Title: Physiological Optics III
1Physiological Optics III
- Dr. Prasert Padungkiatsakul
2Ocular Motility
3Horopter
- is a spatial map of corresponding points across
the retina, appear to be _at_ the same distance from
the observer as the fixation point - zero disparity, an equidistant horopter
- representing how we perceive 3-D visual space
- Theoretical point horopter the locus of all
points in visual space that are imaged on
corresponding points in each eye w/n the eyes are
converged to aim _at_ particular fixation point.
4Horopter
- Extends both horizontal and vertical
- Longitudinal / horizontal horopter a slice of
the horopter along the horizontal plane. - The two fovea, each representing the oculocentric
primary visual direction are corresponding points
5Corresponding points
- A point w/c displacement by a degree off the
fovea in one eye and an equal displacement off
the fovea in the same direction in the other eye
6Vieth-Müller circle
- The set of all possible pairs of corresponding
points would be stimulated by objects lying
anywhere on a circle that intersects the fixation
point and the nodal points of the two eyes.
theoretical horopter circle, geometric horopter
7(No Transcript)
8Horopter
- Because we cant stimulate the retinal points
directly, but we do know the corresponding points
in each eye by placing an object in physical
space so that its images in each eye are formed
on corresponding points. - With controlled condition, eye stationary,
convergence symmetrically, we can map the whole
corresponding retinal points horopter
9Facts about corresponding points
- are perceived as having identical visual
directions in the two eyes. ?can split the images
of an object into two independent segregated
images each presented to one eye, and see where
the object can be seen from visual direction for
each eye.
10Facts about corresponding points
- Have no binocular disparity.
- Horopter show us all the points in space that are
perceived as being _at_ the same distance from the
eye as the fixation points - ?horopter have zero disparity and be seen in a
flat plane equidistant to the fixation point - ? no fusional eye movements needed
11Facts about corresponding points
- If images displaced off the corresponding points,
we get crossed disparity or uncrossed disparity - ?horopter the location in visual space of
boundaries between crossed and uncrossed
disparities as we fixate a particular point - ? horopter will be the place in space where we
are most sensitive to changes in depth, change
from crossed to uncrossed, objects will appear to
change from being closer than fixation point to
farther away
12Facts about corresponding points
- As locations in space deviate more from the
horopter, crossed or uncrossed disparity will be
introduced, and eventually diplopia will occur as
the limits of Panums areas are reached. - ? horopter the center of the range in w/c we
have single vision
13Methods of Measuring the Horopter (Horopter
criteria)
- Identical visual directions
- Equidistance / Stereoscopic depth matching
- Singleness / Haplopia
- Minimum stereoacuity threshold
- Zero vergence
14Identical visual direction horopter
- Measured by comparing two rods, one by both eyes
(Fixation point), other upper half be seen by one
eye and bottom half be seen by another eye w/
polaroid filter - The subject move the rod forward and backward
until both half-images of the rod appear to line
up perfectly
15(No Transcript)
16(No Transcript)
17Identical visual direction horopter
- The subject move the rod forward and backward
until both half-images of the rod appear to line
up perfectly - Plot all the corresponding points horopter
- Thick line, become thicker _at_ periphery because
the elevated spatial localization thresholds in
the peripheral retina
18(No Transcript)
19Identical visual direction horopter
- W/ fixation disparity, the horopter will be
displace inward for eso FD, outward for exo FD
relative to where the physical fixation rod line - Because the eyes are not really aiming _at_ the
physical fixation rod, they aimed _at_ a true
fixation point slightly in front of (eso FD) or
behind it (exo FD) - The horopter is then simply shifted toward where
the visual axes of the two eyes crossing
20(No Transcript)
21Equidistance / Stereoscopic depth matching
horopter
- Apparent frontoparallel plane (AFPP) method
- More precise method and easy to do w/ untrained
subjects - If an object produces monocular images that have
zero disparity, the visual direction of their
images must also be identical
22Procedure
- The subject views a numbers of rods while
fixating the middle rod. - The subject then adjusts the distances of all of
the other rod until they all appear to be _at_ the
same distance away as the middle rod in a plane
parallel to the subjects face - The horopter will be curved, but the percept of
the subject will be a flat plane
23Equidistance / Stereoscopic depth matching
horopter
- The shape of the frontoparallel plane as
perceived by the subject will be the mirror
images of the horopter setting - Move the rod closer than fixation because the
horopter farther away from the fixation and they
are trying to compensate for this by moving the
rod inward to make their position appear in
alignment
24Equidistance / Stereoscopic depth matching
horopter
- Advantage the examiner can see the shape of
horopter directly from the subjects placement
the rods - Disadvantage fails to reflect the effects of FD
25(No Transcript)
26(No Transcript)
27(No Transcript)
28(No Transcript)
29Singleness / Haplopia horopter
- The horopter is related to the absolute placement
of the rods in space, the bias in the rod
setting, and stereoscopic threshold can be
obtained from the variance of those setting, or
our sensitivity _at_ detecting binocular disparities
between the images of the rods
30(No Transcript)
31(No Transcript)
32Singleness / Haplopia horopter
- Images on slightly noncorresponding points may be
fused into a single percept as long as they lies
w/in Panums fusional area - Measures the extent of Panums fusional area _at_
the fovea and _at_ eccentric location
33Procedure
- The arrangement of the rods is similar to AFPP
- Middle rod is always fixated, the 2nd test rod is
moved closer to the subject until diplopia is
reached. Repeated moving the rod farther away - Zone of singleness space between these two
limits - Measurement are made w/ test rods _at_ several
eccentricities on either side of the fixated rod
34(No Transcript)
35Singleness / Haplopia horopter
- The center of this zone of singleness is taken to
be the singleness horopter - The haplopia horopter indicates where
corresponding points lies - The width of the zone of singleness reflects
Panums area in w/c noncorresponding points are
still seen as single - The present FD can bias the location of the
horopter
36(No Transcript)
37Minimum stereoacuity threshold (Maximum
stereoacuity horopter)
- Measured by fixating on a central rod while
measuring stereoscopic threshold for a second - More eccentric rod, determining the smallest
stereoscopic disparity (change in depth) that can
be detected for that rod - Relies on the observation that we are most
sensitive to changes in disparity and less so
from changes relative to a nonzero
38Procedure
- Start the test rods _at_ the same perceived distance
as the fixation point, move it in depth until the
subject just perceives it as being _at_ a different
distance - Repeat this for different target distance.
- The measurement is obtained by determining the
variance of the rod settings that are seen as
lying _at_ the same distance - The problem w/ this technique is that it is
extremely time-consuming and difficult, is not
practical use
39(No Transcript)
40Zero vergence
- The most difficult horopter to measure
- Requires measurements of extremely small fusional
movements of the eyes using sensitive objective
eye movement recording equipment - The subject would view the fixation target, and a
second target would be flashed momentarily - If the test target fell on noncorresponding
points in the two eyes, the exposure of the test
target elicits a motor fusional response from the
subject
41Zero vergence
- If the test target has a binocular disparity
because it lies off the horopter, it would serve
as a stimulus to the vergence eye movement system
42(No Transcript)
43Important to note
- For all horopter criteria, horopter are typically
measured with the eyes fixating a target _at_ the
same vertical height, to ensure symmetric
convergence
44The shape of the empirical horopter and its
analysis
- The Vieth-Müller circle is defined by 3 points,
the fixation point and the entrance pupils of the
eye. - For any point on the circle, the angle between
the entrance pupil, and fixation point for the
left eye (angle ?1) is equal to same angle for
the right eye (angle ?2) - Angle ?1 and ?2 are called external longitudinal
angle
45The shape of the empirical horopter and its
analysis
- The Vieth-Müller circle is the loci of all
corresponding retinal points as influenced by the
optic of the eye - R the ratio of the tangent of the two external
longitudinal angles _at_ any point on the horopter - R the relative magnification of the retinal
images between two eyes
46(No Transcript)
47- R 1, ?1 ?2, the left and right eye
magnification are equal
48The shape of the empirical horopter and its
analysis
- R ? 1 the physical targets are not actually
lined up in physical space, although they are
perceived as being lined up. - R gt 1 angle ?2 gt angle ?1 in physical space, the
right eyes image gt the left eyes - R lt 1 angle ?1 gt angle ?2, the left eyes images
gt the right eyes
49- R gt 1 ?2 gt ?1 in physical space, right gt left
- R lt 1 ?1 gt ?2, left gt right
50The shape of the empirical horopter and its
analysis
- Plot the value of R for each data point on the
horopter as a function of the magnitude of the
angle w/ ?2 ? analytical plot - Interested in 2 values
- Slop H
- Y-intercept, R0
51The shape of the empirical horopter and its
analysis
- The analytical plot is simply the graph of the
equation - R H(tan?2) R0
- R0 value of the tangent ratio R measured _at_ the
fixation point, the ratio of the magnification of
the image size in one eye relative to the fellow
eye - This relative magnification results in a tilting
of the horopter relative to the frontal plane
52The shape of the empirical horopter and its
analysis
- R0 1, there is no skewing of the horopter, flat
slope _at_ the fixation point
53(No Transcript)
54The shape of the empirical horopter and its
analysis
- R0 ? 1, there is uniform relative magnification
(equal magnification _at_ every retinal location),
tilting the horopter - R0 gt 1, left image larger, horopter is rotated
toward that eye. - R0 lt 1, right image larger, horopter is rotated
toward that eye.
55(No Transcript)
56The shape of the empirical horopter and its
analysis
- Empirical horopter does not coincide w/ the
theoretical Vieth-Müller circle - The horopter tends to be less sharply curved
- The different between the horopter and the
Vieth-Müller circle is called Hering-Hillebrand
horopter deviation, H - It tell us that our perception of space is warped
a little
57(No Transcript)
58The shape of the empirical horopter and its
analysis
- H tell us the relative curvature of the horopter
- H 0, horopter lies on Vieth-Müller circle
- H , horopter is less curved than Vieth-Müller
circle - H ? horopter is more curved than Vieth-Müller
circle
59(No Transcript)
60The shape of the empirical horopter and its
analysis
- H is typically in the range of 0.1 to 0.2
- H is a measure of nonuniform relative
magnification across visual field - Local sign are not laid out equiangularly, nasal
more packed gt temporal
61(No Transcript)
62The shape of the empirical horopter and its
analysis
- The precise shape of the empirical horopter is a
function of the fixation distance used when
measure it. - W/ greater fixation distance, the horopter curves
more and more away from the observer, eventually
becoming convex - Abathic distance distance _at_ w/c the apparent
and objective frontal planes coincide, horopter
is flat
63The shape of the empirical horopter and its
analysis
- H 2a/b
- 2a the interpupillary distance
- b fixation distance
- The bathic distance is typically about 6 m from
the observer - The curvature of the Vieth-Müller circle is also
changing proportionately w/ increased fixation
distance
64(No Transcript)
65Vertical horopter
- _at_ near point fixation distances, the theoretical
vertical horopter is a straight line parallel to
the head and intersecting the Vieth-Müller circle
_at_ the fixation point. - Empirically, vertical horopter tilts away from
true vertical - Vertical horopter actually inclines w/ its top
farther away from the observer than the bottom
66(No Transcript)
67Vertical horopter
- Being less inclined relative to the visual axis
w/ near fixation and more inclined w/ distance
fixation - This inclination increase until, _at_ distance, the
empirical vertical horopter tend to lie parallel
to the ground below eye level - The most natural horizontal surface _at_ distance
are below eye level
68The horopter in abnormal binocular vision
- Aniseikonia a different in magnification
between the two eyes, different size or shape
69Aniseikonia
- About 2-3 of population
- Different retinal images size between 2 eyes
- Optical origin (Optical aniseikonia)
- Neural origin (Neural or essential aniseikonia)
70Optical aniseikonia
- Axial aniseikonia (Axial anisometropia)
- Refractive aniseikonia (Refractive anisometropia)
- Induced aniseikonia caused by external optical
factors, an afocal magnifier called size lens.
71Neural aniseikonia
- A small magnitude nonoptical aniseikonia that can
occur even in emmetropes, two retinal images are
physically equal in size yet still perceived to
be different in size. - Optical and Neural aniseikonia are independent
phenomena that can either have an additive effect
or cancel out one another.
72Aniseikonia
- May have a substantial effect on binocular visual
perception, distorting our 3-D perception,
degrading stereopsis, large enough inducing
binocular suppression.
73Size lens
- A thick lens w/ parallel front and back surfaces
that changes the magnification of an image w/o
having any dioptic power. - Spherical surface both front and back overall
magnifier - Cylindrical surface both front and back induce
magnification in one meridian a meridional size
lens, cause shape changes in viewed objects.
74Size lens
- Magnifying effects
- Power factor
- Shape factor
75Power factor
- h vertex dist.
- Fv back vertex power of the lens
76Shape factor
- t lens thickness
- n? index of refraction
- F1 front surface power
77Size lens
- Magnifying effects
- In afocal magnifier, there is no refractive power
?no power factor, only the shape factor
78Meridional magnifier
- Place the magnifier axis 90 ?magnification occur
on horizontal meridian - AFPP rotate about fixation point. Because the
difference in horizontal image sizes between two
eyes. ?R changes , horopter would be rotated in
the opposite direction as the AFPP - Called geometric effect
79(No Transcript)
80Geometric effect
- Can be explained easily by the geometry of the
horopter and of the magnified images. - Horopter rotate toward the magnified eye,
observer perceives the world as rotated away from
the manified eye
81Geometric effect
- The degree rotation / tilting of the visual space
equation - tan ? (M-1)/(M1)d/a
- M magnification of the size lens
- d viewing distance
- a ½ of PD
82Geometric effect
- Stronger magnification, the greater rotation /
tilting - Shorter the viewing distance, the greater
rotation / tilting - This condition is quite confusing to the patient
because the depth information - Binocular cues, horizontal disparities
- Monocular cues, overlap, texture gradients
83Leaf room
- Literally a room in w/c the wall, floor, ceiling
are covered w/ leaves to help obscure monocular
cues to depth - The entire room look tilted and distorted w/n a
magnifier is place before one eye, geometric
effect is that the wall, floor, and ceiling all
appear to slant - W/ an axis 90 afocal magnifier on the right eye,
right wall appear to be farther away than the
left, except from the tilting of the AFPP in the
geometric effect
84(No Transcript)
85Leaf room
- The apparent size of leaves on the wall varies as
a function of the perceived distance of the
walls, - The floor slant downward to the right, ceiling
slant upward to the right - ?the square leaves room no longer appears to be
square
86Leaf room
- The changes in the ceiling and floor (vertical
position) is not magnification effects, because
the magnification induces only horizontal
binocular disparities - The changes in the apparent size and distance of
the side walls create a secondary illusion of
slant in the floor and ceiling - Only horizontal disparities produced a percept of
depth, vertical disparities does not
87Vertical magnification
- A small amount of vertical disparity leads to
diplopia because humans have limited vertical
fusional eye movement capabilities - ?an axis 180 meridional size lens would produce
no change in the apparent AFPP or horopter - But the world will seem tilted, similarly the
effect produced by an axis 90 magnifier placed in
front of the fellow eye.
88(No Transcript)
89Vertical magnification
- Nobody know why vertical magnification in one eye
looks like horizontal magnification in the other
eye - Called induced effect because it cannot be
explained in terms of geometry
90Induced effect
- W/n uniformly magnified in the horizontal and
vertical meridians, both geometric and induced
effects will be generated, the strength of these
two percepts is roughly equal for small degrees
of overall magnification but in the opposite
direction - ?If uniformly magnify an image in one eye by a
small amount, it will have little or no effect on
the orientation of the AFPP, the geometric and
induced effects will simply cancel each other out
91(No Transcript)
92Aniseikonia
- Can be produced by asymmetric convergence, to
bifoveally fixate a nearpoint target that is not
on the vertical midline, the fixate target closer
than the other ? different retinal image size. - Every diopter of refractive difference between
two eyes ? 1.4 relative magnification between
two eyes - Magnification is equal in all meridians image ?no
tilting image
93Aniseikonia
- Induced effect break down w/ magnification
greater than 5-7 - Relative magnification difference in aniseikonia
gt 7 ? disruption of fusion ? amblyopia if
present in an infant
94Oblique magnification
- Produces a different kind of tilted percept,
cyclodisparity - Vertical lines tilted toward the meridian of
magnification. This tilt translates to horizontal
binocular disparities that ?in magnitude as you
move vertically from the fovea, the vertical
disparities are opposite in sign for the upper
and lower visual field - inclination / declination effect
95Inclination /declination effect
- A percept that the world is tilted about the
horizontal meridian, top of VF is tilted away
from you and the bottom toward to you or vice
versa
96Cyclovergence eye movements
- In oblique magnification, cyclorotary eye
movements act in a compensatory manner, lessen
the perceptual effects of the cyclodisparity - Patients may complain of the floor appearing to
tilt upward or downward - Oblique cylindrical lenses may also produce this
effect
97Knapps law
- Uncorrected refractive emmetropes has little
effect on image size relative to that of the
emmetropic eye - Uncorrected axial ametropia produces an image
size much different from that of the emmetropic
eye, correction w/ spectacle lenses placed near
the anterior focal plane of the eye will produce
an image size that is the same as that of an
emmetropic eye
98Aniseikonia
- Unilateral intraocular lens implants (IOLs)
following cataract extraction exhibit substantial
aniseikonia, especially IOL _at_ antr chamber, even
some patients w/ bilateral IOLs - Monocular refractive surgery
- High cylindrical lenses having unequal power in
different meridians ? unequal magnification in
these meridians ? the geometric and induced
effects are unequal
99Aniseikonia
- The skewing of the horopter w/ spectacle
correction of high anisometropia and astigmatism
explains in part the complaint distortions of
environment around them - Force the patient to make unequal amplitude
saccades and pursuit in each eye
100Brecher Maddox rod technique
- Left eye views two penlights, w/ right eye
viewing them through a Maddox rod - Right eye seen two red streaks of light
- Iseikonia equal space between the penlights and
space between streaks of light - Aniseikonia the two spacings are unequal
101(No Transcript)
102Space eikonometer
- A form of stereoscope w/ two vertical lines and
an oblique cross as target - A person w/ aniseikonia will see the cross as
rotated instead of in a flat plane parallel to
the eyes and/or one of the vertical lines closer
to the observer - Iseikonic lens the lens for correcting the
aniseikonia by modifying the front surface
curvature, thickness, and refractive index
103(No Transcript)
104Aniseikonia
- Changes the fixation distance, the geometric
effect, and induced effect distort our percept of
space and distorted horopter - All of these case show the R (uniform
magnification) has been altered not for H
(nonuniform magnification) - The value of H does not change even under a
variety of condition
105Aniseikonia
- The manipulations all reflect optical changes to
the horopter rather than neural changes - W/ prism, there is a nonuniform magnification
across the prism, more magnification _at_ the apex
than _at_ the base - Cause nonuniform distortions in the perception of
visual space and in the horopter
106Prism
- Base-out ? cause visual space to curve concave
toward the viewer - Base-in ? cause visual space to curve convex
toward the viewer
107(No Transcript)
108Adaptation to Lens and Prism
- Prescribing lens and prism, the space perception
should not sacrificed in the attempt to obtain
the best VA, use caution if that lens induces
aniseikonia - The visual system is capable of adaptation to
distortions of visual space, the adaptation is
only partial, still some remaining spatial
distortion - W/ geometric effect being neutralized w/in 3-4
days and induced effect 5-6 days
109Adaptation to Lens and Prism
- Oblique magnification, the strength of this
adaptation is less. - The binocular visual system can tolerate small
amounts of aniseikonia w/o loss of function - ?40 of emmetropes have neural aniseikonia of at
least 0.8 ? clinical symptoms (asthenopia) can
occur w/in 1-2 magnification differences, beyond
5 begins it influence stereoscopic thresholds
110Adaptation to Lens and Prism
- Oblique magnification, the strength of this
adaptation is less. - The binocular visual system can tolerate small
amounts of aniseikonia w/o loss of function - ?40 of emmetropes have neural aniseikonia of at
least 0.8 ? clinical symptoms (asthenopia) can
occur w/in 1-2 magnification differences, beyond
5 begins it influence stereoscopic thresholds