Title: Students
1Students Understanding of Human Nature An
Analogical Approach
R. BROCK FROST AND ERIC AMSEL Weber State
University
- Abstract
- Psychology, Science and Humanities students were
interviewed to assess their judgments of how
human beings, animals (great apes), and machines
(computers) were alike. Science students were
more likely than others to judge that human
beings and animals were similar, but Psychology
students were more likely to justify their
judgments with relational analogies than literal
similarities.
- Methods
- Participants
- Students were initially screened to assess their
academic background. Those with appropriate
backgrounds were then contacted and offered 5.00
to participate in the study. The sample consisted
of 30 (15M and 15F) seniors or juniors who were
majoring in Psychology (N10), Natural Science
(N10 3 Physics, 3 Microbiology, and 4 Zoology),
or Arts and Humanities (N10 8 English, 1
History, and 1 Communication). - Interview
- Participants judgments and justifications of the
similarity of humans and animals and humans and
machines were assessed in an interview format.
The interview began with general questions
regarding the similarity between humans and
animals or machines. Other questions were also
posed, but only the results from the general
questions are reported. Participants were asked,
To what extent are human beings similar to
animals (e.g., great apes)/machines (e.g.,
computers)? Participants recorded their ratings
on a 7-point scale, anchored by Not at all
Similar (1), A Little Similar (2), Somewhat Alike
(3) Moderately Similar (4), A Good Deal Alike
(5), Very Alike (6), Identical (7). - Participants were probed about their ratings of
entity similarity with follow-up questions such
as, Why do you say that they are _________?,
What makes them ______?, and What exactly are
you saying is alike about humans and _______?
The probes were designed to elicit justifications
for participants similarity ratings. The
justifications were coded as literal similarity
(elements of one entity are also found in the
other) or relational analogy (relations between
elements in one entity are also found in another)
based on Gentner Woolf (2000). An intermediate
code was also established for cases with elements
of each justification (partial analogy). - There were two orders of presenting the
interview. Half the participants in each group
were first posed questions addressing
human/animal similarities and the other half were
posed questions addressing human/machine
similarities.
- Results
- Justifications were coded on an interval scale,
with Literal Similarity coded as 1, Partial
Analogy as 2, and Relational Analogy as 3 (see
Table 1). Inter-rater reliability based on all
the responses of 30 of the participants was 96. - Two analyses tested the prediction that
Psychology students would be more likely than
others to judge commonalities between entities
based on relational analogies. - The first analysis was a 3 (Groups) by 2
(Entities) ANCOVA on similarity ratings with sex
and order as covariates. There was a main Groups
effect which approached significance,
F(2,25)3.17, p.059. Science students (M4.77)
judged greater overall similarity between
entities than did Humanities students (M3.43),
with Psychology students (M3.95) no different
from the other groups. The main effect was
modulated by a Groups by Entity interaction
effect which also approached significance,
F(2,25)2.85, p.076. As shown in Table 2,
Science students judged greater similarity than
Humanities and Psychology students in the Animal
condition, F(2,25)6.58, plt.01, but there was no
Groups difference in the Machine condition,
F(2,25).33, ns. - The second analysis was a 3 (Groups) by 2
(Entity) ANCOVA on justification responses with
sex and order as covariates. There was a main
Group effect, F(2,25)6.75, plt.01. Psychology
students (M2.10) had higher justification
responses than did Humanities students (M1.31),
with Science students (M1.89) no different from
the other groups. There was a significant Group
by Entity interaction effect, F(2,25) 6.31,
plt.01 (see Table 3). Psychology students had
higher justification responses than Humanities
and Science students in the Animal condition,
F(2,25)10.94, plt.001. Psychology and Science
students had higher justifications responses
compared to Humanities students, in the Machine
Condition, an effect which approached
significance F(2,25)3.03, p.066
- Discussion
- It was predicted that compared to others,
Psychology students would be more likely to judge
commonalities between humans, animals, and
machines based on relational analogies than
literal similarities. - The prediction was confirmed most strongly for
Human/Animal comparisons. Although, compared to
Science students, Psychology students judged less
similarity between Animals and Humans, they
justified their similarity judgments with
relational analogies more so than others. - Such a pattern may reflect Amsel et al.s (2005)
claim that Psychology majors continue to hold
onto the tenets of Folk psychology, with its
assumption of the uniqueness of human beings,
despite adopting those of Scientific psychology
with its relationally-based continuity between
human beings, animals, and computers. We are
continuing to collect data freshmen and
sophomores motivated to major in Psychology to
confirm this analysis. We predict that would-be
psychology majors will be less inclined than
advanced Psychology students to judge humans and
animals as similar and justify their judgments
with relational analogies.
Table 1 Justifications of Similarity Ratings 1.
Literal Similaritya. Animals and humans are both
bipedal, have hands.b. Humans and computers
dont work the way they are supposed to. 2.
Partial Analogya. Both humans and computers
solve problems, a machine uses a set....mechanism
while a human just tries different stuffb. Both
have gone through evolution, have opposable
thumbs, and similar social systems. 3.
Relational Analogya. The brain is
compartmentalized, different areas do different
stuff, kinda like computer programs.b. Humans
and apes have anatomical structures with similar
functions.
Introduction University students enter
psychology classes with an intuitive theory about
the nature of the discipline (Amsel et al.,
2005). This intuitive theory (called Folk
Psychology) is an inherently unscientific account
of behavior in terms of conscious mental states
(DAndrade, 1987). Folk Psychology is
inconsistent with Scientific Psychology, which is
an account of behavior based on the influence of
genetic, biological, cognitive, and sociocultural
forces. Amsel et al. (2005) found that Psychology
students more strongly distinguish between the
two theories than do others, embracing the tenets
of Scientific Psychology without rejecting those
of Folk Psychology. They propose that the two
forms of explanation conceptually coexist.
Following up on the previous work, the present
study explores how Psychology and other students
think about how human beings are like animals
(i.e., great apes) and machines (i.e.,
computers). In Scientific Psychology such
commonalties are based more on relational
analogies than literal similarities. For example,
beyond any literal similarity, human beings, like
computers, are thought to compute, store, and
retrieve information. Similarly, humans, like
animals, are thought to evolve species-specific
behavior which may not be literally similar. It
was hypothesized that compared to others,
Psychology students would be more likely to judge
commonalities between humans, animals, and
machines based on relational analogies than
literal similarities.
- References
- Amsel, E., Anderson, C., Corbin, P. (April,
2005). Conceptual change in psychology
majors understanding of the discipline. Poster
presented at RMPA, Phoenix AZ. - Carey, S. (2000). Science education as conceptual
change. Journal of Applied Developmental
Psychology, 21, 13-19. - D'Andrade, R. G. (1987). A folk model of the
mind. In D. Holland and N. Quinn (Eds.) Cultural
Models in Language and Though (pp. 112- 148).
Cambridge UK Cambridge University Press. - Gentner, D. Wolff, P. (2000). Metaphor and
knowledge change. In E. Dietrich A. Markman
(Eds.), Cognitive dynamics Conceptual change in
humans and machines (pp. 295-342). Mahwah, NJ
Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. - Nersessian, N. J. (1989). Conceptual change in
science and in science education. Synthèse, 80,
163-183.