Title: Sources of Bias in Randomised Controlled Trials
1Sources of Bias in Randomised Controlled Trials
- David Torgerson
- Director, York Trials Unit
- djt6_at_york.ac.uk
- www.rcts.org
2Selection Bias - A reminder
- Selection bias is one of the main threats to the
internal validity of an experiment. - Selection bias occurs when participants are
SELECTED for an intervention on the basis of a
variable that is associated with outcome. - Randomisation or other similar methods abolishes
selection bias.
3After Randomisation
- Once we have randomised participants we eliminate
selection bias but the validity of the experiment
can be threatened by other forms of bias, which
we must guard against.
4Forms of Bias
- Subversion Bias
- Technical Bias
- Attrition Bias
- Consent Bias
- Ascertainment Bias
- Dilution Bias
- Recruitment Bias
5Bias (cont)
- Resentful demoralisation
- Delay Bias
- Chance Bias
- Hawthorne effect
- Analytical Bias.
6Subversion Bias
- Subversion Bias occurs when a researcher or
clinician manipulates participant recruitment
such that groups formed at baseline are NOT
equivalent. - Anecdotal, or qualitative evidence (I.e gossip),
suggest that this is a widespread phenomenon. - Statistically this has been demonstrated as
having occurred widely.
7Subversion - qualitative evidence
- Schulz has described, anecdotally, a number of
incidents of researchers subverting allocation by
looking at sealed envelopes through x-ray lights. - Researchers have confessed to breaking open
filing cabinets to obtain the randomisation code.
Schulz JAMA 19952741456.
8Quantitative Evidence
- Trials with adequate concealed allocation show
different effect sizes, which would not happen if
allocation wasnt being subverted. - Trials using simple randomisation are too
equivalent for it to have occurred by chance.
9Poor concealment
- Schulz et al. Examined 250 RCTs and classified
them into having adequate concealment (where
subversion was difficult), unclear, or inadequate
where subversion was able to take place. - They found that badly concealed allocation led to
increased effect sizes showing CHEATING by
researchers.
10Comparison of concealment
Schulz et al. JAMA 1995273408.
11Case Study
- Subversion is rarely reported for individual
studies. - One study where it has been reported was for a
large, multicentred surgical trial. - Participants were being randomised to 5 centres
using sealed envelopes.
12Case-study (cont)
- After several hundred participants had been
allocated the study statistician noticed that
there was an imbalance in age. - This age imbalance was occurring in 3 out of the
5 centres. - Independently 3 clinical researchers were
subverting the allocation
13Mean ages of groups
14Example of Subversion
15Concealment
- Both the Schulz and Kjaergard considered sealed
opaque envelopes to be adequate measures of
concealment. - Envelopes can be subverted by being opened in
advance.
16More Evidence
- Hewitt and colleagues examined the association
between p values and adequate concealment in 4
major medical journals. - Inadequate concealment largely used opaque
envelopes. - The average p value for inadequately concealed
trials was 0.022 compared with 0.052 for adequate
trials (test for difference p 0.045).
Hewitt et al. BMJ2005 March 10th.
17More Examples
- Berger has collected 30 case examples of
potential subversion of the allocation process in
clinical trials. - Because allocation subversion is scientific
misconduct it is likely that there are many
other, undetected, cases.
Berger. Selection Bias and Covariate Imbalances
in Randomized Clinical Trials 2005 Wiley,
Chicester.
18Recent Blocked Trial
- This was a block randomised study (four patients
to each block) with separate randomisation at
each of the three centres. Blocks of four cards
were produced, each containing two cards marked
with "nurse" and two marked with "house officer."
Each card was placed into an opaque envelope and
the envelope sealed. The block was shuffled and,
after shuffling, was placed in a box.
Kinley et al., BMJ 2002 3251323.
19What is wrong here?
Southampton Southampton Sheffield Sheffield Doncaster Doncaster
Doctor Nurse Doctor Nurse Doctor Nurse
500 511 308 319 118 118
Kinley et al., BMJ 3251323.
20Problem?
- If block randomisation of 4 were used then each
centre should not be different by more than 2
patients in terms of group sizes. - Two centres had a numerical disparity of 11.
Either blocks of 4 were not used or the sequence
was not followed.
21Restricted allocation and subversion
- The drawback with any form of allocation
restriction is that it allows some prediction. - Simple randomisation has no memory of the
previous allocation. In contrast, blocked
allocation allows the probability of an
allocation to be linked to the previous
allocation. - Merely guessing that the next allocation will be
the opposite of the previous one will result in a
prediction more accurate than by chance. - This can, in theory, allow subversion.
22Possible subversion
- In a RCT of rehabilitation for the treatment of
hip fracture gross baseline imbalances were
detected favouring the control group. - Secure telephone allocation had been used. But
blocked allocation, size 6, had been used. - Exploratory analysis of imbalances suggested
partially successful prediction of block
allocation.
Turner J. 2002, Unpublished PhD Thesis,
University of York.
23Wither restricted allocation?
- Simple randomisation followed by analysis of
covariance (ANCOVA) is as efficient as restricted
randomisation and ANCOVA for sample sizes gt 50. - Restricted allocation increases risk of
prediction and predictability. - For large trials simple allocation followed by
ANCOVA reduces risk of prediction.
Rosenberger WF, Lachin JM. Randomisation in
clinical trials Theory and practice. Wiley
Interscience, 2002, John Wiley and Sons, New York.
24Subversion - more evidence
- In a survey of 25 researchers 4 admitted to
keeping a log of previous allocations to try
and predict future allocations.
Brown et al. Stats in Medicine, 2005,243715.
25Testing for subversion
- Comparison of baseline characteristics may help
if subversion is suspected. Although this will
only identify gross subversion. - If blocked allocation is used a statistical test
Bergner-Exner test, may help identify
subversion.
26Concealment Recommendations
- Allocation sequence must be independently
generated and kept secret from the people who are
enrolling participants. - A secure method of giving allocation to the
recruiters must be developed, opaque envelopes
are inadequate.
27Subversion - summary
- Appears to be widespread.
- Secure allocation usually prevents this form of
bias. - Need not be too expensive.
- Essential to prevent cheating.
28Secure allocation
- Can be achieved using telephone allocation from a
dedicated unit. - Can be achieved using independent person to
undertake allocation.
29Technical Bias
- This occurs when the allocation system breaks
down often due a computer fault. - A great example is the COMET I trial (COMET II
was done because COMET 1 suffered bias).
30COMET 1
- A trial of two types of epidural anaesthetics for
women in labour. - The trial was using MIMINISATION via a computer
programme. - The groups were minimised on age of mother and
her ethnicity. - Programme had a fault.
COMET Lancet 200135819.
31COMET 1 Technical Bias
32COMET II
- This new study had to be undertaken and another
1000 women recruited and randomised. - LESSON Always check the balance of your groups
as you go along if computer allocation is being
used.
33Attrition Bias
- Usually most trials lose participants after
randomisation. This can cause bias, particularly
if attrition differs between groups. - If a treatment has side-effects this may make
drop outs higher among the less well
participants, which can make a treatment appear
to be effective when it is not.
34Attrition Bias
- We can avoid some of the problems with attrition
bias by using Intention to Treat Analysis, where
we keep as many of the patients in the study as
possible even if they are no long on treatment.
35Selection bias after randomisation
- Selection bias is avoided if ALL participants who
are randomised are completely followed up. - Often there is some attrition after
randomisation some refuse to continue to take
part. - Or some may refuse the intervention but can still
be tracked IMPORTANT to distinguish between
these.
36What is wrong here?
37Ascertainment Bias
- This occurs when the person reporting the outcome
can be biased. - A particular problem when outcomes are not
objective and there is uncertainty as to
whether an event has occurred. - Example, of homeopathy study of histamine, showed
an effect when researchers were not blind to the
allocation but no effect when they were. - Multiple sclerosis treatment appeared to be
effective when clinicians unblinded but
ineffective when blinded.
38Resentful Demoralisation
- This can occur when participants are randomised
to treatment they do not want. - This may lead to them reporting outcomes badly in
revenge. - This can lead to bias.
39Resentful Demoralisation
- One solution is to use a patient preference
design where only participants who are
indifferent to the treatment they receive are
allocated. - This should remove its effects.
40Hawthorne Effect
- This is an effect that occurs by being part of
the study rather than the treatment.
Interventions that require more TLC than controls
could show an effect due to the TLC than the drug
or surgical procedure. - Placebos largely eliminate this or TLC should be
given to controls as well.
41Analytical Bias
- Once a trial has been completed and data gathered
in it is still possible to arrive at the wrong
conclusions by analysing the data incorrectly. - Most IMPORTANT is ITT.
- Also inappropriate sub-group analyses is a common
practice.
42Intention To Treat
- Main analysis of data must be by groups as
randomised. Per protocol or active treatment
analysis can lead to a biased result. - Those patients not taking the full treatment are
usually quite different to those that are and
restricting the analysis can lead to bias.
43Sub-Group Analyses
- Once the main analysis has been completed it is
tempting to look to see if the effect differs by
group. - Is treatment more or less effective in women?
- Is it better or worse among older people?
- Is treatment better among people at greater risk?
44Sub-Groups
- All of these are legitimate questions. The
problem is the more subgroups one looks at the
greater is the chance of finding a spurious
effect. - Sample size estimations and statistical tests are
based on 1 comparison only.
45Sub-Group and example.
- In a large RCT of asprin for myocardial
infarction a sub-group analysis showed that
people with the star signs Gemini and Libra
aspirin was INEFFECTIVE. - This is complete NONSENSE!
- This shows dangers of subgroup analyses.
Lancet 1988ii349-60.
46Sub groups
- To avoid spurious findings these should be
pre-specified and based on a reasonable
hypothesis. - Pre-specification is important avoid data
dredging as if you torture the data enough it
will confess.
47Summary
- Despite the RCT being the BEST research method
unless expertly used it can lead to biased
results. - Care must be taken to avoid as many biases as
possible.