Title: Assumptions in Cognitive Neuropsychology
1Assumptions in Cognitive Neuropsychology
- Aims and Objectives
- By the end of this lecture you will have learned
- The key aims of cognitive psychology
- The core assumptions relevant to cognitive
neuropsychology and their implications - Required Reading
- Rapp Ch1.
2Clinical Neuropsychologyvs Cognitive
Neuropsycholgy?
- Both approaches linked on many levels, but there
are also significant differences in their
empahsis. - CLINICAL - focuses on the effects of brain damage
on psychological processes - Determine pathology
- Characterise deficit
- Establish baseline
- COGNITIVE - focuses on undestanding impairments
in psychological processes in terms of
disruptions to information processing elements
involved. - e.g. cognitive approach requires a MODEL of
healthy function - this may or may not be tied
back to neuroanatomy
3Aims of Cognitive Neuropsychology
- 1. Model confirmation / development
- According to Ellis and Young, cognitive
neuropsychologists - explain patterns of impaired and intact
cognitive performance seen in brain injured
patients in terms of damage to one or more of the
components of a theory or model of normal
cognitive functioning - draw conclusions about normal, intact cognitive
processes from the patterns of impaired and
intact capabilities seen in brain-injured
patients - These two approaches are obviously linked, but
differ in emphasis.
4Aims of Cognitive Neuropsychology
- 2. Cognitive localisation -
- The attempt to specify specific areas of the
brain as being involved in certain processes - - some researchers NEVER do this (Ultra or
Radical CNs) - - others talk about it but know that it is a
separate issue from the cognitive theories
themselves - - cognitive neuroscientists (e.g. Damasio /
Goldman-Rakic) are particularly concerned with
this and use data from patients, NCs, animal
studies, molecular studies etc.. - Theoretical vs Anatomical paradigms Mackay (2001)
5Assumptions in Cognitive Neuropsychology
- These are the topics of considerable debate
- The debates are very complex! (see extended
reading list for some examples) - These asumptions have very important
implications for the methodologies used in
cognitive neuropsychology (e.g single case vs
group studies) and the inferences which cognitive
neuropsychologists can draw from their data - These assumptions are also important to bear in
mind when interpreting functional neuroimaging
data.
6Cognitive Neuropsychology
Assumptions
Modularity - Mental life is orchestrated by
multiple cognitive processors or modules
Neurological specificity (isomorphism) - there is
a correspondence between the organisation of the
mind and the organisation of the brain (both lead
to locality assumption)
Transparency - observable behaviour will indicate
which module is dysfunctional
Subtractivity - Performance reflects total
cognitive system minus the impaired module(s)
Universality - There are no individual
differences in the organisation of cognitive
modules
7Assumption of Modularity
Mental life is orchestrated by multiple cognitive
processors or modules Marr (1982) - Principle of
Modular Design - any large computation should be
split up into a collection of small, nearly
independent, specialized subprocesses
- Advantages of modularity
- Reduces computational costs
- Makes complex systems easier to improve / evolve
- Simplifies error-detection / correction
- Local damage remains local
8Assumption of Modularity
- Concept of modularity extended by Fodor in The
Modularity of Mind (1983) - Modules are
- Domain specific
- Innately specified
- Informationally encapsulated
- Fast
- Hardwired (neurally specific)
- Autonomous
- Not assembled
- Operation is mandatory
- Only 1 3 are generally accepted by
neuropsychologists
9Assumption of Modularity
Domain Specificity Each module can only process
one type of input
E.g. is there a single Person Recognition Module
that operates on visual and acoustic inputs?
10Assumption of Modularity
Domain Specificity Each module can only process
one type of input
Not if modules are truly domain specific.
Face Recognition Module
Voice Recognition Module
OUTPUT
11Assumption of Modularity
Informationally Encapsulated The processes are
carried out in isolation from and ignorance of
processing in other modules
Some psychologists have interpreted this in a
very strict way (e.g. Farah) One implication is
that information processing can only proceed in a
bottom up fashion. E.g. evidence of top-down
influences on processing can be used as evidence
against the assumption of modularity. Fodor was
himself less strict, arguing that it meant not
having access to a persons expectations,
beliefs, presumptions or desires
12Assumption of Modularity
- Modularity in a strictly Fodorian sense causes
problems for cognitive neuropsychology- - Reading is clearly not innate
- Not all cognitive modules appear mandatory
(e.g.Recognition may be mandatory but is name
recall?) - What about top-down processing?
-
- Fodor also argued that only input (and possibly
output) processes are modular - Most cognitive psychologists assume that
central processes (e.g. reasoning, decision
making) are also modular to some extent.
13Assumption of Modularity
Neo-Fodorian account of modularity (Coltheart,
1999)
The other Fodorian criteria are not necessary
features of modules.
Whether or not a module possesses any of these
features becomes an interesting empirical question
Assumption of modularity is linked with
assumption of locality (Farah, 1994) and logic of
double dissociation
14Assumption of Isomorphism
- There is a correspondence between the
organisation of the mind and the organisation of
the brain - A critical assumption, that is rarely
acknowledged - Very important for the interpretation of
functional neuroimaging data. - Also important for clinical neuropsychology
- Its importance depends on how cognitive you
like your cognitive neuropsychology.
15Assumption of Isomorphism
- HOWEVER
- It is possible to have functional modularity but
not anatomical modularity - E.g cognitive modules may be distributed across
wide areas of cortex - This would imply that any brain damage ought to
impair a large number of modules - The fact that so many patients exist with highly
selective disorders suggest that the assumption
is broadly tenable.
16Assumption of Transparency
- observable behaviour will indicate which module
is dysfunctional - E.g. careful analysis of the pattern of impaired
/ intact performance, and the ways in which
patients perform neuropsychological tests will
allow valid conclusions to be drawn. - Brain damaged performance reflects
- True contribution from disruption to one or
more hypothesised modules - Individual Differences (c.f. Lecture on
Methodologies) - Effects of compensatory mechanisms
- Effects from disruption to modules other than the
hypothesised modules (c.f. Lecture on
Methodologies)
17Assumption of Transparency
- HOWEVER
- Individual differences Deficit may have preceded
the injury The Martian within us problem
(Caramazza). - (Individual differences may become increasingly
tractable given modern techniques.) - Compensatory Mechanisms According to CN these
must reflect the strategic use of existing
modules rather than the generation of new
modules. - E.g brain damage does not result in the de novo
creation of cognitive modules resulting in a
system which is uninterpretable in terms of
models of normal systems. - Multiple Modules Strictly localised brain damage
is incredibly rare (see next lecture) - Easy to ascribe performance to damage in module
A, when in fact it is module B that is damaged.
18Assumption of Subtractivity
- Performance reflects total cognitive system minus
the impaired module(s) - The cognitive system of the brain damaged
subject is the same as that of a normal subject
apart from a local modification - - The existing modules carry on exactly as
before - - No new modules are formed
- - The organisation of the existing modules
remains unchanged. - Neuroscience research suggests the brain may have
much more plasticity than previously assumed.
19Assumption of Universality
- This assumption is crucial for any group studies
in cognitive science - Allows us to consider the average performance of
a group of individuals to be representative of
any individual in the population from which the
group was drawn. - HOWEVER
- Individual differences are increasingly obvious
and difficult to ignore - Patients rarely have exactly the same brain
damage.