Obtaining data - PowerPoint PPT Presentation

About This Presentation
Title:

Obtaining data

Description:

Obtaining data Available data are data that were produced in the past for some other purpose but that may help answer a present question inexpensively. – PowerPoint PPT presentation

Number of Views:60
Avg rating:3.0/5.0
Slides: 22
Provided by: frie164
Learn more at: http://people.uncw.edu
Category:

less

Transcript and Presenter's Notes

Title: Obtaining data


1
Obtaining data
  • Available data are data that were produced in the
    past for some other purpose but that may help
    answer a present question inexpensively. The
    library and the Internet are sources of available
    data.
  • Government statistical offices are the primary
    source for demographic, economic, and social data
    (visit the Fed-Stats site at www.fedstats.gov).
  • Beware of drawing conclusions from our own
    experience or hearsay. Anecdotal evidence is
    based on haphazardly selected individual cases,
    which we tend to remember because they are
    unusual in some way. They also may not be
    representative of any larger group of cases.
  • Some questions require data produced specifically
    to answer them. This leads to designing
    observational or experimental studies.

2
Observational study Record data on individuals
without attempting to influence the responses. We
typically cannot prove cause effect this
way. Example Based on observations you make in
nature,you suspect that female crickets choose
theirmates on the basis of their health. ?
Observehealth of male crickets that mated.
Experimental study Deliberately impose a
treatment on individuals and record their
responses. Lurking variables can be
controlled. Example Deliberately infect some
males with intestinal parasites and see whether
females tend to choose healthy rather than ill
males.
3
  • a sample is a collection of data drawn from a
    population, intended to represent the population
    from which it was drawn a census is an attempt
    to sample every individual in the population.
  • an experiment imposes a so-called treatment on
    individuals in order to observe their responses.
    This is in opposition to an observational study
    which simply observes individuals and measures
    variables of interest without intervention
  • go over Examples 3.4-3.6 on p. 176-177 (Chapter
    3, Introduction)

4
Terminology of experiments
  • The individuals in an experiment are the
    experimental units. If they are human, we call
    them subjects.
  • In an experiment, we do something to the subject
    and measure the response. The something we do
    (explanatory variable) is a called a treatment,
    or factor. The values of the factor are called
    its levels. Sometimes a treatment is a
    combination of levels of more than one factor.
  • The factor may be the administration of a drug
    the different dosages are its levels.
  • One group of people may be placed on a
    diet/exercise program for six months (treatment),
    and their blood pressure (response variable)
    would be compared with that of people who did not
    diet or exercise. Two levels here on diet, not
    on diet

5
  • Go over example 3.8 on page 179 (3.1, 1/8) and
    below an example of a designed experiment with
    two factors and six treatments. Also see Ex.
    3.9, p. 180 (3.1, 2/8) for an example of an
    experiment not designed well... The lack of a
    control group causes the problem...

6
  • If the experiment involves giving two different
    doses of a drug, we say that we are testing two
    levels of the factor.
  • A response to a treatment is statistically
    significant if it is larger than you would expect
    by chance (due to random variation among the
    subjects). We will learn how to determine this
    later.
  • In a study of sickle cell anemia, 150 patients
    were given the drug hydroxyurea, and 150 were
    given a placebo (dummy pill). The researchers
    counted the episodes of pain in each subject.
    Identify
  • The subjects
  • The factors / treatments
  • And the response variable
  • (patients, all 300)
  • 1 factor, 2 levels (hydroxyurea and placebo)
  • (episodes of pain)

7
  • In principle, experiments can give good evidence
    for causation through what we call randomized
    controlled comparative experiments.
  • The need for comparative experiments is shown in
    Example 3.9 on p. 180 a control group is needed
    so the experimenter can control the effects of
    outside (lurking) variables
  • The use of randomization is illustrated in
    Example 3.10 (3.1, 3/8) a chance mechanism is
    used to divide the experimental units into groups
    to prevent bias.

8
  • The logic behind randomized comparative
    experiments is given on p. 183 (3.1, 4/8)
  • Randomization produces groups of subjects that
    should be similar in all respects before the
    treatments are applied
  • Comparative design ensures that influences other
    than the treatment operate equally on all groups
  • Therefore, differences in the response must be
    due either to the treatment or to chance in the
    random assignment of subjects to the groups.
  • This lead to three basic principles of
    experimental design on page 183-184

9
  • Control the effects of lurking variables on the
    response, usually by comparing two or more
    treatments
  • Randomize use a chance mechanism to assign
    experimental units to treatments. See the Table
    B of random digits discussed on the later slides
  • Repeat each treatment on many units to reduce
    chance variation in the results
  • Then if you see differences in the response they
    are called statistically significant if they
    would rarely occur by chance

10
Caution about experimentation
The design of a study is biased if it
systematically favors certain outcomes.
The best way to exclude biases in an experiment
is to randomize the design. Both the individuals
and treatments are assigned randomly.
11
  • Other ways to remove bias
  • A double-blind experiment is one in which neither
    the subjects nor the experimenter know which
    individuals got which treatment until the
    experiment is completed. The goal is to avoid
    forms of placebo effects and biases in
    interpretation.
  • The best way to make sure your conclusions are
    robust is to replicate your experimentdo it
    over. Replication ensures that particular results
    are not due to uncontrolled factors or errors of
    manipulation.

12
Designing controlled experiments
Sir Ronald FisherThe father of statistics He
was sent to Rothamsted Agricultural Station in
the United Kingdom to evaluate the success of
various fertilizer treatments.
  • Fisher found the data from experiments going on
    for decades to be basically worthless because of
    poor experimental design.
  • Fertilizer had been applied to a field one year
    and not in another in order to compare the yield
    of grain produced in the two years. BUT
  • It may have rained more, or been sunnier, in
    different years.
  • The seeds used may have differed between years as
    well.
  • Or fertilizer was applied to one field and not to
    a nearby field in the same year. BUT
  • The fields might have different soil, water,
    drainage, and history of previous use.
  • ? Too many factors affecting the results were
    uncontrolled.

13
Fishers solution
Randomized comparative experiments
  • In the same field and same year, apply fertilizer
    to randomly spaced plots within the field.
    Analyze plants from similarly treated plots
    together.
  • This minimizes the effect of variation within the
    field in drainage and soil composition on yield,
    as well as controlling for weather.

F F F F F F
F F F F F F F F
F F F F F
F F F F F F F F
F F F F F
F F F F
14
A Table of Random Digits can be used to Randomize
an Experiment
  • any digit in any position in the table is as
    equally likely to be 0 as 1 as 2 as as 9
  • the digits in different positions are independent
    in the sense that the value of one has no
    influence on the value of any other
  • any pair of random digits has the same chance of
    being picked as any other (00, 01, 02, 99)
  • any triple of random digits has the same chance
    of being picked as any other (000, 001, 999)
  • and so on

15
  • Now use Table B to randomly divide the 40
    students in Ex. 3.10 into the two groups (control
    group and experimental group)
  • Step 1 Label the experimental units with as few
    digits as possible
  • Step 2 Decide on a protocol for how you will
    place the chosen units into the groups
  • Step 3 Start anywhere in the Table and begin
    reading random digits. Matching them with
    labeled experimental units and following the
    protocol creates the groups.
  • Go over example 3.11 on page 185ff (3.1, 5/8) in
    detail until you understand!

16
  • EX.3.10 We need to randomly divide the 40
    students into two groups of 20-the cell phone
    talking while driving and the driving group only.
  • List and number (label) all available subjects
    (the group of 40).
  • Decide that the first 20 students chosen go to
    the experimental group the remainder to the
    control group (this is the protocol)
  • Scan Table B in groups of numbers that are two
    digits long. Match the digits with the labels and
    follow the protocol to form the groups.

45 46 71 17 09 77 55 80 00 95 32 86
32 94 85 82 22 69 00 56
17
  • There are many types of experimental designs in
    use today in the sciencesread about these on p.
    189-191 (3.1, 7/8 8/8)
  • Completely randomized all experimental units
    are allocated at random among all treatments (Ex.
    3.10)
  • Block designs A block is a group of experimental
    units or subjects known in advance to be similar
    in some way that is expected to affect the
    response to the treatments. Knowing this, the
    experimenter can create a block design, in which
    the random assignment of units is carried out
    separately within each block. See examples
    3.18-3.20 for some examples
  • Matched pairs This is a common design in which a
    block design is used to compare just two
    treatments. Sometimes each subject receives both
    treatments (acts as its own control), or there is
    a before-after design.

18
Completely randomized designs
Completely randomized experimental
designs Individuals are randomly assigned to
groups, then the groups are randomly assigned to
treatments.
19
Block designs
In a block, or stratified, design, subjects are
divided into groups, or blocks, prior to the
experiment to test hypotheses about differences
between the groups. The blocking, or
stratification, here is by gender.
20
Matched pairs designs
Matched pairs Choose pairs of subjects that are
closely matchede.g., same sex, height, weight,
age, and race. Within each pair, randomly assign
who will receive which treatment. It is also
possible to just use a single person, and give
the two treatments to this person over time in
random order (before/after). In this case,
the matched pair is just the same person at
different points in time. Pre/post testing of a
new teaching method is another example...
21
  • Read the Introduction Section 3.1. Watch the
    StatTutors - I'll assign them officially on the
    StatsPortal. Pay particular attention to all the
    Examples. Make sure you understand the
    terminology and the sketches of the types of
    designs... Also, make sure you can use Table B
    to perform a completely randomized design.
  • Do 3.3, 3.4, 3.6, 3.7, 3.9, 3.11, 3.12, 3.18,
    3.19, 3.21, 3.26, 3.27-3.29, 3.35, 3.39
  • Test 1 will cover Chapters 1-3 and some parts of
    Ch.4. Start getting ready for it!
Write a Comment
User Comments (0)
About PowerShow.com