BIAS AND CONFOUNDING - PowerPoint PPT Presentation

About This Presentation
Title:

BIAS AND CONFOUNDING

Description:

BIAS AND CONFOUNDING Nigel Paneth HYPOTHESIS FORMULATION AND ERRORS IN RESEARCH All analytic studies must begin with a clearly formulated hypothesis. – PowerPoint PPT presentation

Number of Views:237
Avg rating:3.0/5.0
Slides: 28
Provided by: Epidemiology
Learn more at: http://www.bibalex.org
Category:

less

Transcript and Presenter's Notes

Title: BIAS AND CONFOUNDING


1
BIAS AND CONFOUNDING
  • Nigel Paneth

2
HYPOTHESIS FORMULATION AND ERRORS IN RESEARCH
  • All analytic studies must begin with a clearly
    formulated hypothesis. The hypothesis must be
    quantitative and specific. It must predict a
    relationship of a specific size.

3
  • For example
  •   Babies who are breast-fed have less illness
    than babies who are bottle-fed.
  •  
  • Which illnesses? How is feeding type defined?
    How large a difference in risk?
  • A better example
  •   Babies who are exclusively breast-fed for
    three months or more will have a reduction in the
    incidence of hospital admissions for
    gastroenteritis of at least 30 over the first
    year of life.

4
  • Only specific prediction allows one to draw
    legitimate conclusions from a study which tests a
    hypothesis. But even with the best formulated
    hypothesis, two types of errors can occur.
  •  
  • Type 1 - observing a difference when in truth
    there is none.
  •  
  • Type 2 - failing to observe a difference when
    there is one.

5
  • These errors are generally produced by one or
    more of the following
  •  
  • RANDOM ERROR
  • RANDOM MISCLASSIFICATION
  • BIAS
  • CONFOUNDING

6
RANDOM ERROR
  • Deviation of results and inferences from the
    truth, occurring only as a result of the
    operation of chance. Can produce type 1 or type 2
    errors.

7
RANDOM (OR NON-DIFFERENTIAL) MISCLASSIFICATION
  • Random error applied to the measurement of an
    exposure or outcome. Errors in classification
    can only produce type 2 errors, except if applied
    to a confounder or to an exposure gradient.

8
BIAS
  • Systematic, non-random deviation of results and
    inferences from the truth, or processes leading
    to such deviation. Any trend in the collection,
    analysis, interpretation, publication or review
    of data that can lead to conclusions which are
    systematically different from the truth.
    (Dictionary of Epidemiology, 3rd ed.)
  •  

9
MORE ON BIAS
  • Note that in bias, the focus is on an artifact
    of some part of the research process (assembling
    subjects, collecting data, analyzing data) that
    produces a spurious result. Bias can produce
    either a type 1 or a type 2 error, but we usually
    focus on type 1 errors due to bias.

10
MORE ON BIAS
  • Bias can be either conscious or unconscious. In
    epidemiology, the word bias does not imply, as in
    common usage, prejudice or deliberate deviation
    from the truth.

11
CONFOUNDING
  • A problem resulting from the fact that one
    feature of study subjects has not been separated
    from a second feature, and has thus been
    confounded with it, producing a spurious result.
    The spuriousness arises from the effect of the
    first feature being mistakenly attributed to the
    second feature. Confounding can produce either a
    type 1 or a type 2 error, but we usually focus on
    type 1 errors.

12
THE DIFFERENCE BETWEEN BIAS AND CONFOUNDING
  • Bias creates an association that is not true,
    but confounding describes an association that is
    true, but potentially misleading.

13
EXAMPLES OF RANDOM ERROR, BIAS, MISCLASSIFICATION
AND CONFOUNDING IN THE SAME STUDY
  • STUDY In a cohort study, babies of women who
    bottle feed and women who breast feed are
    compared, and it is found that the incidence of
    gastroenteritis, as recorded in medical records,
    is lower in the babies who are breast-fed.

14
EXAMPLE OF RANDOM ERROR
  • By chance, there are more episodes of
    gastroenteritis in the bottle-fed group in the
    study sample, producing a type 1 error. (When in
    truth breast feeding is not protective against
    gastroenteritis).
  • Or, also by chance, no difference in risk was
    found, producing a type 2 error (When in truth
    breast feeding is protective against
    gastroenteritis).

15
EXAMPLE OF RANDOM MISCLASSIFICATION
  • Lack of good information on feeding history
    results in some breast-feeding mothers being
    randomly classified as bottle-feeding, and
    vice-versa. If this happens, the study finding
    underestimates the true RR, whichever feeding
    modality is associated with higher disease
    incidence, producing a type 2 error.

16
EXAMPLE OF BIAS
  • The medical records of bottle-fed babies only are
    less complete (perhaps bottle fed babies go to
    the doctor less) than those of breast fed babies,
    and thus record fewer episodes of
    gastro-enteritis in them only.
  • This is called ias because the observation itself
    is in error.

17
EXAMPLE OF CONFOUNDING
  • The mothers of breast-fed babies are of higher
    social class, and the babies thus have better
    hygiene, less crowding and perhaps other factors
    that protect against gastroenteritis. Crowding
    and hygiene are truly protective against
    gastroenteritis, but we mistakenly attribute
    their effects to breast feeding. This is called
    confounding. because the observation is correct,
    but its explanation is wrong.

18
PROTECTION AGAINST RANDOM ERROR AND RANDOM
MISCLASSIFICATION
  • Random error can work to falsely produce an
    association (type 1 error) or falsely not produce
    an association (type 2 error).
  • We protect ourselves against random
    misclassification producing a type 2 error by
    choosing the most precise and accurate measures
    of exposure and outcome.

19
PROTECTION AGAINST TYPE 1 ERRORS
  • We protect our study against random type 1
    errors by establishing that the result must be
    unlikely to have occurred by chance (e.g. p lt
    .05). P-values are established
    entirely to protect against type 1 errors due to
    chance, and do not guarantee protection against
    type 1 errors due to bias or confounding. This is
    the reason we say statistics demonstrate
    association but not causation.

20
PROTECTION AGAINST TYPE 2 ERRORS
  • We protect our study against random type 2
    errors by
  • providing adequate sample size, and
  • hypothesizing large differences.
  • The larger the sample size, the easier it will
    be to detect a true difference, and the largest
    differences will be the easiest to detect.
    (Imagine how hard it would be to detect a 1
    increase in the risk of gastroenteritis with
    bottle-feeding).

21
TWO WAYS TO INCREASE POWER
  • The sample size needed to detect a significant
    difference is called the power of a study.
  •  Choosing the most precise and accurate measures
    of exposure and outcome has the effect of
    increasing the power of our study, because of
    variances of the outcome measures, which enter
    into statistical testing, are decreased.
  • Having an adequate sized sample of study subjects
  •  

22
KEY PRINCIPLE IN BIAS AND CONFOUNDING
  • The factor that creates the bias, or the
    confounding variable, must be associated with
    both the independent and dependent variables
    (i.e. with the exposure and the disease).
    Association of the bias or confounder with just
    one of the two variables is not enough to produce
    a spurious result.

23
  • In the example just given
  •  
  • The BIAS, namely incomplete chart recording, has
    to be associated with feeding type (the
    independent variable) and also with recording of
    gastroenteritis (the dependent variable) to
    produce the false result.
  •  
  • The CONFOUNDING VARIABLE (or CONFOUNDER) better
    hygiene, has to be associated with feeding type
    and also with gastroenteritis to produce the
    spurious result.

24
  • Were the bias or the confounder associated with
    just the independent variable or just the
    dependent variable, they would not produce bias
    or confounding.
  • This gives a useful rule
  • If you can show that a potential confounder is
    NOT associated with either one of the two
    variables under study (exposure or outcome),
    confounding can be ruled out.

25
GOOD STUDY DESIGN PROTECTS AGAINST ALL FORMS OF
ERROR
26
SOME TYPES OF BIAS
  • 1. SELECTION BIAS
  •  
  • Any aspect of the way subjects are assembled in
    the study that creates a systematic difference
    between the compared populations that is not due
    to the association under study.  

27
  • 2. INFORMATION BIAS
  • Any aspect of the way information is collected
    in the study that creates a systematic difference
    between the compared populations that is not due
    to the association under study. (some call this
    measurement bias). The incomplete chart
    recording in the baby feeding example would be a
    form of information bias.
  • Other examples -
  • Diagnostic suspicion bias
  • Recall bias
  •  Sometimes biases apply to a population of
    studies, rather than to one study, as in
    publication bias (tendency to publish papers
    which show positive results).
Write a Comment
User Comments (0)
About PowerShow.com