Week 4. Null subjects (and some more root infinitives) - PowerPoint PPT Presentation

About This Presentation
Title:

Week 4. Null subjects (and some more root infinitives)

Description:

GRS LX 700 Language Acquisition and Linguistic Theory Week 4. Null subjects (and some more root infinitives) Null subjects (in English) Until after around 2 years old ... – PowerPoint PPT presentation

Number of Views:378
Avg rating:3.0/5.0
Slides: 105
Provided by: PaulHa53
Learn more at: https://www.bu.edu
Category:

less

Transcript and Presenter's Notes

Title: Week 4. Null subjects (and some more root infinitives)


1
GRS LX 700Language Acquisition andLinguistic
Theory
  • Week 4. Null subjects(and some more root
    infinitives)

2
Null subjects (in English)
  • Until after around 2 years old, kids will often
    omit subjects
  • Drop bean.
  • Fix Mommy shoe.
  • Helping Mommy.
  • Want go get it.
  • Why?

3
Null subjects
  • Lots of languages allow you to drop the subject.
  • Italian, Spanish the verb generally carries
    enough inflection to identify the person, number
    of the subject.
  • Chinese where the subject is obvious from
    context it can be left out.
  • Not in English though Lets talk about Bill.
    Left. Bought groceries. Dropped eggs.
  • On the view that kids know language, but are just
    trying to figure out the specific details
    (principles and parameters), one possibility is
    that they always start out speaking Italian (or
    Chinese) until they get evidence to the contrary.
  • Null subjects are grammatical for kids

4
Null subjects
  • Kids do tend to speak in short sentences. There
    seem to in fact be identifiable stages in terms
    of the length of the kids sentences (one-word
    stage, two-word stage, multi-word stage), often
    measured in terms of MLU (mean length of
    utterance) which roughly corresponds to
    linguistic development.
  • Perhaps the kids just trying to say a three-word
    sentence in a two-word window, so something has
    to go.
  • That is, some kind of processing limitation.

5
Subject vs. object drop
A E S
Subject 57 61 43
Object 8 7 15
6
Null subjects
  • Subjects (in a non-null subject language like
    English) are way more likely to be dropped than
    objects. Theres something special about
    subjects.
  • Makes a processing account more difficult to
    justify.
  • Bloom (1990) made some well-known proposals about
    how the null subject phenomenon could be seen as
    a processing issue, and tried to explain why
    subjects are the most susceptible to being
    dropped.
  • See also Hyams Wexler (1993) for a reply.

7
Null subjects vs. time
  • Null subjects seem to be pretty robustly confined
    to a certain portion of linguistic development.
    Theres a pretty sharp dropoff at around 2.5 or
    3.
  • Hamanns Danish kids illustrate this well.

8
Why cant English kids really be speaking Italian?
  • In Italian, subjects can be dropped (but need not
    be), in English, they cant be dropped at all.
  • So since having subjects is consistent with
    Italian, whats going to signal to the kid that
    theyve got the wrong kind of language?
  • A subset problem.
  • Possible solution? Expletive it and there.
  • In Italian, null subjects are allowed wherever a
    subject pronoun would be, including embedded
    finite clauses (I know that he has left) and
    finite root questions (What has he bought?).
  • In Kid English, null subjects never show up in
    these environments. It doesnt seem so much like
    Italian.

9
Ok, maybe these kids are speaking Chinese
  • In adult Chinese, subjects can also be omitted.
  • In Italian, Spanish, the allowability of null
    subjects was taken to be tied to the verbal
    agreement. Something about the rich agreement
    licenses null subjects.
  • In Chinese, there is no agreement morphology, so
    that isnt whats allowing null subjects.
  • Proposal What allows argument omission in
    Chinese is a form of topic drop. They are allowed
    roughly when they are old information,
    recoverable.

10
Speaking Chinese?
  • Suppose that these are parameters.
  • Pro-drop for the Italian/English difference.
  • Topic-drop for the Chinese/English difference.
  • Kid English isnt Pro-drop.
  • In Topic-drop languages, subjects arent
    particularly privileged.
  • Subjects are often old information, but when
    objects are old information, they too can be
    dropped.

11
Not speaking Chinese
  • Weve already seen that Kid English
    overwhelmingly drops subjects, not objects.
  • 33 subjects,4 objects(Wang et al 1992)
  • Kid English looks like English with some extra
    null subjects.
  • But Kid Chinese drops even more subjects and lots
    more objects.
  • 47 subjects,23 objects.
  • Kid Chinese looks like Chinese with maybe some
    extra null subjects.

12
Parameters are quick
  • And recall that Italian allows null subjects in
    embedded clauses, wh-questions, etc.
  • Kid Dutch and French have practically no null
    subjects in wh-questions.
  • Kid Italian has something like 56 null subjects
    in wh-questions.
  • If Chinese/Dutch is distinguished by
    topic-drop and Italian/English is
    distinguished by pro-drop, the kids already
    know what theyre trying to speak by the time
    were testing them.

13
Processing accounts?
  • Kids have severely limited processing power, and
    so they leave off subjects to ease the load.
    (Bloom 1990)
  • In favor
  • Length limitations even in imitations
  • Kids omit things other than subjects
  • Some kids dont eliminate subjects, only reduce
    their frequency.

14
Processing accounts
  • Contra? Hyams points out
  • Build houseCathy build house
  • Go nurseryLucy go nursery
  • Kathryn want build another house.
  • Bloom So, no absolute limit on length, only a
    tendency to reduce length.

15
Bloom (1990)
  • Bloom (1970) found
  • negated sentences tend to lack subjects more
    frequently then non-negated sentences.
  • Bloom (1990)
  • Hypothesis sentences without subjects will have
    longer VPs than sentences with subjects.
  • Looked at past tense verbs and cognitive states
    (need) to avoid any confusion with imperatives.

16
Bloom (1990)
  • VP length (words from verb to the end) counted
    for sentences with and without subjects.
  • Results Mean length of VP in sentences with
    subjects were (statistically) significantly
    shorter than those without.
  • E.g., Adam 2.333 with, 2.604 without.

17
Bloom (1990)
  • In fact, long subjects (lexical subjects),
    short subjects (pronouns), and null subjects
    correlated with an increase in VP length as well.

18
Bloom (1990)
  • And why are subjects dropped more frequently than
    objects?
  • Two possibilities?
  • Subjects tend to be given (old) information (low
    informativeness, more expendable)
  • Maybe processing saves the heaviest load for
    last

19
Hyams Wexler (1993)
  • Blooms (1990) approach (processing) cant be
    right either.
  • The difference between subjects and objects is
    big, and only rate of subject drop changes.
  • Adam Eve both drop around 40-50 of their
    subjects in an early stage, and in a later stage
    are down to 15-30meanwhile their rate of object
    drop stays around 5-10.

20
Hyams Wexler (1993)
  • Informativeness?
  • All else being equal, the ratio of missing
    subjects to specific subjects should be equal to
    the ratio of missing objects to specific
    objects.
  • Turns out that kids drop specific subjects about
    twice as often (Adam 52) as they drop specific
    objects (Adam 21).

21
Hyams Wexler (1993)
  • Considering Italian adults, we find exactly the
    same correlation Bloom reported for English kids
    VP seems to be longer where there is null
    subject, shorter with a pronoun, and shorter
    still with a lexical subject.

22
Hyams Wexler (1993)
  • Regardless of why the correlation holds, if it is
    a processing deficiency in kids, what is it for
    the Italian adults?
  • Seems like kids act like theyre speaking a
    language where the null subject is a grammatical
    option. Note might be slightly different from a
    null subject language though. Point dropping
    subjects is grammatical for these kids, not an
    error.

23
Hyams Wexler (1993)
  • Output omission model predicts ratio of overt
    lexical subjects to overt pronouns should
    increase over time.
  • Pronouns are easier, theyll survive. Lexical
    subjects are harder, theyll be dropped. Initial
    advantage to visible pronouns.
  • Grammatical omission model predicts ratio of
    overt lexical subjects to overt pronouns should
    decrease over time.
  • If null subjects are a form of pronoun for kids,
    they will dilute the pool, putting visible
    pronouns at an initial disadvantage.

24
Hyams Wexler (1993)
  • We find Ratio of overt lexical subjects to overt
    pronouns decreases over time
  • Adam goes from about 31 in favor of lexical
    subjects (during subject drop stage) to 12
    (after subject drop stage).
  • When hes dropping subjects, they are coming out
    of the pronoun pilethe number of lexical
    subjects is staying about the same across
    development.

25
Hyams Wexler (1993)
  • Ok, so maybe pronouns are more difficult than
    lexical nouns? (Doesnt fit well with the length
    of VP result, but maybe?)
  • Problem is kids show a steady level of object
    pronouns throughout this time periodand output
    omission model doesnt have anything to say about
    subject vs. object.

26
Hyams Wexler (1993)
  • Basic conclusion
  • Null subjects dont seem to arise in child
    language solely due to processing difficulty.
  • Rather, they seem to be allowed in the child
    grammar.
  • This allows a distinction between subject (high
    rate of omission) and object (low rate of
    omission)
  • Explains the tradeoff between null subjects and
    pronouns (and the VP length/subject correlation)
    if the principles governing availability of
    subject drop are similar to those at work in
    Italian.

27
So what allows null subjects?
  • Heres where we start to tie in to other
    properties of that age.
  • Notice that in English (a non-null subject
    language) you can have a grammatical null subject
    in one context
  • I want Ø to have a fire drill
  • Ø to have a fire drill would make my day.

28
So what allows null subjects?
  • Subjects of infinitives can be null.
  • Kids at the age where subjects are often missing
    often use infinitive verb forms.
  • Perhaps thats the key Since kids can use
    infinitives where adults cant (main clause main
    verb), this allows them to use null subjects in
    those sentences as a side effect.

29
Proportion of null subjects in finite and
non-finite clauses
30
Null subjects
  • Null subject parameter(s) is/are not initially
    mis-set (kids dont all start off speaking
    Italian or Chinesecontra Hyams 1986, 1992)
    rather, child null subjects are (at least in
    part) due to the availability of non-finite verbs
    (the OI stage).
  • Most null subjects are licensed by being the
    subject of a nonfinite verb (i.e. PRO)
  • But there are still some null subjects with
    finite verbs Well return to this.

31
Null subjects and C
  • Crisma (1992) French kids typically (1/114 1
    vs. 407/100241) do not produce null subjects
    with a wh-phrase.
  • Valian (1991) English kids typically (9/5522)
    do not produce null subjects with a wh-phrase.
  • Poeppel Wexler (1993) German kids typically
    exclude null subjects from post-V2 position.

32
Null subjects and C
  • It looks like If the kid shows evidence of CP
    (wh-words, V2), then the kid also does not drop
    the subject.
  • Rizzis idea
  • A discourse-licensed null subject is available
    only in the highest specifier in the tree
    (topic-drop).
  • Axiom CProot
  • Kids dont get the axiom until between 2-3
    years old.

33
Truncated trees
  • The result (of not having CProot) is that kids
    are allowed to have truncated structurestrees
    that look like adult trees with the tops chopped
    off.
  • Importantly The kids dont just leave stuff
    outthey just stop the tree early. So, if the
    kid leaves out a functional projection, s/he
    leaves out all higher XPs as well.

34
Truncation
  • If kid selects anything lower than TP as the
    root, the result is a root infinitivewhich can
    be as big as any kind of XP below TP in the
    structure.
  • Note in particular, though, it cant be a CP.
  • So we expect that evidence of CP will correlate
    with finite verbs.

35
Truncation
  • Pierce (1989) looking at French observed that
    there are almost no root infinitives with subject
    cliticsthis is predicted if these clitics are
    instances of subject agreement in AgrS if there
    is no TP, there can be no AgrSP.

36
Truncation
  • There is some dispute in the syntax literature as
    to whether the position of NegP (the projection
    responsible for the negative morpheme) is higher
    or lower than TP in the tree.
  • If NegP is higher than TP, we would expect not to
    find negative root infinitives.

37
Truncation and NegP
  • But we do find negative Root Infinitives(Pierce
    1989) in the acquisition of French, negation
    follows finite verbs and preceds nonfinite verbs
    (that isFrench kids know the movement properties
    of finiteness, and thus they have the concept of
    finiteness).

38
Truncation and NegP
  • So, is TP higher than NegP?
  • Hard to say conclusively from the existing French
    data because there are not many negative root
    infinitivesbut further study of child language
    could lead to a theoretical result of this sort
    about the adult languages, if we assume a
    truncation analysis of child language.

39
S O Vfin AgrSP?
  • Usually (Poeppel Wexler 1993) German kids put
    finite verbs in second position, and leave
    nonfinite verbs at the end.
  • Occasionally one finds a finite verb at the end.
  • Rizzi suggests we could look at this as an
    instance of a kid choosing AgrSP as root, where
    CP is necessary to trigger V2.
  • PW had to basically consider these noise.

40
Truncation and null subjects
  • As for null subjects
  • If the tree is just a VP, the subject can be
    omitted in its base positionits still in the
    specifier of the root.
  • If the tree is just a TP, the subject can be
    omitted from the normal subject positionnote
    that this would be a finite verb with a null
    subject.
  • If the tree is a CP and SpecCP is filled (like in
    a wh-question) we expect no null subjects.

41
Null subject languages vs. root infinitives
  • Italian seems to show no (or very very few) root
    infinitives. If this is maturation of RootCP
    how could languages vary?
  • Rizzi suggests
  • In English, V doesnt move
  • In French, tensed verbs move to AgrS (I),
    untensed verbs may move to AgrS
  • In Italian, all verbs move to AgrS

42
Null subject languages vs. root infinitives
  • The idea is that a verb in Italian needs to get
    to AgrSit has a feature/property (parametric)
    that marks it as needing to get to AgrS in a
    grammatical sentence. Hence, the kid needs AgrS.
  • English verbs have no such need, so the English
    kids have to rely on RootCP to tell them to keep
    going.

43
Null subject languages vs. root infinitives
  • Rizzi and Wexler capture NS/OI similarly
  • Wexler AgrS does not need a subject in its
    specifier in Italian, so there is no competition
    between AgrS and T, and thus no need for root
    infinitives. AgrS and T are always both there.
  • Rizzi AgrS can never be omitted in Italian,
    because the verb needs AgrS to be there. Having
    AgrS implies T. AgrS and T are always both there.

44
Back to null subjects vs. Fin
  • Bromberg Wexler (1995) promote the idea that
    null subjects with finite verbs arise from a kind
    of topic drop (available to adults in special
    contexts).
  • Proposal (Bromberg Wexler)Topic-drop applies
    to Very Strong TopicsKids sometimes take (in
    reality) non-VS topics to be VS topics (a
    pragmatic error)

45
Prediction about NS
  • RIs have two ways of licensing NSs
  • PRO (regular licensing of null subject)
  • Topic drop
  • Finite verbs have one way to license a NS
  • Topic drop
  • So We expect more null subjects with root
    infinitives (which we in fact see).
  • Cf. Rizzi Subject in highest specifier can
    always be dropped, and RIs also allow PRO. Same
    story, basically.

46
Bromberg, Wexler, wh-questions, and null subjects
  • If topic drop is something which drops a topic in
    SpecCP
  • and if wh-words also move to SpecCP
  • we would not expect null subjects with
    non-subject (e.g., where) wh-questions where the
    verb is finite (so PRO is not licensed).
  • Cf. Rizzi Same prediction if you have a CP, a
    subject in SpecTP wont be in the highest
    specifier, so it cant be dropped. One
    difference Rizzi predicts no nonfinite
    wh-questions at all, hence no null subjects at
    all.

47
Bromberg, Wexler, wh-questions, and null subjects
  • Finiteness of null/pronominal subjects, Adams
    wh-questions (Bromberg Wexler 1995)

Finite Nonfinite
Null 2 118
Pronoun 117 131
48
Truncation
  • Rizzis truncation theory predicts
  • No wh-questions with root infinitives
  • wh-question ? CP, but
  • CP ? IP, and
  • IP ? finite verb
  • And of course we wouldnt expect null subjects in
    wh-questions if null subjects are allowed (only)
    in the specifier of the root.

49
Truncation?
  • Guasti points out that although Bromberg Wexler
    did find null subjects in wh-questions in
    English, English is weird in this respect.
  • Arguably, null subjects are precluded from
    wh-questions in most other languages.

50
V2 and wh-null subjects
  • German and Dutch have extremely few root
    infinitives when there is anything in SpecCP.
  • This does go with Rizzis prediction
  • But they are V2 languagesfinite verbs are what
    you find in C, and when SpecCP is filled, there
    must be something in C. Hence, Wexlers
    prediction seems to be
  • V2 language ? no wh-question root infinitives
  • And this seems closer to accurate, given English.

51
V2 and wh-null subjects
  • And yet, Crismas (1992) findings and Hamann
    Plunketts (1998) findings suggest that French
    (not V2) also shows almost no null subjects in
    wh-questions.
  • So whats different about English?
  • French, Dutch, German basically never have null
    subjects in wh-questions.
  • English allows them readily.

52
Adult null subjects(diary drop)
  • Both Rizzi and Bromberg Wexler appeal to
    properties of adult language to justify the child
    null subjects.
  • BW suggest that topic drop is available in
    English, but only for Very Strong topics, and
    what kids are doing wrong is identifying far too
    many things as VS topics.
  • Rizzi suggests that the ability to drop a subject
    in the highest specifier is available in certain
    registers (diary drop) (where presumably
    RootCP is disregarded, or at least relaxed to
    allow RootIP).
  • Saw John today. Looked tired.

53
Hamann Plunkett (1998)
  • Finite null subjects. Hamann discussed this
    question If null subjects are licensed by RIs,
    what should we say about the null subjects with
    finite verbs? W had previously said topic drop,
    but H showed that Danish kids use of null
    subjects with finite verbs correlated highly with
    the use of RIs in general.
  • Thats a problem because topic drop according
    to BW is due to kids mistaking what can be a VS
    topic, and should be independent of Tense/Agr.
    For truncation, though, the same basic mechanism
    is at work creating both finite null subjects and
    RIs.

54
Root infinitives vs. time
  • The timing on root infinitives is pretty robust,
    ending around 3 years old.

55
Wexler (2000)
  • Are there really lots of null subjects with
    finite verbs in Danish?
  • Idea køb-er looks like present tense finite, but
    it could be missing T (hence legitimately license
    NS).
  • Agr, Tns køb-er (present) (adult)
  • -Agr, Tns køb-e (infinitive) no NS allowed
  • -Agr, -Tns køb-e (infinitive) NS allowed
  • Agr, -Tns køb-er (present) NS allowed.
  • Predicts No NSs with past tense verbs like
    køb-de (since unambiguously Tns, which is the
    thing that prevents NS). True?

56
Hamann (2002) vs. Wexler
  • Well, not really vanishingly small
  • Jens (20-34 mos.s) 14/42 (33) NS past.
  • Anne (18-30 mos.) 13/33 (39) NS past.
  • Hamann herself prefers a truncation story to
    account for these finite NS corresponds to
    truncating at TP.
  • Yet, dont forget about Swahili, and the
    apparently visible effects of ATOM.

57
Interpretation and functional categories
  • A basic premise of Hoekstra Hyams (1998) is
    that tense is a means of connecting between the
    structural meaning and the discourse. Tense
    anchors a sentence in the discourse.
  • They propose that the relation between discourse
    (CP) and T must be signaled (to ground an
    utterance), and is signaled by different things
    in different languages.
  • Dutch number morphology ? only these have RIs?
  • Japanese tense morphology
  • Italian, Spanish, Catalan person morphology

58
Underspecification of number?
  • HH propose in light of this that whats wrong
    with kids has to do with number specifically. OI
    languages are those where number is crucial in
    the finite inflection.
  • HH picked up on something about when these RIs
    seem to be used. It seemed that there are certain
    verbs that showed up in the nonfinite form, but
    others that didnt.

59
Eventivity Constraint
  • In particular, it seems that RIs show up only
    with verbs referring to events not with verbs
    referring to states, not with auxiliary verbs.
    Finite verbs seem to have no such restriction.
    Original research on Dutch on French, also
    Russian.
  • Eventivity ConstraintRIs are restricted to
    event-denoting predicates.

60
Modal Reference Effect
  • The other thing is that RIs often have a modal
    meaning (can, will, must, want to..) (pretty
    dramatic in Dutch, German, French).
  • Poeppel Wexler (1993) did give a German example
    from Andreas that showed an RI with seemingly no
    modal meaning (Thorsten Ball haben) if HH are
    right, this was noise.
  • Modal Reference EffectWith overwhelming
    frequency, RIs have modal interpretations.

61
English weird
  • English doesnt seem to conform to the pattern.
    Ud Deen (1997) found
  • plenty of bare stative verbs (EC)
  • Man have it
  • Ann need Mommy napkin
  • Papa want apple
  • plenty of non-modal bare verbs (MRE)
  • Dutch 86 of RIs have modal meaning. Cf. 3of
    finite forms.
  • English 13 of bare forms have modal meaning Cf,
    12 of finite forms..

62
HHs hypothesis
  • Number is an inflectional property both of the
    nominal and the verbal system.
  • though it arises in the nominal system.
  • Missing determiners and RIs are both a symptom of
    underspecified Number.
  • Spec-head agreementcommunicates
    number(under)specificationto the verb.

63
HH (1998) BUCLD
  • Looked at Niek (CHILDES, Dutch).
  • They found that with finite DPs, the verb was
    pretty much always finite too.
  • They found that with nonfinite DPs, the verb
    was somewhat more likely to be nonfinite than
    with a finite DP, but still overwhelmingly
    favored finite DPs.
  • Only null subjects didnt overwhelmingly favor
    finite V. (NS 45 nonfinite).

64
HH (1998) BUCLD
  • All things being equal, we might have expected a
    11 correlation between finite DP subjects and
    finite V, if it were a matter of Spec-head
    agreement. We dont have that. We have a
    one-directional relation.
  • If DP is finite, V is finite.
  • If V is nonfinite, DP is nonfinite.

65
HH (1998) BUCLD
  • In a sense, one setting cares about its partner
    in the Spec-Head relationship, and the other
    setting doesnt.
  • Finite V seems not to care whether the subject is
    finite or not.
  • Nonfinite V does seem to care, and requires a
    nonfinite subject.
  • More specifically, there is a default, and the
    default does not need to be licensed (and
    non-defaults do).
  • This goes along with an assumption that either
    the syntax doesnt make person distinctions if
    the morphology doesnt, or that this part of
    checking is really about morphology.

66
HH (1998) BUCLD
  • In Dutch, 3sg is default.
  • 1sg verb licensed only by a 1sg subject.
  • 3sg verb licensed by any old subject.
  • In English, 3sg is not the default. Its the one
    marked form.
  • 3sg verb licensed only by a 3sg subject.
  • bare verb licensed by any old subject.

67
Thus
  • The doggie bark.
  • He bark
  • Doggie sit here.
  • Doggie barks.
  • Het hondje hier zitten.
  • He hier zitten.
  • Hondje hier zitten.
  • Hondje zit hier.

68
cf. Schütze Wexler
  • the English bare form is ambiguous between an
    infinitive and a finite form (HH98101)
  • Although stated in different terminology, and
    addressing a slightly different arena of facts,
    the basic concept is the same as that in SW96.
  • TA -gt finite (-s)
  • T-A, -TA, -T-A -gt nonfinite (stem)
  • but A ones will have A properties (e.g. NOM),
    even if just a stem form. Same for T.

69
English bare form ? infinitive
  • SW and HH agree that the English bare form
    isnt strictly speaking (necessarily) the true
    infinitive.

70
HH and interpretation
  • Claim RIs are interpreted as -realized, the
    contribution of the infinitival morpheme itself.
  • Languages with an infinitival morpheme and actual
    RIs should show 100 modal (-realized)
    interpretation with RIs.
  • English, with a Ø infinitival morpheme, obscures
    the correlation in practice, we expect only some
    (the actually infinitive) bare forms to be modal.

71
epistemic vs. deontic
  • John must leave.
  • Deontic About the way the world isnt now but
    needs to be.
  • John must know French.
  • Epistemic About our beliefs about the world.
  • Seems to be a correlation between eventivity
    and modality type, in the adult language.

72
Modality and kids
  • In other circles of research, people have
    proposed that kids basically dont have
    epistemic uses of modality (John must be a
    genius) before about 3 years oldfor whatever
    reason.
  • If thats true, theres only deontic modality
    (John must go to class).
  • If deontic modality only goes with eventive
    predicates, were done. Kids RIs are modal,
    necessarily deontic, hence necessarily with
    eventive verbs.

73
English must be different
  • English bare forms are not (necessarily)
    infinitives, not necessarily modal, hence not
    necessarily deontic, eventive.
  • Hence, the EC and MRE appear not to hold of
    English, but for reasons we can now understand.

74
A pause to regroup
  • English bare form is unmarked, only -s is
    unambiguously TA.
  • Do is a reflex of T (and/or A), and as
    expected, almost never in negative sentences was
    there a post-negation inflected verb (she doesnt
    go vs. she not goes).
  • The actual infinitive morpheme in English is Ø,
    so we cant differentiate bare forms between
    infinitives and other bare forms.
  • The infinitive morpheme seems to carry modal
    meaningin languages where you can see it you can
    tell. Effectively RI only with eventives.

75
A pause to regroup
  • HH propose that the languages which show OIs are
    those which rely (only) on number in their
    inflectional system. Those that dont (Japanese
    tense only, Italian person) seem to be
    immune. Hence, person is the special, possibly
    omitted thing for kids.
  • This isnt really distinctly at odds with ATOM.
    Wexler suggests that the problem is with
    double-movement of the subject, but movement of
    the subject might itself be driven by person
    features in recent versions of the syntactic thy.

76
A pause to regroup
  • HH observed a correlation between specified
    (finite) subjects and verbal form.
  • Specifically,finite subjects seem to cause
    finite verbs. Not obvious why this would be under
    ATOM directly, but it might be something like
    what HH suggestthere is feature sharing between
    the subject and the AgrP. It might be interesting
    to see if finite subjects necessarily always
    show the reflex of AgrP and not necessarily of TP.

77
Legendre et al. (2000)
  • Wexler During OI stage, kids sometimes omit T,
    and sometimes omit Agr. Based on a choice of
    which to violate, the requirement to have T, to
    have Agr, to have only one.
  • (cf. Kids in a pickle slide)
  • Legendre et al. Looking at development (of
    French), it appears that the choice of what to
    omit is systematic we propose a system to
    account for (predict) the proportion of the time
    kids omit T, Agr, both, neither, in progressive
    stages of development.

78
Optimality Theory
  • Legendre et al. (2000) is set in the Optimality
    Theory framework (often seen in phonology, less
    often seen applied to syntax).
  • Grammar is a system of ranked and violable
    constraints

79
Optimality Theory
  • In our analysis, one constraint is Parse-T, which
    says that tense must be realized in a clause. A
    structure without tense (where TP has been
    omitted, say) will violate this constraint.
  • Another constraint is F (Dont have a
    functional category). A structure with TP will
    violate this constraint.

80
Optimality Theory
  • Parse-T and F are in conflictit is impossible
    to satisfy both at the same time.
  • When constraints conflict, the choice made (on a
    language-particular basis) of which constraint is
    considered to be more important (more highly
    ranked) determines which constraint is satisfied
    and which must be violated.

81
Optimality Theory
  • So if F gtgt Parse-T, TP will be omitted.
  • and if Parse-T gtgt F, TP will be included.

82
Optimality Theory
  • Grammar involves constraints on the
    representations (e.g., SS, LF, PF, or perhaps a
    combined representation).
  • The constraints exist in all languages.
  • Where languages differ is in how important each
    constraint is with respect to each other
    constraint.

83
Optimality Theory big picture
  • Universal Grammar is the constraints that
    languages must obey.
  • Languages differ only in how those constraints
    are ranked relative to one another. (So,
    parameter ranking)
  • The kids job is to re-rank constraints until
    they match the order which generated the input
    that s/he hears.

84
Legendre et al. (2000)
  • Proposes a system to predict the proportions of
    the time kids choose the different options among
  • Omit TP
  • Omit AgrSP
  • Omit both TP and AgrSP
  • Include both TP and AgrSP (violating UCC)

85
French v. English
  • English TAgr is pronounced like
  • /s/ if we have features 3, sg, present
  • /ed/ if we have the feature past
  • /Ø/ otherwise
  • French TAgr is pronounced like
  • danser NRF
  • a dansé (3sg) past
  • je danse 1sg (present)
  • jai dansé 1sg past

86
The idea
  • Kids are subject to conflicting constraints
  • Parse-T Include a projection for tense
  • Parse-Agr Include a project for agreement
  • F Dont complicate your tree with functional
    projections
  • F2 Dont complicate your tree so much as to
    have two functional projections.

87
The idea
  • Sometimes Parse-T beats out F, and then theres
    a TP. Or Parse-Agr beats out F, and then theres
    an AgrP. Or both Parse-T and Parse-Agr beat out
    F2, and so theres both a TP and an AgrP.
  • But what does sometimes mean?

88
Floating constraints
  • The innovation in Legendre et al. (2000) that
    gets us off the ground is the idea that as kids
    re-rank constraints, the position of the
    constraint in the hierarchy can get somewhat
    fuzzy, such that two positions can
    overlap. F Parse-T

89
Floating constraints
  • F Parse-T
  • When the kid evaluates a form in the constraint
    system, the position of Parse-T is fixed
    somewhere in the rangeand winds up sometimes
    outranking, and sometimes outranked by, F.

90
Floating constraints
  • F Parse-T
  • (Under certain assumptions) this predicts that we
    would see TP in the structure 50 of the time,
    and see structures without TP the other 50 of
    the time.

91
French kid data
  • Looked at 3 French kids from CHILDES
  • Broke development into stages based on a modified
    MLU-type measure based on how long most of their
    utterances were (2 words, more than 2 words) and
    how many of the utterances contain verbs.
  • Looked at tense and agreement in each of the
    three stages represented in the data.

92
French kid data
  • Kids start out using 3sg agreement and present
    tense for practically everything (correct or
    not).
  • We took this to be a default
  • (No agreement? Pronounce it as 3sg. No tense?
    pronounce it as present. Neither? Pronounce it as
    an infinitive.).

93
French kid data
  • This means if a kid uses 3sg or present tense, we
    cant tell if they are really using 3sg (they
    might be) or if they are not using agreement at
    all and just pronouncing the default.
  • So, we looked at non-present tense forms and
    non-3sg forms only to avoid the question of the
    defaults.

94
French kids data
  • We found that tense and agreement develop
    differentlyspecifically, in the first stage we
    looked at, kids were using tense fine, but then
    in the next stage, they got worse as the
    agreement improved.
  • Middle stage looks likecompetition between
    Tand Agr for a single node.

95
A detail about counting
  • We counted non-3sg and non-present verbs.
  • In order to see how close kids utterances were
    to adults utterances, we need to know how often
    adults use non-3sg and non-present, and then see
    how close the kids are to matching that level.
  • So, adults use non-present tense around 31 of
    the timeso when a kid uses 31 non-present
    tense, we take that to be 100 success
  • In the last stage we looked at, kids were
    basically right at the 100 success level for
    both tense and agreement.

96
Proportion of non-present and non-3sg verbs
97
Proportion of non-finite root forms
98
A model to predict the percentages
  • Stage 3b (first stage)
  • no agreement
  • about 1/3 NRFs, 2/3 tensed forms F2 FParse
    T ParseA

99
A model to predict the percentages
  • Stage 4b (second stage)
  • non-3sg agreement and non-present tense each
    about 15 (about 40 agreeing, 50 tensed)
  • about 20 NRFs F2 FParseT ParseA

100
A model to predict the percentages
  • Stage 4c (third stage)
  • everything appears to have tense and agreement
    (adult-like levels) F2 FParseT ParseA

101
Predicted vs. observedtense
102
Predicted vs. observedagrt
103
Predicted vs. observedNRFs
104
?
  • ? ?
  • ?
  • ? ?
  • ? ?
  • ?
  • ?
Write a Comment
User Comments (0)
About PowerShow.com