Sources of Bias in Randomised Controlled Trials - PowerPoint PPT Presentation

About This Presentation
Title:

Sources of Bias in Randomised Controlled Trials

Description:

Sources of Bias in Randomised Controlled Trials David Torgerson Director, York Trials Unit djt6_at_york.ac.uk www.rcts.org Selection Bias - A reminder Selection bias is ... – PowerPoint PPT presentation

Number of Views:102
Avg rating:3.0/5.0
Slides: 48
Provided by: ssolm
Category:

less

Transcript and Presenter's Notes

Title: Sources of Bias in Randomised Controlled Trials


1
Sources of Bias in Randomised Controlled Trials
  • David Torgerson
  • Director, York Trials Unit
  • djt6_at_york.ac.uk
  • www.rcts.org

2
Selection Bias - A reminder
  • Selection bias is one of the main threats to the
    internal validity of an experiment.
  • Selection bias occurs when participants are
    SELECTED for an intervention on the basis of a
    variable that is associated with outcome.
  • Randomisation or other similar methods abolishes
    selection bias.

3
After Randomisation
  • Once we have randomised participants we eliminate
    selection bias but the validity of the experiment
    can be threatened by other forms of bias, which
    we must guard against.

4
Forms of Bias
  • Subversion Bias
  • Technical Bias
  • Attrition Bias
  • Consent Bias
  • Ascertainment Bias
  • Dilution Bias
  • Recruitment Bias

5
Bias (cont)
  • Resentful demoralisation
  • Delay Bias
  • Chance Bias
  • Hawthorne effect
  • Analytical Bias.

6
Subversion Bias
  • Subversion Bias occurs when a researcher or
    clinician manipulates participant recruitment
    such that groups formed at baseline are NOT
    equivalent.
  • Anecdotal, or qualitative evidence (I.e gossip),
    suggest that this is a widespread phenomenon.
  • Statistically this has been demonstrated as
    having occurred widely.

7
Subversion - qualitative evidence
  • Schulz has described, anecdotally, a number of
    incidents of researchers subverting allocation by
    looking at sealed envelopes through x-ray lights.
  • Researchers have confessed to breaking open
    filing cabinets to obtain the randomisation code.

Schulz JAMA 19952741456.
8
Quantitative Evidence
  • Trials with adequate concealed allocation show
    different effect sizes, which would not happen if
    allocation wasnt being subverted.
  • Trials using simple randomisation are too
    equivalent for it to have occurred by chance.

9
Poor concealment
  • Schulz et al. Examined 250 RCTs and classified
    them into having adequate concealment (where
    subversion was difficult), unclear, or inadequate
    where subversion was able to take place.
  • They found that badly concealed allocation led to
    increased effect sizes showing CHEATING by
    researchers.

10
Comparison of concealment
Schulz et al. JAMA 1995273408.
11
Case Study
  • Subversion is rarely reported for individual
    studies.
  • One study where it has been reported was for a
    large, multicentred surgical trial.
  • Participants were being randomised to 5 centres
    using sealed envelopes.

12
Case-study (cont)
  • After several hundred participants had been
    allocated the study statistician noticed that
    there was an imbalance in age.
  • This age imbalance was occurring in 3 out of the
    5 centres.
  • Independently 3 clinical researchers were
    subverting the allocation

13
Mean ages of groups
14
Example of Subversion
15
Concealment
  • Both the Schulz and Kjaergard considered sealed
    opaque envelopes to be adequate measures of
    concealment.
  • Envelopes can be subverted by being opened in
    advance.

16
More Evidence
  • Hewitt and colleagues examined the association
    between p values and adequate concealment in 4
    major medical journals.
  • Inadequate concealment largely used opaque
    envelopes.
  • The average p value for inadequately concealed
    trials was 0.022 compared with 0.052 for adequate
    trials (test for difference p 0.045).

Hewitt et al. BMJ2005 March 10th.
17
More Examples
  • Berger has collected 30 case examples of
    potential subversion of the allocation process in
    clinical trials.
  • Because allocation subversion is scientific
    misconduct it is likely that there are many
    other, undetected, cases.

Berger. Selection Bias and Covariate Imbalances
in Randomized Clinical Trials 2005 Wiley,
Chicester.
18
Recent Blocked Trial
  • This was a block randomised study (four patients
    to each block) with separate randomisation at
    each of the three centres. Blocks of four cards
    were produced, each containing two cards marked
    with "nurse" and two marked with "house officer."
    Each card was placed into an opaque envelope and
    the envelope sealed. The block was shuffled and,
    after shuffling, was placed in a box.

Kinley et al., BMJ 2002 3251323.
19
What is wrong here?
Southampton Southampton Sheffield Sheffield Doncaster Doncaster
Doctor Nurse Doctor Nurse Doctor Nurse
500 511 308 319 118 118

Kinley et al., BMJ 3251323.
20
Problem?
  • If block randomisation of 4 were used then each
    centre should not be different by more than 2
    patients in terms of group sizes.
  • Two centres had a numerical disparity of 11.
    Either blocks of 4 were not used or the sequence
    was not followed.

21
Restricted allocation and subversion
  • The drawback with any form of allocation
    restriction is that it allows some prediction.
  • Simple randomisation has no memory of the
    previous allocation. In contrast, blocked
    allocation allows the probability of an
    allocation to be linked to the previous
    allocation.
  • Merely guessing that the next allocation will be
    the opposite of the previous one will result in a
    prediction more accurate than by chance.
  • This can, in theory, allow subversion.

22
Possible subversion
  • In a RCT of rehabilitation for the treatment of
    hip fracture gross baseline imbalances were
    detected favouring the control group.
  • Secure telephone allocation had been used. But
    blocked allocation, size 6, had been used.
  • Exploratory analysis of imbalances suggested
    partially successful prediction of block
    allocation.

Turner J. 2002, Unpublished PhD Thesis,
University of York.
23
Wither restricted allocation?
  • Simple randomisation followed by analysis of
    covariance (ANCOVA) is as efficient as restricted
    randomisation and ANCOVA for sample sizes gt 50.
  • Restricted allocation increases risk of
    prediction and predictability.
  • For large trials simple allocation followed by
    ANCOVA reduces risk of prediction.

Rosenberger WF, Lachin JM. Randomisation in
clinical trials Theory and practice. Wiley
Interscience, 2002, John Wiley and Sons, New York.
24
Subversion - more evidence
  • In a survey of 25 researchers 4 admitted to
    keeping a log of previous allocations to try
    and predict future allocations.

Brown et al. Stats in Medicine, 2005,243715.
25
Testing for subversion
  • Comparison of baseline characteristics may help
    if subversion is suspected. Although this will
    only identify gross subversion.
  • If blocked allocation is used a statistical test
    Bergner-Exner test, may help identify
    subversion.

26
Concealment Recommendations
  • Allocation sequence must be independently
    generated and kept secret from the people who are
    enrolling participants.
  • A secure method of giving allocation to the
    recruiters must be developed, opaque envelopes
    are inadequate.

27
Subversion - summary
  • Appears to be widespread.
  • Secure allocation usually prevents this form of
    bias.
  • Need not be too expensive.
  • Essential to prevent cheating.

28
Secure allocation
  • Can be achieved using telephone allocation from a
    dedicated unit.
  • Can be achieved using independent person to
    undertake allocation.

29
Technical Bias
  • This occurs when the allocation system breaks
    down often due a computer fault.
  • A great example is the COMET I trial (COMET II
    was done because COMET 1 suffered bias).

30
COMET 1
  • A trial of two types of epidural anaesthetics for
    women in labour.
  • The trial was using MIMINISATION via a computer
    programme.
  • The groups were minimised on age of mother and
    her ethnicity.
  • Programme had a fault.

COMET Lancet 200135819.
31
COMET 1 Technical Bias
32
COMET II
  • This new study had to be undertaken and another
    1000 women recruited and randomised.
  • LESSON Always check the balance of your groups
    as you go along if computer allocation is being
    used.

33
Attrition Bias
  • Usually most trials lose participants after
    randomisation. This can cause bias, particularly
    if attrition differs between groups.
  • If a treatment has side-effects this may make
    drop outs higher among the less well
    participants, which can make a treatment appear
    to be effective when it is not.

34
Attrition Bias
  • We can avoid some of the problems with attrition
    bias by using Intention to Treat Analysis, where
    we keep as many of the patients in the study as
    possible even if they are no long on treatment.

35
Selection bias after randomisation
  • Selection bias is avoided if ALL participants who
    are randomised are completely followed up.
  • Often there is some attrition after
    randomisation some refuse to continue to take
    part.
  • Or some may refuse the intervention but can still
    be tracked IMPORTANT to distinguish between
    these.

36
What is wrong here?
37
Ascertainment Bias
  • This occurs when the person reporting the outcome
    can be biased.
  • A particular problem when outcomes are not
    objective and there is uncertainty as to
    whether an event has occurred.
  • Example, of homeopathy study of histamine, showed
    an effect when researchers were not blind to the
    allocation but no effect when they were.
  • Multiple sclerosis treatment appeared to be
    effective when clinicians unblinded but
    ineffective when blinded.

38
Resentful Demoralisation
  • This can occur when participants are randomised
    to treatment they do not want.
  • This may lead to them reporting outcomes badly in
    revenge.
  • This can lead to bias.

39
Resentful Demoralisation
  • One solution is to use a patient preference
    design where only participants who are
    indifferent to the treatment they receive are
    allocated.
  • This should remove its effects.

40
Hawthorne Effect
  • This is an effect that occurs by being part of
    the study rather than the treatment.
    Interventions that require more TLC than controls
    could show an effect due to the TLC than the drug
    or surgical procedure.
  • Placebos largely eliminate this or TLC should be
    given to controls as well.

41
Analytical Bias
  • Once a trial has been completed and data gathered
    in it is still possible to arrive at the wrong
    conclusions by analysing the data incorrectly.
  • Most IMPORTANT is ITT.
  • Also inappropriate sub-group analyses is a common
    practice.

42
Intention To Treat
  • Main analysis of data must be by groups as
    randomised. Per protocol or active treatment
    analysis can lead to a biased result.
  • Those patients not taking the full treatment are
    usually quite different to those that are and
    restricting the analysis can lead to bias.

43
Sub-Group Analyses
  • Once the main analysis has been completed it is
    tempting to look to see if the effect differs by
    group.
  • Is treatment more or less effective in women?
  • Is it better or worse among older people?
  • Is treatment better among people at greater risk?

44
Sub-Groups
  • All of these are legitimate questions. The
    problem is the more subgroups one looks at the
    greater is the chance of finding a spurious
    effect.
  • Sample size estimations and statistical tests are
    based on 1 comparison only.

45
Sub-Group and example.
  • In a large RCT of asprin for myocardial
    infarction a sub-group analysis showed that
    people with the star signs Gemini and Libra
    aspirin was INEFFECTIVE.
  • This is complete NONSENSE!
  • This shows dangers of subgroup analyses.

Lancet 1988ii349-60.
46
Sub groups
  • To avoid spurious findings these should be
    pre-specified and based on a reasonable
    hypothesis.
  • Pre-specification is important avoid data
    dredging as if you torture the data enough it
    will confess.

47
Summary
  • Despite the RCT being the BEST research method
    unless expertly used it can lead to biased
    results.
  • Care must be taken to avoid as many biases as
    possible.
Write a Comment
User Comments (0)
About PowerShow.com