Interpersonal Skills - PowerPoint PPT Presentation

1 / 67
About This Presentation
Title:

Interpersonal Skills

Description:

Interpersonal Skills Health Psychology student doctors Although student doctors found chemistry and biology relatively easy dealing with their patients is not so easy. – PowerPoint PPT presentation

Number of Views:399
Avg rating:3.0/5.0
Slides: 68
Provided by: garys7
Category:

less

Transcript and Presenter's Notes

Title: Interpersonal Skills


1
Interpersonal Skills
  • Health Psychology

2
student doctors
  • Although student doctors found chemistry and
    biology relatively easy dealing with their
    patients is not so easy.
  • Battenburg and Gerritsma (1983) student doctors
    found it hard to
  • 1. hard to initiate conversation
  • 2. decide on diagnosis
  • 3. cope with patients emotions

3
Patients
  • Perhaps patients also find it difficult to talk
    to doctors and therefore avoid going to see them.
  • Patients make 11 Lay consultations for every one
    consultation with a doctor (Scambler and Scambler
    1984).

4
Pitts (1991)
  • Pitts (1991) suggests there are three reasons for
    going to the doctor
  • Persistence of symptoms
  • Critical incident - e.g. pain gets worse
  • Expectation of treatment

5
Kent and Dalgleish (1996)
  • Kent and Dalgleish (1996) two types of patient
    satisfaction that should be considered
  • Cognitive satisfaction how happy the patient is
    with the treatment etc.
  • Emotional satisfaction how happy the patient is
    with the doctors level of interest and concern.

6
What patients like
  • People often judge the adequacy of their care by
    criteria that are irrelevant to the technical
    quality of the care. What people do know is
    whether or not they liked the practitioner
    whether he or she was warm and friendly or cool
    and uncommunicative. (Feletti, Firman,
    SansonFisher, 1986 Scarpaci, 1988 Ware et al.,
    1978).

7
Patients are poor judges
  • Even more significant, since people are poor
    judges of technical quality of care, they often
    judge technical quality on the basis of the
    manner in which care is delivered (BenSira, 1976,
    1980). For example, if a physician expresses
    uncertainty about the nature of the patient's
    condition, patient satisfaction declines

8
Mooney, K. M., 2001
  • Mooney, K. M., 2001, 'Predictors of patient
    satisfaction in an outpatient surgery clinic.
    Plastic Surgical Nursing, 21, 3, 162-4

9
Aim
  • To investigate which elements of the
    patient-practitioner relationship lead to
    satisfied patients.

10
Participants
  • An opportunity sample of 345 patients (96 per
    cent of those asked to participate) attending an
    out-patient plastic surgery clinic.
  • Informed consent was obtained.

11
Procedure
  • The participants were required to evaluate items
    such as how long they waited to get an
    appointment, time spent waiting at the surgery
    before the doctor was seen, the explanation given
    about any procedures undergone, the technical
    skills (thoroughness, competence and carefulness)
    of the practitioner and the interpersonal skills
    (courtesy, sensitivity, friendliness etc.) of the
    practitioner on a 5-point scale ranging from poor
    to excellent.

12
Results
  • 60 per cent rated their overall level of
    satisfaction as excellent and 30 per cent as very
    good. The quality of interaction with the
    practitioner received the highest individual
    rating, while those concerned with the facilities
    and access to services were rated lower. The
    interpersonal skills of the doctor were found to
    contribute more to patient satisfaction than the
    technical skills of the doctor and were
    considered to be a better predictor of patient
    satisfaction.

13
Smucker, D. R., Konrad, T. R., Curtis, P., Carey,
T. S., 1998
  • , 'Practitioner self-confidence and patient
    outcomes in acute back pain', Archives of Family
    Medicine, 7, 223-8

14
Participants
  • 189 doctors and chiropractors, randomly selected
    from licensing databases in North Carolina, USA,
    who regularly treated patients for lower back
    pain. Informed consent was obtained.
  •  

15
Procedure
  • The medical practitioners were sent a postal
    questionnaire to complete. The questionnaire
    contained ten items such as, 'I lack the
    diagnostic knowledge and tools to treat someone
    with lower back pain', 'I know exactly what to do
    to treat someone with lower back pain' and 'I
    feel very comfortable treating people with lower
    back pain', which assessed their self-confidence
    (the first four items on the scale) and attitudes
    (the next four items on the scale) in dealing
    with patients with lower back pain.

16
Procedure
  • The last two items dealt with knowledge of the
    progression from acute to chronic low back pain
    and patient satisfaction with treatment. The
    practitioners had to use a 5-point Likert scale
    (1 strongly agree, 5 strongly disagree) to
    record their level of agreement with each
    statement. The scores for the first four items
    were added together to generate a self-confidence
    score for each practitioner and those for the
    next four yielded an attitude score. The last two
    items were treated individually.

17
Procedure
  • The medical practitioners were also asked to
    provide contact details of any patients who came
    to them for treatment for lower back pain and had
    not yet received any treatment. Additionally, all
    the patients had to own a telephone and be able
    to speak English. A total of 1633 patients were
    recruited and informed consent was obtained from
    them. The patients were telephoned immediately
    after their initial visit to their practitioner,
    and again after two, four, eight, 12 and 24 weeks
    or until they had fully recovered from this
    episode of lower back pain.

18
Procedure
  • The length of time until they had returned to a
    level of functioning equal to that before the
    onset of the lower back pain was recorded.
  • The practitioners' self-confidence scores were
    then compared with the length of time taken by
    the patients to return to the same level of
    functioning as prior to the lower back pain.

19
Results
  • 179 (95 per cent) of the 189 practitioners sent
    the questionnaire returned it, and of these 162
    (86 per cent - 107 doctors, 55 chiropractors)
    completed all ten items.
  • A strong correlation was found between scores on
    the first four items (measuring self-confidence)
    and the next four items (measuring attitudes) for
    both doctors and chiropractors. The relationship
    between the item dealing with patient
    satisfaction and the self-confidence score was
    higher for the chiropractors than the doctors.

20
Results
  • Despite differences in levels of self-confidence
    and attitudes among the health practitioners,
    there was no significant relationship for either
    of these factors with the length of time it took
    patients to recover functionality. Thus it is not
    possible to use a practitioner's level of
    self-confidence or attitude as an indicator of
    the speed of recovery from lower back pain.

21
Ogden et al (2002)
  • Ogden et al (2002) explored the impact of the way
    in which uncertainty was expressed (behaviourally
    versus verbally) on doctor's and patient's
    beliefs about patient confidence. Second the
    study examined the role of the patient's personal
    characteristics and knowledge of their doctor as
    a means to address the broader context.

22
Ogden et al (2002)
  • Matched questionnaires were completed by GPs
    (n66, response rate92) and patients (n550,
    response rate88) from practices in the
    south-east of England.

23
Ogden et al (2002)
  • The results showed that the majority of GPs and
    patients viewed verbal expressions of uncertainty
    such as Let's see what happens' as the most
    potentially damaging to patient confidence and
    both GPs and patients believed that asking a
    nurse for advice would have a detrimental effect.

24
Ogden et al (2002)
  • In contrast, behaviours such as using a book or
    computer were seen as benign or even beneficial
    activities. When compared directly, GPs and
    patients agreed about behavioural expressions of
    uncertainty, but the patients rated the verbal
    expressions as more detrimental to their
    confidence than anticipated by the doctors.

25
Ogden et al (2002)
  • In terms of the context, patients who indicated
    that both verbal and behavioural expressions of
    uncertainty would have the most detrimental
    impact upon their confidence were younger, lower
    class and had known their GP for less time.

26
Barnett (2002)
  • Barnett (2002) has found that a quarter of
    surgeons are brusque, unsympathetic or impatient
    when they break bad news to patients. Family
    doctors are better at breaking bad news, but most
    patients are told by surgeons (86). 106 cancer
    patients were interviewed. 94 of these had been
    told by doctors and the rest by family members.

27
Barnett (2002)
  • The patients were asked to rate the way the news
    was delivered in four categories positive,
    neutral, negative and very negative. In 26 per
    cent of the cases, memories of the moment were
    negative or very negative. There were also
    complaints about the lack of clear, simple
    information. (The Times 01-07-02)

28
Doctors are sometimes accused of not listening
  • Beckman and Frankel (1984) studied 74 visits to
    the doctor. In only 23 of the cases did the
    patient have the opportunity to finish his or her
    explanation of concerns.
  • In 69 of the visits, the doctor interrupted,
    directing the patient towards a particular
    disorder.

29
Doctors are sometimes accused of not listening
  • Moreover, on average doctors interrupted after
    their patients had spoken for only 18 seconds.

30
Doctors can be trained in Non-Verbal Communication
  • Birdwhistell (1970) estimated that only 30 to 35
    of the social meaning of a conversation is
    carried by words alone.
  • Non-verbal communication includes features of
    speech such as
  • tone of voice,
  • inflection,
  • rates of speaking,
  • duration and pauses.

31
non-verbal communication
  • Other forms of non-verbal communication are
    conveyed by gestures, dress, physical proximity,
    facial expressions, posture and orientation.

32
Argyle (1975) four major uses
  • To assist speech, for example in synchronising
    conversation or supplementing speech by putting
    stress on certain words, or pausing between words
    or varying the tone and speed of speech
  • As a replacement for speech
  • To signal attitudes, e.g. trying to look cool
  • To signal emotional states, i.e. we can tell how
    a person is really feeling by looking at their
    facial expression or posture.

33
  • Exercise
  • On the card in front of you is written an
    emotion. You have to stand up in front of the
    group and communicate this emotion non-verbally,
    that is you must not use any words. You can
    communicate vocally by altering such things as
    the pitch, tone and volume of your voice by
    counting from 1 to 5 whilst using any other
    non-verbal channel. Other members of the group
    write down the emotion they think is being
    demonstrated as each member takes his turn.

34
Emotional words
  • Fear, Disbelief, Sadness, Dominance, Boredom,
    Disgust, Interest, Shame, Anger, Surprise, Love,
    Embarrassment, Admiration, Happiness

35
Smiling a lot can make people happy.
  • Zuckerman et al (1981) divided males and females
    into three groups.
  • The first group saw a film of a pleasant scene.
  • The second group were shown a film of a neutral
    scene.
  • The third group were shown a nasty film.

36
Within each group
  • a third were asked to suppress their facial
    expressions,
  • a third were asked to exaggerate their facial
    expressions
  • and the other third were not asked to do anything
    apart from watching the film.

37
Results
  • The people who exaggerated their facial
    expressions showed higher levels of arousal and
    reported stronger positive or negative emotional
    reactions, compared with the other two groups.
  • So making patients smile will make them feel
    happier about themselves.
  • Learning to suppress facial expressions at times
    of stress could reduce stress.

38
Doctors dress.
  • McKinstry and Wang (1991) Pictures of same doctor
    dressed formally or informally.
  • Pictures of formally dressed doctors rated
    higher for the amount of confidence the patients
    had in them, and on how happy they would be to
    see them.
  • Older and professional-class patients
    particularly preferred the formally dressed
    doctors.

39
Touch
  • Jourard (1966) considered where it is acceptable
    to be touched and by whom.
  • Doctors need to be careful not to alarm the
    patient by touching them in a 'no go' area
    without their permission.

40
(No Transcript)
41
Cultural differences
  • Jourard (1966) also found cultural differences in
    the amount of touching. Observing people in cafes
    around the world he counted the number of times
    people touched each other during the course of
    one hour. His results were

42
Touch
Place Number of touches
San Juan (Puerto Rico) 180
Paris 110
London 0
43
(No Transcript)
44
British Nurses
  • Davitz Davitz (1985) report that American
    patients' perceptions of British nurses might be
    influenced by different cultural norms
  • The expression of a range of emotions on the
    part of American patients, in many situations,
    often made the British nurses uncomfortable and
    even more reserved. It is interesting to note
    that a number of patients whom we interviewed
    judged this discomfort as dislike, insensitive,
    and hard-boiled. 'They're efficient,' noted one
    patient, 'but they're not sympathetic.'

45
Whitcher Fisher (1979)
  • A second piece of research highlights the status
    differences involved in touching. Whitcher
    Fisher (1979) arranged for nurses to either touch
    or not touch patients while providing them with
    information about impending operations. The
    nurses in the'touch condition'touched the
    patients on the hand whilst showing them a
    booklet describing the operation, whereas those
    in the 'no touch' condition did not touch the
    patients at all. All the nurses were female. The
    patients were asked for their views about the
    hospital and the prospective operation.

46
Whitcher Fisher (1979)
  • After the operation, the patients' blood pressure
    was measured. Female patients touched by nurses
    reported lower anxiety, more positive feelings to
    the hospital and had lower blood pressure after
    the operation than those not touched. On the
    other hand, male patients who were touched
    reported greater anxiety, more negative feelings
    and higher blood pressure after the operation
    than those who were not touched.

47
Whitcher Fisher (1979)
  • Whitcher Fisher (1979) suggest that one
    explanation for these results stems from status
    differences. Higher status individuals are at
    liberty to touch lower status individuals, but
    not vice versa. Thus females perceived the
    touching as a sign of caring and warmth males
    perceived it as a threatening gesture, which
    communicated the nurses' superior status in the
    hospital setting.

48
questioning
  • 1.   the most important part of questioning is
    listening
  • 2.   determine the reasons for asking the
    questions
  • 3.   do not ask too many questions

49
questions fall into the following categories
  • closed questions
  • open questions
  • affective questions
  • probing questions
  • leading questions

50
closed questions
  • closed questions are questions which require very
    short answers and are useful for anxious or
    nervous people so that tension can be reduced.
    Asking too many closed questions means that the
    doctor has to ask lots of questions to get
    information and they spend less time listening to
    the patient. As an exercise try asking a friend a
    series of closed questions for as long as
    possible.

51
open questions
  • open questions give the respondents the
    opportunity to respond in anyway they wish. There
    is no correct answer. A disadvantage is
    curtailing rambling irrelevances, though the use
    of well timed closed questions can bring a
    wandering conversation back to the issue at hand.

52
three main types of sequences
  • Funnelling. Beginning and interview with an open
    question and gradually becoming more specific.
  • Inverse funnelling. Going from specific details
    to general topics.
  • The Tunnel. Asking a series of closed questions.

53
Jesudason (1976)
  • Jesudason (1976) compared open and closed
    questions in finding out what foods were taboo
    during lactation (mothers producing milk for
    their babies) for Indian women. The sample
    consisted of 1151 women who were asked either to
    name the foods that were taboo (open) or were
    read out a list of 12 foods and asked whether
    they ate each food during lactation (closed).

54
Jesudason (1976)
  • About 53 did not report any food taboos when
    given the question in open form. When these women
    were read the list of 12 foods, 32 considered
    five or more items taboo.

55
affective questions.
  • affective questions. These are questions about
    the patients feelings and emotions and help to
    communicate concern and empathy.

56
Probing questions.
  • These questions are used to get a patient talking
    when they are not forthcoming. Hackney and
    Cormier (1979) suggests the use of the "accent"
    and "minimal" prompt. The accent is a short
    re-statement that echoes and focuses a previous
    statement.

57
Probing questions.
  • The minimal prompts use a large number of
    non-verbal responses such as "uh-huh", "mmm",
    "ah", and "yes, I see." Non-verbal behaviours
    such as leaning forward would also act as
    prompts. A problem with using too many probing
    questions is that the interview can become an
    interrogation.

58
leading questions
  • Conversational lead.
  • Pressurised agreements.
  • Hidden subtleties.

59
conversational lead.
  • This type of leading question is used to
    anticipate agreement with the patient and thus
    convey the impression of friendliness and
    attentiveness. An example would be "isn't she a
    marvellous cook?".

60
pressurised agreements.
  • This type of question puts pressure on people to
    agree with the questioner. For example "you do,
    of course, brush your teeth every day?". These
    types of questions should be avoided by doctors
    because it leads to invalid responses.

61
hidden subtleties.
  1. This type of question leads the respondent
    without their knowledge. Loftus (1975)
    interviewed 40 people about headaches and
    headache products, ostensibly for market
    research.

62
hidden subtleties.
  • They were asked either "do you get headaches
    frequently, and if so, how often?" Or "do you get
    headaches occasionally, and if so, how often?"
  • The average number of headaches in the
    "frequently" group was 2.2 Whereas in the
    "occasionally" group it was 0.7 headaches a week.

63
hidden subtleties.
  • The subjects were also asked how many products
    they had tried for the headaches. One group was
    given a choice of one, two, or three Another the
    choice of one, five, or ten. The first groups
    average was 3.3, the second's 5.2. Similar
    effects can be used by substituting "short" with
    "tall" or "the" with "a".

64
Savage and Armstrong (1990)
  • Savage and Armstrong (1990) found that patients
    were more satisfied with a directed
    consultation rather than a sharing
    consultation.

65
Savage and Armstrong (1990)
  • Directed consultation statements made such as
    you are suffering from, it is essential that
    you take this medication, you should be better
    in . days, come and see me in . days.
  • Sharing consultation what do you think that is
    wrong?, Would you like a prescription?, Are
    there any other problems?, When would you like
    to come and see me again?

66
Savage and Armstrong (1990)
  • 359 randomly selected patients free to choose
    their doctor. 200 results used.
  • 2 questionnaires one immediately and one a week
    later.
  • Results overall a high level of satisfaction,
    but higher for directed group. Higher for
    satisfaction with explanation of doctor and
    with own understanding of the problem. More
    likely to report that they had been greatly
    helped.

67
The end
Write a Comment
User Comments (0)
About PowerShow.com