Intensity discrimination is the process of distinguishing one stimulus intensity from another

1 / 45
About This Presentation
Title:

Intensity discrimination is the process of distinguishing one stimulus intensity from another

Description:

Intensity discrimination is the process of distinguishing one stimulus intensity ... The increment threshold data of a rod monochromat (circles) plotted along ... –

Number of Views:201
Avg rating:3.0/5.0
Slides: 46
Provided by: NOR652
Category:

less

Transcript and Presenter's Notes

Title: Intensity discrimination is the process of distinguishing one stimulus intensity from another


1
Intensity Discrimination
Intensity discrimination is the process of
distinguishing one stimulus intensity from another
2
Two types Difference thresholds the two
stimuli are physically separate Increment
thresholds the two stimuli are immediately
adjacent or superimposed
3
Fig. 1.1
4
Fig. 1.2
5
Theory and Practice
Theory
6
Fig. 2.7 From Dr. Krafts course Hecht, Shlaer
Pirenne Photon emission follows a Poisson
distribution
To distinguish a flash with a mean of 8 from a
flash with a mean of 9 quanta is impossible! The
distributions overlap almost completely
7
Mean of 8, vs. mean of 9
8
Mean of 8, vs. mean of 12
9
Mean of 8, vs. mean of 16
10
Mean of 8, vs. mean of 20
11
In a Poisson distribution, the variance is equal
to the mean. The standard deviation (SD) is the
square root of the mean. In a two-alternative
forced-choice task, to reach threshold (75
correct), LT must differ from L by 0.95 SD.
(e.g., ?L 0.95 SD)
12
Moreover, as L increases, the minimum ?L also
increases with the
because the variance in a Poisson distribution
equals the mean, so the SD changes with the
square root of the mean
13
An ideal observer would follow the
14
Theory and Practice
In practice
at low background intensities, human observers
behave as an ideal detector (follow the
deVries-Rose Law)
15
Fig. 3.1
16
(No Transcript)
17
(No Transcript)
18
Fig. 3.1
19
You always can determine the Weber fraction, even
when Webers Law does not hold
20
Fig. 3.2
Webers Law does NOT hold (?L/ L rises as L
decreases)
Webers Law holds
21
Both the deVries-Rose and Webers laws fail to
account
for thresholds at high light intensities
Fig. 3.3
The increment threshold data of a rod
monochromat (circles) plotted along
with the theoretical lower limit (deVries-Rose,
dotted line) and the predictions of Webers
2
Law (solid line). Luminance values are in cd/m
. (Redrawn from Hess et al. (1990)
22
More practical issues How changes in other
stimulus dimensions affect the Weber fraction
23
1 Stimulus size the Weber fraction is lower
(smaller) for larger test stimuli
Fig. 3.4
24
(No Transcript)
25
(No Transcript)
26
More practical issues Is a target visible under
certain conditions?
D
Log Weber Fraction,
L/L
2
This is the targets Weber fraction. It is NOT a
threshold
Test Field Diameter
4'
1
121'
0
Is a spot with a particular luminance, relative
to background, visible? It depends on its size.
-1
-2
If the target is 121, it is visible If 4, it is
not visible
-3
-7
-6
-5
-4
-3
-2
-1
0
1
2
3
2
Log Background Intensity, L (cd/m
)
27
Need to distinguish between the Weber fraction of
a target vs. the threshold of a viewer. For a
subject or patient viewing a target, if the
subjects Weber fraction is below a line, then
the subjects threshold is better (smaller). If
the Weber fraction of a target is below the line,
the target is NOT visible to someone whose
threshold is on the line.
28
The smaller the threshold ?L, the smaller is the
value of the Weber fraction for a given
background L, (only the numerator changes) and
the more sensitive the visual system is to
differences in light intensity.
29
Fig. 1.2
30
The dinner plate example Plate with luminance
of 0.0102 footlamberts. Background is 0.01
footlamberts ?L is thus 0.0102 0.01
0.0002. ?L/L 0.0002/0.01 0.02 (plate is 2
more intense) From Figure 3-4, can learn that
this is not visible.
31
.
32
More practical issues Is a target visible under
certain conditions?
D
Log Weber Fraction,
L/L
2
Test Field Diameter
4'
1
Is a spot with a particular luminance, relative
to background, visible? It depends on its size.
121'
0
Threshold Weber fraction for 121 objects
-1
-2
Plates Weber fraction
-3
-7
-6
-5
-4
-3
-2
-1
0
1
2
3
2
Log Background Intensity, L (cd/m
)
33
Continuing How changes in other stimulus
dimensions affect the Weber fraction
2 Short-duration flashes are harder to see (are
less discriminable) than long-duration
flashes That is, the threshold ?L increases as
flash duration becomes shorter.
34
Sensitivity 1/threshold
35
Continuing How changes in other stimulus
dimensions affect the Weber fraction
3 Threshold ?L varies with eccentricity from
the fovea At low luminance levels, threshold is
lowest (sensitivity is highest) about 15-20
degrees from fovea and the fovea is blind At
high luminance levels, threshold is lowest at the
fovea
36
This is the basis for visual field tests
Fig. 3.5
37
(No Transcript)
38
Sensory Magnitude Scales Revisited Using the
just noticeable difference (jnd) to create a
scale for sensory magnitude vs. stimulus
magnitude L threshold ?L LT LT is one jnd
more intense than L. LT threshold ?L LT2 LT2
is one jnd more intense than LT And so on
39
Sensory Magnitude
12
10
8
6
4
L
2
0
0
50
100
150
200
2
Stimulus Luminance, L (cd/m
)
40
Sensory Magnitude
12
10
L threshold ?L LT LT is one just noticeable
difference (jnd) more intense than L.
8
6
LT
4
L
2
0
0
50
100
150
200
2
Stimulus Luminance, L (cd/m
)
41
Sensory Magnitude
12
LT threshold ?L LT2 LT2 is one jnd more
intense than LT and 2 jnds larger than L
10
8
LT2
6
LT
4
L
2
0
0
50
100
150
200
2
Stimulus Luminance, L (cd/m
)
42
Sensory Magnitude
12
LTn1
10
LTn
8
When Webers Law holds, the threshold ?Ls keep
getting larger, so 1 jnd is a larger increase in
stimulus luminance
LT2
6
LT
4
L
2
0
0
50
100
150
200
2
Stimulus Luminance, L (cd/m
)
43
Fechners Law
Sensory Magnitude
12
10
8
6
4
2
Fechner's Law Log(L)
0
0
50
100
150
200
2
Stimulus Luminance, L (cd/m
)
44
(No Transcript)
45
Comparing Fechners Law with Stevens Power Law
Fig. 3.6
Stevens Power Law resembles Fechners Law when
the exponent is lt1
Write a Comment
User Comments (0)
About PowerShow.com