets because (Intact sum -
Scram sum) is the same in both cases - In Random Effects GLM, Data set 1 would be more
likely to be significant because all 3 subjects
show a trend in the same direction (intact gt
scrambled), whereas in data set 2, only 2 of 3
subjects show a difference in that direction
81Strategies for Exploration vs. Publication
- Deductive approach
- Have a specific hypothesis/contrast planned
- Run all your subjects
- Run the stats as planned
- Publish
- Inductive approach
- Run a few subjects to see if youre on the right
track - Spend a lot of time exploring the pilot data for
interesting patterns - Find the story in the data
- You may even change the experiment, run
additional subjects, or run a follow-up
experiment to chase the story
- While you need to use rigorous corrections for
publication, do not be overly conservative when
exploring pilot data or you might miss
interesting trends - Random effects analyses can be quite
conservative so you may want to do exploratory
analyses with fixed effects (and then run more
subjects if needed so you can publish random
effects)
82Part 4
- To Localize or Not to Localise?
83To Localize or Not to Localise?
84Methodological Fundamentalism
The latest review I received
85Approach 1 Voxelwise Statistics
- You dont necessarily need a priori hypotheses
(though sometimes you can use less conservative
stats if you have them) - Average all of your data together in Talairach
space - Compare two (or more) conditions using precise
statistical procedures within every voxel of the
brain. Any area that passes a carefully
determined threshold is considered real. - Make a list of these areas and publish it.
This is the tricky part!
86Voxelwise Approach Example
- Malach et al., 1995, PNAS
- Question Are there areas of the human brain that
are more responsive to objects than scrambled
objects - You will recognize this as what we now call an LO
localizer, but Malach was the first to identify LO
LO (red) responds more to objects, abstract
sculptures and faces than to textures, unlike
visual cortex (blue) which responds well to all
stimuli
LO activation is shown in red, behind MT
activation in green
87The Danger of Voxelwise Approaches
- This is one of two tables from a paper
- Some papers publish tables of activation two
pages long - How can anyone make sense of so many areas?
Source Decety et al., 1994, Nature
88Approach 2 Region of interest (ROI) analysis
- If you are looking at a well-established area
(such as visual cortex, motor cortex, or the
lateral occipital complex), its fairly easy to
activate and identify the area - Do the stats and play with the threshold till you
get something believable in the right vicinity
based on anatomical location (e.g., sulcal
landmarks) or functional location (e.g.,
Talairach coordinates from prior studies) - Once you have found the ROI, do independent
experiments, extract the time course information
and determine whether activation differences
between conditions are significant - Because the runs that are used to generate the
area are independent from those used to test the
hypothesis, liberal statistics (p lt .05) can be
used
89Example of ROI Approach
Culham et al., 2003, Experimental Brain
Research Does the Lateral Occipital Complex
compute object shape for grasping?
Step 1 Localize LOC
Intact Objects
Scrambled Objects
90Example of ROI Approach
Culham et al., 2003, Experimental Brain
Research Does the Lateral Occipital Complex
compute object shape for grasping?
Step 2 Extract LOC data from experimental runs
Grasping
Reaching
NS p .35
NS p .31
91Example of ROI Approach
Very Simple Stats
BOLD Signal Change Left Hem. LOC BOLD Signal Change Left Hem. LOC
Subject Grasping Reaching
1 0.02 0.03
2 0.19 0.08
3 0.04 0.01
4 0.10 0.32
5 1.01 -0.27
6 0.16 0.09
7 0.19 0.12
Then simply do a paired t-test to see whether the
peaks are significantly different between
conditions
Extract average peak from each subject for each
condition
NS p .35
NS p .31
- Instead of using BOLD Signal Change, you can
use beta weights - You can also do a planned contrast in Brain
Voyager using a module called the ROI GLM
92Utility of Doing Both Approaches
- We also verified the result with a voxelwise
approach
Verification of no LOC activation for grasping gt
reaching even at moderate threshold (p lt .001,
uncorrected)
93Example The Danger of ROI Approaches
- Example 1 LOC may be a heterogeneous area with
subdivisions ROI analyses gloss over this - Example 2 Some experiments miss important areas
(e.g., Kanwisher et al., 1997 identified one
important face processing area -- the fusiform
face area, FFA -- but did not report a second
area that is a very important part of the face
processing network -- the occipital face area,
OFA -- because it was less robust and consistent
than the FFA.
94Comparing the two approaches
- Voxelwise Analyses
- Require no prior hypotheses about areas involved
- Include entire brain
- Often neglect individual differences
- Can lose spatial resolution with intersubject
averaging - Can produce meaningless laundry lists of areas
that are difficult to interpret - You have to be fairly stats-savvy and include all
the appropriate statistical corrections to be
certain your activation is really significant - Popular in Europe
95Comparing the two approaches
- Region of Interest (ROI) Analyses
- Extraction of ROI data can be subjected to simple
stats (no need for multiple comparisons,
autocorrelation or random effects corrections) - Gives you more statistical power (e.g., p lt .05)
- Hypothesis-driven
- Useful when hypotheses are motivated by other
techniques (e.g., electrophysiology) in specific
brain regions - ROI is not smeared due to intersubject averaging
- Important for discriminating abutting areas
(e.g., V1/V2) - Easy to analyze and interpret
- Neglects other areas which may play a fundamental
role - If multiple ROIs need to be considered, you can
spend a lot of scan time collecting localizer
data (thus limiting the time available for
experimental runs) - Works best for reliable and robust areas with
unambiguous definitions - Popular in North America
96A Proposed Resolution
- There is no reason not to do BOTH ROI analyses
and voxelwise analyses - ROI analyses for well-defined key regions
- Voxelwise analyses to see if other regions are
also involved - Ideally, the conclusions will not differ
- If the conclusions do differ, there may be
sensible reasons - Effect in ROI but not voxelwise
- perhaps region is highly variable in stereotaxic
location between subjects - perhaps voxelwise approach is not powerful enough
- Effect in voxelwise but not ROI
- perhaps ROI is not homogenous or is
context-specific
97Part 5
- The War of Non-Independence
98Finding the Obvious
A priori probability of getting JQKA sequence
(1/13)4 1/28,561 A posteriori probability of
getting JQKA sequence 1/1 100
- Non-independence error
- occurs when statistical tests performed are not
independent from the means used to select the
brain region
Arguments from Vul Kanwisher, book chapter in
press
99Non-independence Error
- Egregious example
- Identify Area X with contrast of A gt B
- Do post hoc stats showing that A is statistically
higher than B - Act surprised
- More subtle example of selection bias
- Identify Area X with contrast of A gt B
- Do post hoc stats showing that A is statistically
higher than C and C is statistically greater than
B
Arguments from Vul Kanwisher, book chapter in
press Figure from Kriegeskorte et al., 2009,
Nature Neuroscience
100Double Dipping How to Avoid It
- Kriegeskorte et al., 2009, Nature Neuroscience
- surveyed 134 papers in prestiguous journals
- 42 showed at least one example of
non-independence error
101Correlations Between Individual Subjects Brain
Activity and Behavioral Measures
- Sample of Critiqued Papers
- Eisenberg, Lieberman Williams, 2003, Science
- measured fMRI activity during social rejection
- correlated self-reported distress with brain
activity - found r .88 in anterior cingulate cortex, an
area implicated in physical pain perception - concluded rejection hurts
social exclusion gt inclusion
102Voodoo Correlations
The original title of the paper was not
well-received by reviewers so it was changed even
though some people still use the term
Voodoo
2009
- reliability of personality and emotion measures
r .7 - reliability of activation in a given voxel r
.7 - highest expected behavior fMRI correlation is
.74 - so how can we have behavior fMRI correlations
of r .9?!
103Voodoo Correlations
"Notably, 53 of the surveyed studies selected
voxels based on a correlation with the behavioral
individual-differences measure and then used
those same data to compute a correlation within
that subset of voxels."
Vul et al., 2009, Perspectives on Psychological
Science
104Avoiding Voodoo
- Use independent means to select region and then
evaluate correlation - Do split-half reliability test
- WARNING This is reassuring that the result can
be replicated in your sample but does not
demonstrate that result generalizes to the
population
105Is the voodoo problem all that bad?
- High correlations can occur in legitimately
analyzed data - Did voxelwise analyses use appropriate correction
for multiple comparisons? - then result is statistically significant
regardless of specific correlation - Is additional data being used for
- inference purposes?
- if they pretend to provide independent support,
thats bad - presentation purposes?
- alternative formats can be useful in
demonstrating that data is clean (e.g., time
courses look sensible correlations are not
driven by outliers)